• Aucun résultat trouvé

Ce travail met en relation la récente et critiquée approche évolutive des connaissances avec l’ancien cadre de l’étude du raisonnement logique qui, bien qu’en pleine révolution de paradigme, reste une référence solide. Le premier objectif est de comprendre pourquoi nous raisonnons plus facilement et plus volontiers à propos d’un repas qu’à propos d’une règle de grammaire. Cette démarche peut paraître triviale, mais, à notre connaissance, aucune recherche n’a encore utilisé des habillages de connaissances primaires vs. secondaires dans le cadre de problèmes logiques. L’enjeu scientifique initial de confirmer les caractéristiques des deux types de connaissances, notamment en termes de motivation mais aussi de performance, de confiance et de charge cognitive perçue, a conduit à interroger ces résultats de manière plus pratique.

162 L’association successive des connaissances primaires et secondaires semble même dépasser nos attentes : en s’appuyant sur les premières, on pourrait bien favoriser l’engagement et la compréhension des individus face aux secondes.

A l’instar de Lewis Carroll, ce travail maquille les problèmes logiques qui rebutent tant les individus pour les rendre plus plaisants et motivants. En effet, il n’y a rien de plus efficace que l’intérêt et l’engagement pour permettre aux personnes d’évoluer dans un domaine. Les connaissances primaires jouent le rôle des jeux de mots de Carroll mais de manière beaucoup plus générale et peut-être plus efficace : tout le monde semble adhérer à l’influence de ces connaissances à l’inverse de l’humour nonsensique qui ne fait pas toujours consensus. Finalement, le domaine de l’apprentissage bénéficierait à laisser sa chance à l’approche évolutive. « Faites un seul essai, c’est tout ce que je vous demande ! » (Carroll, préface, Logique sans peine, 1966, initialement publié en 1896).

163

Références

Anzures, G., Kelly, D. J., Pascalis, O., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., De Viviés, X., & Lee, K. (2014). Own-and other-race face identity recognition in children: The effects of pose and feature composition. Developmental psychology, 50(2), 469-481. doi: 10.1037/a0033166 Ash, J., & Gallup, G. G. (2007). Paleoclimatic variation and brain expansion during human

evolution. Human Nature, 18(2), 109-124. doi: 10.1007/s12110-007-9015-z

Atkinson, R. K., Mayer, R. E., & Merrill, M. M. (2005). Fostering social agency in multimedia learning: Examining the impact of an animated agent’s voice. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30(1), 117-139. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.07.001

Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. Psychology of learning and motivation, 2, 89-195. doi: 10.1016/S0079- 7421(08)60422-3

Atran, S. (1998). Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and brain sciences, 21(04), 547-569.

Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., & Nurmi, J. E. (2006). Developmental dynamics between mathematical performance, task motivation, and teachers' goals during the transition to primary

school. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(1), 21-40. doi:

10.1348/000709905X51608

Ayres, P. (2006). Using subjective measures to detect variations of intrinsic cognitive load within problems. Learning and Instruction, 16(5), 389-400. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.09.001 Ayres, P., Marcus, N., Chan, C., & Qian, N. (2009). Learning hand manipulative tasks: When

instructional animations are superior to equivalent static representations. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(2), 348-353. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.013

Azevedo, R. (2015). Defining and measuring engagement and learning in science: Conceptual, theoretical, methodological, and analytical issues. Educational Psychologist, 50(1), 84-94. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2015.1004069

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychology of learning and motivation, 8, 47-89. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1

Bago, B., & De Neys, W. (2017). Fast logic?: Examining the time course assumption of dual process theory. Cognition, 158, 90-109. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.014

164 Bailenson, J. N., Shum, M. S., Atran, S., Medin, D. L., & Coley, J. D. (2002). A bird's eye view: Biological categorization and reasoning within and across cultures. Cognition, 84(1), 1-53. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00011-2

Bailey, D. H., & Geary, D. C. (2009). Hominid brain evolution. Human Nature, 20(1), 67-79. doi: 10.1007/s12110-008-9054-0

Baki, A., Kosa, T., & Guven, B. (2011). A comparative study of the effects of using dynamic geometry software and physical manipulatives on the spatial visualisation skills of pre‐service mathematics teachers. British Journal of Educational Technology, 42(2), 291-310. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8535.2009.01012.x

Banks, A.P., & Hope, C. (2014). Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning: An event-related potential study of belief bias. Psychophysiology, 51, 290-297. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12169 Bar-Haim, Y., Ziv, T., Lamy, D., & Hodes, R. M. (2006). Nature and nurture in own-race face

processing. Psychological science, 17(2), 159-163. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01679.x Barrett, T. J., Stull, A. T., Hsu, T. M., & Hegarty, M. (2015). Constrained interactivity for

relating multiple representations in science: When virtual is better than real. Computers & Education, 81, 69-81. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2014.09.009

Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Topics in cognitive science, 2(4), 716-724. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x

Beller, S. (2010). Deontic reasoning reviewed: psychological questions, empirical findings, and current theories. Cognitive Processing, 11(2), 123-132. doi: 10.1007/s10339-009-0265-z Berlin, B., Breedlove, D. E., & Raven, P. H. (1973). General principles of classification and

nomenclature in folk biology. American anthropologist, 75(1), 214-242. doi: 10.1525/aa.1973.75.1.02a00140

Bethell-Fox, C. E., & Shepard, R. N. (1988). Mental rotation: Effects of stimulus complexity and familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(1), 12-23. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.14.1.12

Binkley, M., Erstad, O., Herman, J., Raizen, S., Ripley, M., Miller-Ricci, M., & Rumble, M. (2012). Defining twenty-first century skills. In Griffin, P., McGaw, B., & Care, E. (Eds.), Assessment and teaching of 21st century skills (pp. 17-66). Dordrecht: Springer.

165 Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe and A. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185– 205). Cambridge: MIT Press.

Bjork, R. A. (2013). Desirable difficulties perspective on learning. In Pashler, H. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the mind, (pp. 134-146). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publication.

Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2011). Making things hard on yourself, but in a good way: Creating desirable difficulties to enhance learning. In FABBS Foundation, Gernsbache, M. A., & Pomerantz, J. R. (Eds.) Psychology and the real world: Essays illustrating fundamental contributions to society (pp.56-64). New York: Worth Publishers.

Bjork, R. A., & Kroll, J. F. (2015). Desirable difficulties in vocabulary learning. The American journal of psychology, 128(2), 241-252.

Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child development, 78(1), 246-263. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.x Blanchette, I., Gavigan, S., & Johnston, K. (2014). Does emotion help or hinder reasoning? The

moderating role of relevance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 1049- 1064. doi: 10.1037/a0034996

Bonnefon, J.-F. (2011). Le raisonneur et ses modèles. Grenoble : PUG.

Bonnefon, J. F., Feeney, A., & De Neys, W. (2011). The risk of polite misunderstandings. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(5), 321-324. doi: 10.1177/0963721411418472

Bonnefon, J. F., Girotto, V., & Legrenzi, P. (2012). The psychology of reasoning about preferences and unconsequential decisions. Synthese, 185, 27-41. doi: 10.1007/s11229-011- 9957-x

Bonnefon, J. F., & Villejoubert, G. (2007). Modus Tollens, Modus Shmollens: Contrapositive reasoning and the pragmatics of negation. Thinking & reasoning, 13(2), 207-222. doi: 10.1080/13546780601069488

Braver, T. S., Krug, M. K., Chiew, K. S., Kool, W., Westbrook, J. A., Clement, N. J., Adcock, R. A., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Carver, C. S., Cools, R., Custers, R., Dickinson, A., Sweck, C. S., Fishbach, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Hess, T. M., Isaacowitz, D. M., Mather, M., Murayama, K., Pessoa, L., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., & Somerville, L. H. (2014). Mechanisms

166 of motivation–cognition interaction: challenges and opportunities. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(2), 443-472. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0300-0

Brinch, C. N., & Galloway, T. A. (2012). Schooling in adolescence raises IQ scores. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(2), 425-430. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1106077109

Brockmole, J. R., Davoli, C. C., Abrams, R. A., & Witt, J. K. (2013). The world within reach: Effects of hand posture and tool use on visual cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 38-44. doi: 10.1177/0963721412465065

Brown, G. R., & Richerson, P. J. (2014). Applying evolutionary theory to human behaviour: Past differences and current debates. Journal of Bioeconomics, 16(2), 105-128. doi: 10.1007/s10818-013-9166-4

Brunken, R., Plass, J. L., & Leutner, D. (2003). Direct measurement of cognitive load in

multimedia learning. Educational psychologist, 38(1), 53-61. doi:

10.1207/S15326985EP3801_7

Bucciarelli, M., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1999). Strategies in syllogistic reasoning. Cognitive Science, 23(3), 247-303. doi: 10.1016/S0364-0213(99)00008-7

Bucciarelli, M., Mackiewicz, R., Khemlani, S. S., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2016). Children's creation of algorithms: simulations and gestures. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(3), 297-318. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2015.1134541

Bugental, D. B. (2000). Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: A domain-based approach. Psychological bulletin, 126(2), 187-219. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.187

Bushnell, I. W. R. (2001). Mother's face recognition in newborn infants: Learning and memory. Infant and Child Development, 10(1‐2), 67-74. doi: 10.1002/icd.248

Buss, D. M. (Ed.). (2005). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. USA: John Wiley & Sons. Butterfield, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2001). Errors committed with high confidence are

hypercorrected. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(6), 1491-1494. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1491

Byrne, R. M. (2016). Counterfactual thought. Annual review of psychology, 67, 135-157. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033249

Byrne, P., Becker, S., & Burgess, N. (2007). Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychological review, 114(2), 340-375. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.340

167 Carroll, L. (1966). Logique sans peine. Paris: Hermann.

Carroll, L. (1974). Une histoire embrouillée. Paris: Bélibaste.

Carroll, L. (1887). The Game of Logic. Consultation en ligne le 30/07/2017 [http://pdfbooks.co.za/library/LEWIS_CARROLL-THE_GAME_OF_LOGIC.pdf].

Castro-Alonso, J. C., Ayres, P., & Paas, F. (2015). Animations showing Lego manipulative tasks: Three potential moderators of effectiveness. Computers & Education, 85, 1-13. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2014.12.022

Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: their structure, growth, and action. Oxford: Houghton Mifflin. Chanquoy, L., Tricot, A., & Sweller, J. (2007). La charge cognitive. Théorie et applications.

Paris : Armand Colin.

Chen, Z. (2007). Learning to map: strategy discovery and strategy change in young children. Developmental psychology, 43(2), 386-403. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.386 Chen, O., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2016). Relations between the worked example and

generation effects on immediate and delayed tests. Learning and Instruction, 45, 20-30. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.06.007

Chevalier, A., & Chevalier, N. (2009). Influence of proficiency level and constraints on viewpoint switching: A study in web design. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(1), 126-137. doi: 10.1002/acp.1448

Chi, M. T., & Wylie, R. (2014). The ICAP framework: Linking cognitive engagement to active

learning outcomes. Educational Psychologist, 49(4), 219-243. doi:

10.1080/00461520.2014.965823

Chinn, C., & Duncan, R. G. (2017, August-September). What is the Value of General Knowledge of Scientific Reasoning? 17th Biennial conference of the European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction (EARLI), Tampere, Finland.

Choi, H. H., Van Merriënboer, J. J., & Paas, F. (2014). Effects of the physical environment on cognitive load and learning: towards a new model of cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 26(2), 225-244. doi: 10.1007/s10648-014-9262-6

Chu, F. W., vanMarle, K., & Geary, D. C. (2015). Early numerical foundations of young children’s mathematical development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 205- 212. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.01.006

168 Clement, J. (1982). Students’ preconceptions in introductory mechanics. American Journal of

physics, 50(1), 66-71. doi: 10.1119/1.12989

Cohen, L. J. (1981). Can human irrationality be experimentally demonstrated?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4(03), 317-331. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00009092

Cohen, M. R., & Nagel, E. (1934). An introduction to logic and scientific method. Dématérialisé: Hughes Press.

Cosmides, L., Barrett, H. C., & Tooby, J. (2010). Adaptive specializations, social exchange, and the evolution of human intelligence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 9007-9014. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914623107

Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In Barkow; J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds), The adapted mind, (pp.163-228). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2004). Knowing thyself: The evolutionary psychology of moral reasoning and moral sentiments. The Ruffin Series of the Society for Business Ethics, 4, 93- 128. doi: 10.5840/ruffinx200447

Cosnefroy, O., Nurra, C., & Dessus, P. (2016). Analyse dynamique de la motivation des élèves en début de scolarité obligatoire en fonction de la nature de leurs interactions avec l’enseignant. Éducation et formations, 90, 29-51. doi: hal-01306393

Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration of mental

storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87-114. doi:

10.1017/S0140525X01003922

Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and

why? Current directions in psychological science, 19(1), 51-57. doi:

10.1177/0963721409359277

Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F. D. C., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage, 31(3), 1257-1267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.033

Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1149-1157. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0319

169 Cummins, D. D. (1996). Evidence of deontic reasoning in 3-and 4-year-old children. Memory &

Cognition, 24(6), 823-829. doi: 10.3758/BF03201105

Danchin, E., Giraldeau, L. A., Valone, T. J., & Wagner, R. H. (2004). Public information: from

nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science, 305(5683), 487-491. doi:

10.1126/science.1098254

Darnon, B., & Butera, F. (2005). Buts d'accomplissement, stratégies d'étude, et motivation intrinsèque: présentation d'un domaine de recherche et validation française de l'échelle d'Elliot et McGregor (2001). L'année psychologique, 105(1), 105-131.

Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. New York: D. Appleton and Company. Consultation

en ligne le 16/05/2017 [http://darwin-

online.org.uk/converted/pdf/1861_OriginNY_F382.pdf].

Davis, H., & Pérusse, R. (1988). Numerical competence in animals: Definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(04), 561-579. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00053437

De Bock, D., Deprez, J., Van Dooren, W., Roelens, M., & Verschaffel, L. (2011). Abstract or concrete examples in learning mathematics? A replication and elaboration of Kaminski, Sloutsky, and Heckler's study. Journal for research in Mathematics Education, 42(2), 109- 126.

Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards of intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18(1), 105-115.

De Haan, M., Pascalis, O., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Specialization of neural mechanisms underlying face recognition in human infants. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 14(2), 199- 209. doi: 10.1162/089892902317236849

de Jong T. (2006). Computer simulations: Technological advances in inquiry learning. Science, 312, 532–533. doi: 10.1126/science.1127750

de Jong, T., Linn, M. C., & Zacharia, Z. C. (2013). Physical and virtual laboratories in science and engineering education. Science, 340(6130), 305-308. doi: 10.1126/science.1230579 De Neys, W. (2006). Dual processing in reasoning: Two systems but one reasoner. Psychological

170 De Neys, W. (2012). Bias and conflict: A case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on

Psychological Science, 7(1), 28-38. doi: 10.1177/1745691611429354

De Neys, W. (2014). Conflict detection, dual processes, and logical intuitions: Some clarifications. Thinking & Reasoning, 20(2), 169-187. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2013.854725 De Neys, W., & Bonnefon, J. F. (2013). The ‘whys’ and ‘whens’ of individual differences in

thinking biases. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(4), 172-178. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.02.001 De Neys, W., Cromheeke, S., & Osman, M. (2011). Biased but in doubt: Conflict and decision

confidence. PloS one, 6(1). doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015954

De Neys, W., & Everaerts, D. (2008). Developmental trends in everyday conditional reasoning: The retrieval and inhibition interplay. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 100(4), 252-263. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.03.003

De Neys, W., & Feremans, V. (2013). Development of heuristic bias detection in elementary school. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 258. doi: 10.1037/a0028320

De Neys, W., & Franssens, S. (2009). Belief inhibition during thinking: Not always winning but at least taking part. Cognition, 113(1), 45-61. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.009

De Neys, W., & Glumicic, T. (2008). Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking. Cognition, 106(3), 1248-1299. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.002

De Neys, W., Moyens, E., & Vansteenwegen, D. (2010). Feeling we’re biased: Autonomic arousal and reasoning conflict. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(2), 208- 216. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.2.208

De Neys, W., Schaeken, W., & d'Ydewalle, G. (2003). Causal conditional reasoning and strength of association: The disabling condition case. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 15(2), 161-176. doi: 10.1080/09541440244000058

De Neys, W., Schaeken, W., & d'Ydewalle, G. (2005). Working memory and everyday conditional reasoning: Retrieval and inhibition of stored counterexamples. Thinking & Reasoning, 11(4), 349-381. do : 10.1080/13546780442000222

De Neys, W., & Vanderputte, K. (2011). When less is not always more: stereotype knowledge

and reasoning development. Developmental psychology, 47(2), 432-441. doi:

171 De Neys, W., & Van Gelder, E. (2009). Logic and belief across the lifespan: the rise and fall of belief inhibition during syllogistic reasoning. Developmental science, 12(1), 123-130. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00746.x

De Neys, W., Vartanian, O., & Goel, V. (2008). Smarter than we think: When our brains detect that we are biased. Psychological Science, 19(5), 483-489. doi: 10.1111/j.1467- 9280.2008.02113.x

Dessus, P., Tanguy, F., & Tricot, A. (2015). Natural cognitive foundations of teacher knowledge: An evolutionary and cognitive load account. In Grangeat, M. (Ed.), Understanding science teacher professional knowledge growth, (pp. 177–192). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

Dickstein, L. S. (1978). The effect of figure on syllogistic reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 6(1), 76-83. doi: 10.3758/BF03197431

Dominowski, R. L. (1995). Content effects in Wason’s selection task. In Newstead, S. E., & Evans, J. S. B. T. (Eds), Perspectives on thinking and reasoning: Essays in honour of Peter Wason, (pp.41-65). Hove/Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological science, 16(12), 939-944.doi: 10.1111/j.1467- 9280.2005.01641.x

Dunbar, K., Fugelsang, J., & Stein, C. (2007). Do naïve theories ever go away? In Lovett, M., & Shah, P. (Eds.), Thinking with Data: 33rd Carnegie Symposium on Cognition (pp. 193–205). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

Eliades, M., Mansell, W., Stewart, A. J., & Blanchette, I. (2012). An investigation of belief-bias and logicality in reasoning with emotional contents. Thinking & Reasoning, 18(4), 461-479. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2012.713317

Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2× 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of personality and social psychology, 80(3), 501-519. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.501

Elliot, A. J., & Murayama, K. (2008). On the measurement of achievement goals: Critique, illustration, and application. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 613-628. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.613

172 Ellis, G. F. (2008). Commentary on “An evolutionarily informed education science” by David C.

Geary. Educational Psychologist, 43(4), 206-213. doi: 10.1080/00461520802392216

Engelmann, K., Neuhaus, B.J., & Fischer, F. (2016). Fostering scientific reasoning in education - meta-analytic evidence from intervention studies. Educational Research and Evaluation, 55 (5-6), 333-349. doi: 10.1080/13803611.2016.1240089

Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American psychologist, 49(8), 725-747. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.49.8.725

Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological review, 102(2), 211-245. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.211

Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological review, 100(3), 363-406. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.363

Evans, J. S. B. T. (1989). Bias in Human Reasoning : Causes and Consequences. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Evans, J. S. B. (2002). Logic and human reasoning: an assessment of the deduction paradigm. Psychological bulletin, 128(6), 978-996. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.978

Evans, J. S. B. (2003). In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7(10), 454-459. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012

Evans, J. S. B. T. (2005). Deductive reasoning. In Holyoak, K. J., & Morrison, R. G. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning (pp.169-184). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Evans, J. S. B. (2007). Hypothetical thinking: dual processes in reasoning and judgement. Hove: Psychology Press.

Evans, J. S. B. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 59, 255-278. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629 Evans, J. S. B. (2011). Dual-process theories of reasoning: Contemporary issues and

developmental applications. Developmental Review, 31(2), 86-102. doi:

10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.007

Evans, J. S. B. (2016). Reasoning, biases and dual processes: The lasting impact of Wason (1960). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(10), 2076-2092. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.914547

173 Evans, J. S. B., Barston, J. L., & Pollard, P. (1983). On the conflict between logic and belief in

syllogistic reasoning. Memory & cognition, 11(3), 295-306. doi: 10.3758/BF03196976

Evans, J. S. B., & Curtis-Holmes, J. (2005). Rapid responding increases belief bias: Evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 11(4), 382-389. doi: 10.1080/13546780542000005

Evans, J. S. B., Handley, S. J., & Bacon, A. M. (2009). Reasoning under time pressure: A study of causal conditional inference. Experimental Psychology, 56(2), 77-83. doi: 10.1027/1618- 3169.56.2.77

Evans, J. S. B., Handley, S. J., Neilens, H., & Over, D. E. (2007). Thinking about conditionals: A study of individual differences. Memory & Cognition, 35(7), 1772-1784. doi: 10.3758/BF03193509

Evans, J. S. B., Handley, S. J., Neilens, H., & Over, D. (2010). The influence of cognitive ability and instructional set on causal conditional inference. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(5), 892-909. doi: 10.1080/17470210903111821

Evans, J. S. B., Newstead, S. E., & Byrne, R. M. (1993). Human reasoning: The psychology of deduction. Psychology Press.

Evans, J. S. B., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing

the debate. Perspectives on psychological science, 8(3), 223-241. doi:

10.1177/1745691612460685

Fantz, R. L. (1961). The origin of form perception. Scientific American, 204, 66-72. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0561-66

Faucher, L. (2007). Les émotions morales à la lumière de la psychologie évolutionniste: le dégoût et l'évitement de l'inceste. Les cahiers du LANCI, n°2007-1. Consultation en ligne le 06/10/2012 [http://www.er.uqam.ca/nobel/philuqam/dev/documents/cahiers/2007-01.pdf]. Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in cognitive

sciences, 8(7), 307-314. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002

Fiddick, L., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). No interpretation without representation: The role of domain-specific representations and inferences in the Wason selection task. Cognition, 77(1), 1-79. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00085-8

174 Fischer, F., Hetmanek, A., Engelmann, K., & Opitz, A. (2017, August-September). Scientific Reasoning and Argumentation as Cross-Domain Competence. 17th Biennial conference of the

European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction (EARLI), Tampere, Finland. Fisher, S. E., & Ridley, M. (2013). Culture, genes, and the human revolution. Science, 340(6135),

929-930. doi: 10.1126/science.1236171

Fiske, S. T. (2002). What we know now about bias and intergroup conflict, the problem of the century. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(4), 123-128. doi: 10.1111/1467- 8721.00183

Flinn, M. V., Geary, D. C., & Ward, C. V. (2005). Ecological dominance, social competition, and coalitionary arms races: Why humans evolved extraordinary intelligence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(1), 10-46. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.005

Fodor, J. (2003). L’esprit, ça ne marche pas comme ça. Paris : Odile Jacob.

Fogarty, L., Strimling, P., & Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of teaching. Evolution, 65(10), 2760-2770. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2011.01370.x

Franssens, S., & De Neys, W. (2009). The effortless nature of conflict detection during thinking. Thinking & Reasoning, 15(2), 105-128. doi: 10.1080/13546780802711185

Fyfe, E. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2017) Mathematics practice without feedback: A desirable difficulty in a classroom setting. Instructional Science, 1-18. doi: 10.1007/s11251-016-9401-1 Garland, T. B., & Sanchez, C. A. (2013). Rotational perspective and learning procedural tasks

from dynamic media. Computers & Education, 69, 31-37. doi:

10.1016/j.compedu.2013.06.014

Geary, D. C. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children's cognition: Implications for mathematical development and instruction. American Psychologist, 50(1), 24-37. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.50.1.24

Geary, D. C. (2005). Evolution and cognitive development. In Burges, R. L., & MacDonald, K. (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on human development (pp.99-133). SAGE Publications. doi: 10.4135/9781452233574.n4

Geary, D. C. (2007). Educating the evolved mind: Conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology. In Carlson, J. S., & Levin, J. R. (Eds.), Educating the evolved mind: Conceptual foundations for an evolutionary educational psychology (pp. 1–99). Greenwich: Information Age.

175 Geary, D. C. (2008). An evolutionarily informed education science. Educational

Psychologist, 43(4), 179-195. doi: 10.1080/00461520802392133

Geary, D. C. (2013). Early foundations for mathematics learning and their relations to learning disabilities. Current directions in psychological science, 22(1), 23-27. doi: 10.1177/0963721412469398

Geary, D. C., & Berch, D. (2015). Evolutionary approaches to understanding children’s academic achievement. In Scott, R. A., & Kosslyn, S. M. (Eds.), Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences (pp.1-10). Hoboken: Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0123 Geary, D. C., & Berch, D. (2016). Evolution and Children’s Cognitive and Academic

Development. In Geary, D. C., & Berch, D. (Eds.), Evolution and Children’s Cognitive and Academic Development (pp.217-249). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-29986-0_9

Geary, D. C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child development, 71(1), 57-65. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00118

Gelman, R. (1990). First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data: Number and the animate‐inanimate distinction as examples. Cognitive science, 14(1), 79-106. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_5

Gigerenzer, G., & Brighton, H. (2009). Homo heuristicus: Why biased minds make better inferences. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(1), 107-143. doi: 10.1111/j.1756- 8765.2008.01006.x

Gigerenzer, G., & Hug, K. (1992). Domain-specific reasoning: Social contracts, cheating, and perspective change. Cognition, 43(2), 127-171. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90060-U

Ginns, P. (2005). Meta-analysis of the modality effect. Learning and Instruction, 15(4), 313-331. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2005.07.001

Ginns, P. (2006). Integrating information: A meta-analysis of the spatial contiguity and temporal

contiguity effects. Learning and Instruction, 16(6), 511-525. doi:

10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.10.001

Glenberg, A. M., Goldberg, A. B., & Zhu, X. (2011). Improving early reading comprehension using embodied CAI. Instructional Science, 39(1), 27-39. doi: 10.1007/s11251-009-9096-7

176 Goldstone, R. L., & Son, J. Y. (2005). The transfer of scientific principles using concrete and idealized simulations. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14(1), 69-110. doi: 10.1207/s15327809jls1401_4

Gray, P. (2016). Children’s Natural Ways of Educating Themselves Still Work: Even for the Three Rs. In Geary, D. C., & Berch, D. (Eds.), Evolution and Children’s Cognitive and Academic Development (pp.291-306). Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-29986-0_9

Greene, B. A. (2015). Measuring cognitive engagement with self-report scales: Reflections from

over 20 years of research. Educational Psychologist, 50(1), 14-30. doi:

10.1080/00461520.2014.989230

Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Griggs, R. A., & Cox, J. R. (1982). The elusive thematic‐materials effect in Wason's selection

task. British Journal of Psychology, 73(3), 407-420. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-