• Aucun résultat trouvé

Overview of the experiments

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Partager "Overview of the experiments"

Copied!
14
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

A distributional test of exemplar race models

Denis Cousineau, Serge Larochelle University of Montreal

COUSINED@PSY.UMONTREAL.CA

(2)

abstract

A prediction of the exemplar model proposed by Logan (1992) is that the shape parameter of the RT distribution should stay constant from session to session. One experiment involving a hybrid visual and memory search task shows that this is true for most of the subjects, including subjects in the varied mapping condition, who cannot associate responses to individual exemplars. Results of this experiment are therefore in contradiction with a race model based on exemplars In a second experiment, subjects served in a categorical varied mapping condition in which the sets of stimuli serving as targets and distractors switched roles from trial to trial. The shape parameter was not constant for any of these subjects, although other measures (mean, standard deviation) were similar to results of the previous experiment.. Speed-accuracy trade-off is one factor that alters the fit of the distribution. It is suggested that tasks in which various levels of processing compete might prevent the shape parameter from being constant throughout practice.

(3)

Introduction

A Weibull distribution of RT can result from a race where the fastest runner triggers the response.

Assumptions needed are:

1) Every runner is a random variable

2) Runners are independant and identically distributed

Such a race model was used by Logan (1988, 1992) to implement his exemplar theory of automatization. This theory assumes:

3) Obligatory encoding of every individual stimulus;

4) obligatory retrieval of all stored exemplars.

Using these assumptions, it is easy to show (see equation 1) that the distribution shape parameter c is not altered by practice.

With a transformation using quantiles, the probability density fonction (pdf) can be represented by lines of slope c.

According to exemplar race models, the

"distribution lines" should be parallel from session to session. This parallel line hypothesis was tested in two experiments involving a hybrid memory and visual search task.

(4)

Overview of the experiments

Stimuli were taken from four sets: {1, 4, 5, 8}, {2, 3, 6, 7}, {L, H, R, S} and {Z, B, G, F}. The Memory display and Test display were composed of 1, 2 or 4 characters. One session was composed of 864 trials. The learning phase lasted six sessions

LHRS

2 L 4

*

(500 ms)

Memory display Test display

(5)

Experiment 1

16 subjects participated in one of the following two conditions. In the Consistent Mapping (CM) condition, the targets were chosen from one of the previous sets, while the distractors came from another as shown in Table 1. For half of the subjects, targets and distractors were all digits of all letters (homogeneous condition). For the other half (heterogeneous condition), there was a categorical distinction between targets and distractors. In the Varied Mapping (VM) condition, two sets were merged, targets and distractors being picked at random from this larger, homogeneous or heterogeneous, set.

Table 1: Targets and distractors assignement in CM

HOMOGENEOUS HETEROGENEOUS

Ss Targets Distractor Ss Targets Distractor S1 2, 3, 6, 7 1, 4, 5, 8 S5 2, 3, 6, 7 L, H, R, S S2 1, 4, 5, 8 2, 3, 6, 7 S6 L, H, R, S 2, 3, 6, 7 S3 L, H, R, S Z, B, G, F S7 1, 4, 5, 8 Z, B, G, F S4 Z, B, G, F L, H, R, S S8 Z, B, G, F 1, 4, 5, 8

(6)

Results (1): Fit of the Weibull distribution

A χ2 test of goodness of fit shows no significant difference between the empirical RT distribution and the Weibull distribution for 11 of the 16 subjects. Fits were good for 6 of the 8 Ss. in CM, and for 5 of the 8 Ss. in VM.

Figure 1 presents RT distributions for two typical subjects

Two of the five deviant subjects show highly significant Speed-Accuracy Trade-Off (SATO).

A likelihood ratio test (LRT) was used to compare the quality of fit provided by the Weibull distribution against the fit of the Ex-Gaussian and the Log-Normal distributions. The Weibull distribution provided the best fit for every session of every subject without exception.

Results (2): Parallel line hypothesis

A likelihood ratio test (LRT) was used to compare the fit when the c parameter was free to vary against a model where the c parameter was constrained to be equal throughout the 6 sessions.

Figure 2 presents the c parameters for two typical subjects

For 5 subjects, the c parameters changed significantly from session to session. These are the same five subjects whose data did not fit a Weibull distribution.

(7)

8 subjects participated in this experiment. The stimuli were taken from the same sets as before. However, their role as targets or distractors was decided at random for every trial, as shown in Table 2. This condition is termed Categorical Varied Mapping (CVM) because the stimulus- response mapping varied over trials. However, since the stimuli of any given set served only as targets or distractors on any given trial, subjects could develop a categorical knowledge of the sets. This experiment was identical to Experiment 1 in all other aspects.

Experiment 2

Table 2: Targets and distractors assignement in Exp. 2

HOMOGENEOUS HETEROGENEOUS

Ss Targets Distractors Ss Targets Distractors S1& 1, 4, 5, 8 2, 3, 6, 7 S5& 1, 4, 5, 8 Z, B, G, F S2 2, 3, 6, 7 1, 4, 5, 8 S6 Z, B, G, F 1, 4, 5, 8 S3& L, H, R, S Z, B, G, F S7& 1, 4, 5, 8 Z, B, G, F S4 Z, B, G, F L, H, R, S S8 Z, B, G, F 1, 4, 5, 8

The stimuli are presented on two lines, preceded by a {.

Two subjects had the same stimuli.

{ {

{ {

(8)

Results (1): Fit of the Weibull distribution

RT distributions did not differ from a Weibull distribution for 6 of the 8 subjects (exceptions are Ss 3 and 5). Figure 3 presents two typical subjects.

Subject 5 exhibited significant SATO. We cannot explain at present why subject 3’s RTs did not fit a Weibull distribution.

Results (2): Parallel line hypothesis

No single subject had a constant shape parameter throughout learning (all χ2 > 16.2, p<.006). See figure 4.

This is a very surprising result, because on all other aspects (mean RT, slope as a fonction of set sizes, power learning curves), the performance observed in the CVM-HETERO condition ressembled that obtained in the CM condition of Exp 1, while the performance observed in CVM-HOMO was almost identical to that obtained in VM of Exp 1.

However, the distribution analyses revealed that CVM subjects behaved differently from all other groups.

(9)

Conclusion

Exemplar race models

Exemplar theories cannot account for these results since the response associated to each exemplar varies from trial to trial in both VM and in CVM conditions. Therefore, access to prior exemplar in uninformative. If a race model is to account for the VM results, the runners have to be at a lower level than the exemplars.

Weibull distribution

a) The basic assumption of the Weibull distribution is the independance of the processes participating in the decision.

Between-trial dependancies (such as Speed-Accuracy Trade- Off) violate this assumption. If the SATO is large, it disrupts the RT distribution.

b) The Weibull adequatly explains the CM results in terms of a race among exemplars. However, other interpretations are possible. In CM, exemplars are mapped onto sets (targets vs. distractors) which are also consistently associated to responses. In VM condition, neither exemplars nor sets are consistently mapped to responses. The only race possible would be among the exemplar features.

One possible though post hoc explanation supposes that the race can occur at different levels. In CM and VM conditions, the same level of response would always be used to make the decision. In CVM, however, the same level would not systematically win the race. This instability might alter the RT distributions (see figure 5).

(10)

Equation 1

If the RT distribution for one exemplar is given by the Weibull distribution (assumptions 1 and 2) then, as the number of exemplars n increases (assumptions 3 and 4),

The resulting distribution is also a Weibull distribution, with the scale parameter b reduced.

However, c is not altered by practice.

The following illustrates the linearization of the PDF:

( )

F RT F RT

e

e

e e

n

n

RT a b

n

n RT a b

n

RT a b

RT a bn

c

c

c

c c

c

( )= − − ( )

= −

= −

= − = −













1 1

1

1

1 1

1

1 1

Probability density function

0 0.001 0.002 0.003 0.004 0.005

0 100 200 300 400 500

RT

f ( RT )

c = 1.1 c = 1.5

c = 2.0

a = 0; b = 200

Linearized cumulative prob.

function

-30 -25 -20 -15 -10 -5 0 5

-10 -5 0 5 10

ln ( RT - a )

ln( ln( 1/ 1 - F( RT ) ) )

c = 1.1

c = 2.0 c = 1.5

(11)

3.5 4 4.5 5 5.5 6 6.5

-2.35 -1.85 -1.35 -0.85 -0.35 0.15 0.65

ln(ln(1/1-q))

ln(RT-a)

Subject 2

3.2 3.7 4.2 4.7 5.2 5.7

-2.35 -1.85 -1.35 -0.85 -0.35 0.15 0.65

ln(ln(1/1-q))

ln(RT-a)

Subject 5 Fo this subject, lines

do not deviate from parallelism 2=1.1, p >.50).

For this subject, lines deviate from parallelism

2= 199, p <.001)

Figures 1 and 2

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70

200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 1600 reaction times (ms)

% observed

0 0,001 0,002 0,003 0,004 0,005 0,006 0,007 0,008 0,009

% predicted

Subject 8 session 6

0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140

200 700 1200

reaction times (ms)

% observed

0 0,001 0,002 0,003 0,004

% predicted

Sujet 9 session 6

Significant fit ( χ2 = 53.6 using all six sessions)

No fit

2 = 173.2, p < .05)

(12)

Figures 3 and 4

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90

200 700 1200

reaction times (ms)

% observed

0 0,0005 0,001 0,0015 0,002 0,0025 0,003 0,0035

% predicted

Sujet 3 session 6

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90

200 700 1200

reaction times (ms)

% observed

0 0,001 0,002 0,003 0,004 0,005

% predicted

Sujet 5 session 6

3,5 4 4,5 5 5,5 6 6,5

-7 -6,5 -6 -5,5 -5 -4,5

ln(ln(1/1-q))

ln(RT-a)

session 1 session 3 session 4 session 5 session 6

Subject 5

3,5 4 4,5 5 5,5 6 6,5

-7 -6,5 -6 -5,5 -5 -4,5

ln(ln(1/1-q))

ln(RT-a)

session 1 session 3 session 4 session 5 session 6

Subject 3

These subjects are the two exceptions: They don’t match a Weibull distribution 2 > 224,3 for 6 sessions) nor are their lines parallel 2 > 400)

(13)

Figure 5: a model of RT distribution

overt RT distribution

Decision process(es)

Soldiers (exemplars, feature extractors, or neuron’s firing) Variable

decision criterion

IF decision criterion changes within sessions THEN

⇒ RT distribution is disrupted

IF decision criterion changes between sessions THEN

⇒ RT distribution is well fitted by a Weibull distribution

⇒ c is not constant from session to session

IF decision criterion is stable across experiment THEN

⇒ RT distribution is well fitted by a Weibull distribution

⇒ c is constant from session to session

(14)

C, the shape parameter of the RT distribution, is represented by a line of slope c using adequate transformation (Weibull, 1951).

According to exemplar

race models, distribution

lines should be parallel

because the number of

exemplars n is a linear

function of practice N.

Références

Documents relatifs

Eric Calais, Dominique Boisson, Steeve J. Symithe, Claude Prépetit, Bétonus Pierre, et al.. A Socio- Seismology Experiment in Haiti.. Alternative community-based models exist but

The run tests discussed in section 5 showed that the modification of the VAG model resulted in a better balance between growth and decay, so that the limitation of the wind–sea

Time evolution during P1L2 of the model new production (NP), mixed layer depth (MLD), surface nitrate concentration (NO 3 ), and solar radiation (QSR) at stations S1 (black), S2

We provide an original measure of the foam shear modulus and of the plastic threshold, and show that a simple model of elasto-plastic material allows to predict the scaling

Dans ce chapitre nous avons relaté les différents travaux dans le domaine du comportement au feu ainsi que les différentes approches au niveau des règlements .Plusieurs

In this paper, we offer a comprehensive investigation on the relative efficiency and effectiveness of various crackdown policies using a lab-in-the-field experiment with

stoebe from different eco- geographic regions (composition of “artificial populations”; see Hahn et al 2012) showing original populations with number of families and seeds

As discussed in Sec. 7!, azimuthal deformations are much more intense when the jet is swirling: rotation en- hances the concentration of braid vorticity into secondary