• Aucun résultat trouvé

Motion on a Necker cube leads to micro-pursuit-like eye movements and affects the dynamics of bistability

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Motion on a Necker cube leads to micro-pursuit-like eye movements and affects the dynamics of bistability"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-01726513

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01726513

Submitted on 5 Sep 2018

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

Motion on a Necker cube leads to micro-pursuit-like eye movements and affects the dynamics of bistability

Kevin Parisot, Alan Chauvin, Anne Guérin-Dugué, Ronald Phlypo, Steeve Zozor

To cite this version:

Kevin Parisot, Alan Chauvin, Anne Guérin-Dugué, Ronald Phlypo, Steeve Zozor. Motion on a Necker cube leads to micro-pursuit-like eye movements and affects the dynamics of bistability. ECM 2017 - 19th European Conference on Eye Movements, Aug 2017, Wuppertal, Germany. pp.869 - 878.

�hal-01726513�

(2)

Kevin Parisot, Alan Chauvin, Anne Guérin-Dugué, Ronald Phlypo, Steeve Zozor

kevin.parisot@gipsa-lab.fr, alan.chauvin@univ-grenoble-alpes.fr, anne.guerin@gipsa-lab.fr, ronald.phlypo@gipsa-lab.fr, steeve.zozor@gipsa-lab.fr

Blake, R., Sobel, K. V., & Gilroy, L. A. (2003). Visual motion retards alternations between conflicting perceptual interpretations.

Neuron, 39(5), 869-878.

Leopold, D. A., & Logothetis, N. K. (1999). Multistable phenomena: changing views in perception. Trends in cognitive sciences, 3(7), 254-264.

Kornmeier, J., & Bach, M. (2005). The Necker cube—an ambiguous figure disambiguated in early visual processing. Vision research, 45(8), 955-960.

Motion on a Necker cube leads to micro-pursuit-like eye movements and affects the dynamics of bistability

Theoretical models of bistable perception

References

Apparatus:

Display: 30 by 40 cm (20’’) VisionMaster Pro 513 at 57 cm from participants.

Resolution & refresh rate: 1024 by 768 pixels & 75 Hz.

Eye-tracker: EyeLink 1000 binocular recording at 1000 Hz.

Study the effects of adaptation and noise on perceptual reversal speed:

Conclusions Experimental protocol

Eye-tracking analysis

Behavioral analysis

Adaptation mitigation based on predictable motion of retinal image → excite new populations of neurons.

Warning: depends on participants' gaze stability on fixation cross.

Stimulus motion leads to an increase in reversal speed as predicted by models but effect size is small.

Predictability of motion does not modulate noise and adaptation as expected. Retinal image shifts partially compensated by micro-smooth pursuits but does not suffice to explain behavioral results.

Experimental observations: Micro-movements of the eyes in the form of approximate smooth pursuits within fixation events detected → not studied in the context of multi-stability to our knowledge

→ development of a multi-stable gravitational attractor-based model of fixational eye movements (Parisot et al., ECVP 2017).

Percept

Moreno-Bote, Knill & Pouget, 2011

Fixational eye movements (FEM) displacement, mean sequence inertia (see M1) w.r.t. fixation

event position (see Fig. 4a):

FX ≈ RW

(FX & RW) < LJ

→ LJ condition generates more FEM displacement than FX & RW.

1

2

4

3

Task: stare at fixation cross (screen center) during a sequence of stimulus presentation (Necker cube - Fig. 2a) & report perceptual changes with keypress.

Participants: n = 26 (16 females and 10 males, M(age) = 28.35, SD(age) = 10.93, range = 20-71).

16 participants were analyzed after outliers & incoherent data removal.

Conditions:

FX: no stimulus motion (Fixed).

RW: pseudo-random movements (Random Walk) LJ: smooth Lissajou trajectory

→ Global sequence motion was matched between RW & LJ → 36 deg/sec

Experimental design: 3 blocks of 15 sequences (pseudo-random shuffle) of 5-9 reversals.

Theory: models of perceptual bistability (Moreno-Bote et al., 2011 & Shpiro et al., 2009) are governed by two forces: adaptation & noise.

Experiment goal: manipulate perceptual dynamics using retinal image motion.

Method: Unpredictable motion → add noise & predictable motion → mitigate neural adaptation

Bistable perception: oscillation between different perceptual states under constant physical stimulus.

Models: Constant stimulation → neural adaptation → periodic suppression of alternative percept energy → adaptation drives the choice of percept.

Hypothesis: Predictable retinal image motion

↪ counteract adaptation effect

↪ observe lower reversal speed (cf. binocular rivalry results from Blake, Sobel & Gilroy, 2003).

0.51.01.52.0

RW LJ

Percept reversal speed analysis

FXratio reversal speed FX reversal

speed

Figure 3: Violin plot of FX-ratio percept reversal speed with means and 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 2b: (left) Stimulus centre of gravity motion in RW. (right) Stimulus centre of gravity motion in LJ.

LJ

Retinal image motion Adaptation ↘ Percept reversal speed ↘

RW

Noise Probability of percept reversal Percept reversal speed

nseq: number of reversals Tseq: sequence total time

Figure 2a: Necker cube stimulus (Kornmeier & Bach, 2005)

2,6°

2,4°

0.51.01.52.0

RW LJ

Percept reversal speed analysis

FXratio reversal speed

Stimulus sensitive Task focused

Results (post ocular analysis - Fig. 4e):

1. Effect of motion on reversal speed on stimulus sensitive group in LJ.

2. Effect of predictability of motion on reversal

speed: RW ≈ LJ → retinal image stabilization does not explain bistable perception dynamics results.

← Figure 4e: Reversal speed per condition with distinction of participant groups.

51020502005002000

FX RW LJ

Gaze in fixation displacement analysis

log(Inertia) [deg^2]

← Figure 4a: Statistics of inertia (log-scale) of gaze samples against fixation positions.

Figure 4b: Example of spatial trajectories over a fixation in ocular data.

0123

RW LJ

Mutual information analysis

Mutual information

Stimulus sensitive Task focused

0.20.51.02.05.010.020.0

FX RW LJ

Retinal image displacement analysis

log(Inertia) [deg^2]

Stimulus sensitive Task focused

← Figure 4d: Statistics of inertia (log-scale) of retinal image shifts per condition and participant group.

Figure 4c: Statistics of mutual information. Black bars indicate mean and 95% confidence interval for the 16 participants. Color indicates the distribution of MI for each group - stimulus

sensitive (n=9) & task focused (n=7) - based on their MI in LJ.

Shpiro, A., Moreno-Bote, R., Rubin, N., & Rinzel, J. (2009). Balance between noise and adaptation in competition models of perceptual bistability. Journal of computational neuroscience, 27(1), 37-54.

Moreno-Bote, R., Knill, D. C., & Pouget, A. (2011). Bayesian sampling in visual perception.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(30), 12491-12496.

Figure 1: Schematic diagram of bistable energy potential.

Objectives

Measures:

M1. Inertia:

M2. Mutual information: I(X; Y ) = H(Y ) H(Y |X)

Percept reversal speeds (RS) calculated from measured phase times. As in Blake et al.,

(2003) RS [rev/min] was computed:

Micro-smooth pursuits? Gaze during LJ seemed to follow cube trajectory (see Fig. 4b).

Similarity between gaze in fixations & cube trajectory computed with mutual information (see M2 & Fig. 4c):

LJ ≠ RW

Retinal image displacement, mean fixation inertia (see M1) w.r.t. gaze position (see Fig. 4d):

FX ≈ RW ≈ LJ

RW & LJ: variance of stimulus sensitive group smaller than task focused group.

→ RW & LJ tend to affect control of retinal image motion in stimulus sensitive group.

Results:

RW will increase RS → not strong enough LJ will decrease RS → opposite

LJ results differ from binocular rivalry (Blake; Sobel & Geloy, 2003)

with H(Y) the marginal entropy and H(Y|X) the conditional entropy under the assumptions of normal joint distributions and independence.

FX reversal speed

Models: Visual processing noise → noise triggers percept reversal.

Hypothesis: Unpredictable retinal image motion

↪ variance of visual information input

↪ increase system noise

↪ observe higher reversal speed.

I

s

= 1 N

s

Ns

X

i=1

( ¯ x

i2

+ ¯ y

i2

)

N

s

: number of signal s samples

( ¯ x

i

, y ¯

i

): spatial coordinates of signal s adjusted to di↵erent referential (e.g.

center of screen, center of fixation, stimulus position, etc)

Is = 1 Ns

Ns

X

i=1

( ¯xi2 + ¯yi2)

Ns: number of signal s samples

( ¯xi,y¯i): spatial coordinates of signal s adjusted to di↵erent referential (e.g.

center of screen, center of fixation, stimulus position, etc)

RSseq = nseq 60 Tseq

Références

Documents relatifs

2 Influence of the number of B1 helpers remaining in control and removal colonies after the removal treatment on (a) colony survival (= proportion of nests that succeeded in

The second application, Local Browsing, requests another input type: Instead of providing a location, the user can browse by nearby keywords.. Local Browsing delivers a list of

a - Original : full matrix and full answers parts visible all the time b - Switch : full matrix or full answers parts visible by clicking on it c - Line : one single line of the

Chacun son tour, compléter une case du tableau en fonction du temps travaillé.. Vérifiez le travail avec le tableau

A comparison of these data to two control conditions in which training trials of a single type (backward or forward) are presented at both 12.5 ° and − 25 ° eye elevations

A possible arhiteture of how the major brain areas involved in the ontrol of. voluntary saadi eye movements might ooperate.(Refer to the text

The results show that the new controller can reach a target posed at a starting distance of 1.2 meters in less than 100 control steps (1 second) and it can follow a moving target at

Pairwise comparisons showed that correct saccades were larger when the target stimulus was a face than a vehicle (face: 6.75 ± 0.65°, Vehicle: 6.26 ± 0.89°, p &lt; 0.005) but