• Aucun résultat trouvé

Are rats able to detect stress odours in chicken droppings?

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Are rats able to detect stress odours in chicken droppings?"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-01351520

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01351520

Submitted on 2 Jun 2020

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

Are rats able to detect stress odours in chicken droppings?

Vincent Bombail, Blandine Barret, Laurence Guilloteau, Christine Leterrier, Birte Nielsen

To cite this version:

Vincent Bombail, Blandine Barret, Laurence Guilloteau, Christine Leterrier, Birte Nielsen. Are rats able to detect stress odours in chicken droppings?. 50. International Society for Applied Ethology Congress (ISAE 2016), Jul 2016, Edimbourg, United Kingdom. 2016. �hal-01351520�

(2)

Are rats able to detect stress odours in chicken droppings?

Vincent Bombail a , Blandine Barret a , Laurence Guilloteau b , Christine Leterrier b , and Birte L Nielsen a

a Neurobiologie de l’Olfaction, INRA, Université Paris-Saclay, Jouy-en-Josas, France

b Physiologie de la Reproduction et des Comportements, INRA, Nouzilly, France

• Rats can distinguish between faeces from stressed and non-stressed conspecifics (Valenta & Rigby, 1968; Mackay-Sim & Laing, 1980) .

• If they respond in a similar manner to odours emitted by excreta from stressed chickens and rats, this could indicate a specific odorous stress signature, which might be used for non invasive detection of stress levels (e.g. biomarker)

• We used faeces/excreta from stressed rats (chronic variable stress done as part of another experiment) and chickens (24h simulated transport stress), with matched non-stressed control

Test 1. Male Brown Norway (BN) rats (n=12) were tested for 5 min in a large T-maze (100 x 77 cm 2 ) with a perforated metal ball (tea-ball) placed in each arm of the maze. All rats were tested 6 times, with tests 1 and 6 done with empty tea-balls in both arms. In between, each rat was tested twice with rat faeces and twice with chicken excreta. In each test, the two

tea-balls contained stressed and control faeces/excreta, always from the same species.

Test 2. Male BN rats (n=16) were exposed for 10 min to an odour presented in a

container with a perforated lid in a test arena (50 x 25 cm 2 ). All rats were tested 6 times, with tests 1 and 6 done with an empty container (no odour). In between, each rat was tested once with each of the four types of excreta/faeces in a Latin square design.

Results 1. Rats sniffed the control excreta/faeces longer than they sniffed the stressed excreta/faeces (16.6 vs 13.0 ±0.92 s; P<0.001). The duration of first sniffing was longer for the rat faeces than the chicken excreta (3.6 vs 2.7 ±0.31 s; F1,85=4.1; P=0.045) .

Results 2. Rats spent longer in contact with (17.4 vs 13.7 s; P=0.020) and longer sniffing the stressed than the control odours (15.5 vs 12.6 s; P=0.036). The percentage of time spent in the odour half in contact with the odour container was larger for the stressed odours (10.4 vs 7.9%; P=0.028).

Figure 1. Zoomed chromatogram from GC-MS analyses of

excreta from stressed (black) and control (red) chickens. The area under the peaks indicate the relative abundance of

phenylmethanol – a potential candidate for avian stress odour.

Conclusions

• The two different testing paradigms yielded different responses to stressed excreta/faeces

• However, within each test, the same differences were

seen between stressed and non-stressed excreta/faeces independent of species of origin.

• This would indicate that – both in rats and chickens – stress gives rise to specific volatile organic compounds (VOCs).

• A potential candidate molecule is shown in Figure 1

Funding

This work is supported by INRA métaprogramme GISA (integrated management of animal health)

References

Valenta JG, Rigby MK (1968). Discrimination of the odor of stressed rats. Science 161, 599–601.

Mackay-Sim A, Laing DG (1980). Discrimination of odors from stressed rats by non-stressed rats. Physiol Behav 24, 699–704.

P = 0.008

P = 0.036

Stressed Control

Références

Documents relatifs

Figure 16 : Variation de l’activité enzymatique de catalase cytosolique hépatique et rénale après 30jours de l’administration de la deltaméthrine par voie orale

On étudie l’influence des vibrations verticales harmoniques de hautes fréquences et de faibles amplitudes sur la séparation des espèces d’un mélange binaire saturant

Analyses of a system’s concurrency aspects is performed based on its MoCApplication, depending on the MoC used. For instance, Petri nets [30] are a common formalism to specify

Technical Memorandum (National Research Council of Canada. Associate Committee on Soil and Snow Mechanics), 1957-06-01.. READ THESE TERMS AND CONDITIONS CAREFULLY BEFORE USING

The purpose of the present paper is to describe and validate the hybrid spectral-particle numerical approach used in [9], and to discuss its efficiency, in particular in comparison

To evaluate the effectiveness of agomelatine, venlafaxine, and voluntary wheel running exercise in the prevention of stress-induced anxiety-, depression-, and memory

Independent of age, acute stress improved response inhibition, reflected in higher accuracy for compatible trials and enhanced inhibition-related components (N2, P3 and N2d, P3d of

Strain differences in hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis cross- sensitization were determined by plasma corticosterone (CORT) levels and hippocampal gene expression