• Aucun résultat trouvé

No effect of classroom sharing on educational achievement in twins: a prospective, longitudinal cohort study

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "No effect of classroom sharing on educational achievement in twins: a prospective, longitudinal cohort study"

Copied!
21
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-00490561

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00490561 Submitted on 9 Jun 2010

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access

archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci-entific research documents, whether they are pub-lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

No effect of classroom sharing on educational

achievement in twins: a prospective, longitudinal cohort study

Tinca Jc Polderman, Meike Bartels, Frank C Verhulst, Anja C Huizink, Catherina Em van Beijsterveldt, Dorret I Boomsma

To cite this version:

(2)

RESEARCH

No effect of classroom sharing on educational achievement in twins: a

prospective, longitudinal cohort study

Tinca J. C. Polderman, postdoctoral fellow*1,2, Meike Bartels, assistant professor1,

Frank C. Verhulst, psychiatrist, professor, head2, Anja C. Huizink, associate

professor2, 3, Catharina E. M. van Beijsterveldt, assistant professor1, and Dorret I.

Boomsma, professor, head1

*to whom correspondence should be addressed

1 Department of Biological Psychology, VU University Amsterdam

Van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Tel. 0031 20 598 69 51

Fax. 0031 20 598 88 32

Email: jc.polderman@psy.vu.nl

2 Erasmus University Medical Center - Sophia Children's Hospital, Department of

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Rotterdam, The Netherlands

3 Department of Education, University of Amsterdam,

The Netherlands

(3)

ABSTRACT

Objective:

To investigate the effects of sharing a classroom during primary school on cognitive achievement in twins

Design:

Prospective, longitudinal study in twins registered at birth

Setting:

The Netherlands Twin Register

Participants:

839 monozygotic and 1164 dizygotic twin pairs

Main Outcome Measure:

Educational achievement at age 12 years measured with a standardized test (CITO test) used by 93% of the Dutch primary schools.

Results:

Most twin pairs (72%) shared a classroom during their schooling, 19% were in separate, but parallel classes, and 9% ‘partly’ shared a classroom. Twins who were in parallel classrooms had higher CITO scores (mean 539.51; sd 8.12), compared to twins who shared a classroom (537.99; sd 8.52). When controlling for socio-economic status and externalizing problems before starting primary school (age 3) there was no significant difference in educational achievement between separated and non-separated twin pairs (P=.138). In addition, there was no interaction with sex or zygosity of the twins (P=.798).

(4)

INTRODUCTION

Being part of a twin pair has benefits like always having a friend and companion around, and being part of a unique and special relationship that is not available for singletons. Having a co-twin may, however, also have possible detriments like the inability to develop as a unique and independent individual. Families with multiple births naturally want to offer the optimal conditions in which twins can function effectively, both as multiples as well as individuals. A returning dilemma for them is whether twins should share the same, or a parallel classroom, in other words, whether they should be separated at school or not.

A survey in the UK showed that only one percent of schools had written policies about the education and management of multiple birth children. Also, the decision to separate or not, was often made by educators alone, without the input of parents.1 Most twins spent their schooling years together and 7% of schools reported

they always separated twins. In the Netherlands an increasing practice of separating twins is reported2,3 while in Scandinavian countries multiples almost always share the

classroom.4 Educational policies and practice –independent of a focus on sharing or

separation- seem to be based on popularized stereotypic depictions of twins (in general monozygotic pairs) rather then on empirical research.5 Hay and Preedy6

emphasize a clear and evidence based recommendation on which school policies and even ongoing legislative initiatives in the USA7 can be based, as ‘teachers and

parents should be aware of particular issues that may affect the physical,

intellectual, personal, social, and emotional development of multiple birth children’. Given this argument and the growing multiple birth rate8,9 it is surprising that only

three studies investigated whether or not classroom separation at the primary school is beneficial or detrimental for the behavioral and cognitive development of twins.

(5)

All studies were based on rather large twin samples: a British sample of 878 twin pairs10, a Dutch sample of 5128 twin pairs3, and a Dutch sample of 2878 twin pairs.11

Both Dutch studies (only little overlap in samples; 3.92%) had longitudinal data covering the complete primary school period (i.e. grade 1 to 8), while the study of Tully et al.10 was limited to the first three years of school (age 5 to 7).

The major finding of the first two studies was that internalizing problems in young twins could be attributed to separation of the twins at the beginning of their schooling. However, the effects sizes were small and in the Dutch sample the effects had disappeared in grade 8. For externalizing problems Tully et al.10 found no

differences, while van Leeuwen et al.3 found an effect of separation. Externalizing

problems in separated twins were explained by pre-existing differences between twins, suggesting that the externalizing problems could have been a motivation for parents and teachers to separate the twins. In addition, van Leeuwen et al.3 found

that socio-economic status (SES) was significantly associated with the classroom placement of a twin pair (i.e. separated or together); families with lower SES had their twins more often placed together.

For the cognitive development of twins Webbink et al.11 found no important

differences between twins sharing a classroom, or not. Their results showed a small positive effect for non-separated twins on language and arithmetic in Grade 2, but in the higher grades no effects were found. As zygositity of the twins in this study was unknown the interaction with classroom separation and zygosity was not

investigated. Tully et al.10 found lower reading scores for separated twins, but this

was only the case for monozygotic (MZ) twins at age 7 who were separated after one year of schooling, and this finding did not apply to twins who were separated earlier.

The current study expands on this prior work by examining the effects of classroom separation on educational achievement in a large longitudinal sample of

(6)

examine whether the effects of classroom separation differed for male and female, and for monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins. We used a standardized Dutch achievement test, called CITO test, which is assessed in grade 8 at 93% of the Dutch primary schools. The CITO test highly correlates with IQ performance12 and plays an

important advisory role in the choice of secondary school education. Several

confounding factors may bias the true association between classroom separation and the outcome measure. The following factors were taken into account: a) zygosity (MZ or DZ), b) socio-economic status, c) urbanization (as schools situated in low urbanized areas, might not offer the opportunity to separate twins because only one classroom for a specific grade is available), and d) preexisting and current

externalizing problems.3

METHODS

Sample

All twins were registered with the Netherlands Twin Register (NTR),

established in 1986 by the Department of Biological Psychology at the VU University in Amsterdam. Of all multiple births in the Netherlands about 50% is registered in the NTR.13,14 Data of twins from the 1986–1993 birth cohorts were used in this

study. Surveys on development, health, psychopathology and socio-demographic characteristics have been collected longitudinally at the ages of 1, 2, 3, 5, 7, 10 and 12 years. Response rates at ages 3, 7, 10 and 12 years were 72%, 66%, 64% and 64% respectively (note that if a family did not participate at a particular age, they were approached again for the next mailing).15 For this study, information from

(7)

Together or separated

The surveys sent to the mothers of twins at ages 5 and 12 years contained questions on whether the twins were in the same class. In the Netherlands, most children start primary school at the age of 4 years; compulsory education, however, starts at the age of 5 years. Nearly all children attend primary school for 8 years and go to secondary school around the age of 12 years. The separation of twin pairs can occur when children first start school or during primary school. Mothers of twins who were 12 years of age were asked ‘which statement applies best to the school history of your twins’ a) same school, same classroom b) same school, parallel classes (i.e., same level) c) same school, different levels d) different schools e) partly same class, partly separated. When the twins were 5 years old, the mother was asked whether ‘the twins are now a) together in the same school in the same classroom b) together in the same school but not in the same classroom and c) at different schools’.

Outcome measure

Educational achievement of the twins was assessed with the Dutch CITO-elementary test (Eindtoets Basisonderwijs, 2002, www.cito.nl). The CITO consists of 240 multiple-choice items assessing four different intellectual skills: Language, Mathematics, Information Processing, and World Orientation. Together the

(8)

to IQ (correlations of .41, .50, .60, and .63 between CITO scores assessed at age 12 and IQ performance at age 5, 7, 10, and 12, respectively).

Subjects included

Survey data at age 12 and CITO scores were present for 4929 twins. Twins were excluded a) because of a handicap (n=76); b) because they needed special education (n=15); c) because twins were in different schools (n=86); d) because twins were in different classes (n=513), that represented different levels/grades; e) because data on classroom placement were missing or incomplete (n=88); and f) when the CITO score of one twin of a pair was missing (n= 145, we have no information on the reason for the incomplete data), leaving 4006 twins (2003 twin pairs) in the sample. Of these 370 pairs were monozygotic male pairs (MZM), 269 were dizygotic male pairs (DZM), 469 were monozygotic female pairs (MZF), 302 were dizygotic female pairs (DZF) and 593 were dizygotic opposite sex pairs (DOS). Zygosity was determined by DNA or blood group polymorphisms for 702 pairs. For the remaining same sex twin pairs (n=708), zygosity was based on questionnaire items. Zygosity determination using this questionnaire is 93% accurate.16

Thus the 2003 twin pairs we used in the analyses were healthy, typically developing 12-year-old (complete) twin pairs who were during primary school non-separated (same school, same classroom), non-separated (same school, parallel class) and partly separated (partly same classroom, partly separated; there is no

information about when and how long these twins were separated).

(9)

Possible confounding factors

Socio Economic Status (SES)

Data on Socio Economic Status (SES) from the survey mailed out when the twins were 3,7 and 10 years old were included in the analyses as van Leeuwen et al.3 reported less separated twins in low SES families. For respectively 1752

(assessed at age 3), 1762 (assessed at age 7) and 1766 (assessed at age 10) families SES data were available. As SES scores between age 3, 7 and 10 correlated highly (varying from 0.70 to 0.78), we used the SES score of age 7 when age 10 was not available and the SES score of age 3 when scores at age 7 and 10 were not available. This provided a SES score for 1972 families.

SES was based on a full description of the occupation of the parents and classified using a 5-point scale (1=lowest, 5=highest), according to the system used by Statistics Netherlands.17 For the SES assessment at age 10 the EPG-classification

scheme was used; this combines occupation with information on parental

education.18 In both cases the highest SES score of the two parents determined the

SES of the twin pair. The distribution of SES (from low to high) was 1%, 14%, 43%, 27% and 15%.

Urbanization level

(10)

level (scale of 0–4: very high, high, moderate, low, very low/none). When we had no data on zip codes at age 12, we used data at age 14 or 16. Data on urbanization were available for 1981 families. Missing data (22 families) were due to the fact that some families had moved to new areas with zip codes that were introduced after 2004. The distribution of urbanization (from low to high) was 29%, 23%, 17%, 23% and 8%.

Externalizing problems

We examined whether externalizing problems at age 3, predicted separation at the beginning of the twin’s school career. As part of the twins change in their school placement, we also examined externalizing problems at age 10, as they possibly caused classroom separation later in school. Externalizing problems were assessed with a broad band scale of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) completed by the mother. The Externalizing scale is based on the Aggressive and Rule Breaking Behavior subscales of the CBCL.19 Items are scored on a 3-point scale (i.e., not true

(0), somewhat or sometimes true (1) and very true or often true (2); ratings are based on the occurrence of the behavior during the preceding 6 months.

The highest externalizing score within a twin of a pair was used. Data were available for 1682 complete twin pairs at age 3 (for 260 pairs the survey at age 3 was not completed, for 61 pairs the survey was completed but the items on

externalizing problems were incomplete or missing for at least one twin). At age 10 there were externalizing problem scores for 1781 twin pairs (for 192 pairs the survey at age 10 was not completed, for 30 pairs the survey was completed but the items on externalizing problems were incomplete or missing for at least one twin).

(11)

Data analyses were performed in SPSS 15.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, Illinois). First it was tested at family level whether SES and urbanization levels differed between twin pairs that shared a classroom and twin pairs who were in separate classrooms (χ²-tests). In addition we tested the possibility that externalizing

problems at age 3 differed between both groups at the start of school or later in school (one-way Anova; at age 12 with Bonferroni post-hoc comparisons as in this case data from three groups were analyzed: separated, non-separated and partly separated twins).

Secondly, we tested for significant differences in CITO scores between twins who shared a classroom the entire school period and twins who were in separate classrooms during their schooling. Because twin data consist of non-independent observations, for these analyses the Mixed Modeling option in SPSS was used, in which a correction for family dependency is applied.20,21 In the full model CITO

scores were the dependent variable, while classroom separation, zygosity, sex, and an interaction effect between classroom separation and zygosity were included as fixed effects. Significant predictors for classroom separation at age 5 and at age 12 were also included as fixed effects. Family and zygosity status were incorporated in the model as random effects. Parameter estimation was by maximum likelihood. The type-I error rate was set at 0.01 to accommodate multiple testing.

RESULTS

Descriptives

(12)

This pattern was the same in male and female, MZ and DZ twins. The history of separation (i.e., classroom placement at age 5 compared to age 12) showed that 96 pairs (8%) that were non-separated at age 5 were separated at age 12, and 187 pairs (37%) that were separated at age 5 were non-separated at age 12. Overall, 16% of twin pairs had changed their placement during primary school.

Confounding factors

Classroom separation at age 5 and at age 12 was significantly associated with SES (χ²= 22.909, df= 4, p<.001 and χ²= 38.028, df= 4, p<.001, resp.) and with

urbanization (χ²= 55.257, df= 4, p<.001 and χ²=80.118, df= 4, p<.001, resp.).

Twins from lower SES families and lower urbanization levels were more likely to share a classroom. Also externalizing problems at age 3 were significantly associated with classroom separation at age 5 (F (1, 1583) =8.747, p=.003). Externalizing problems at age 10 and classroom separation at age 12 showed a trend for association (F (2, 1794) =3.939, p=.020) but the Bonferroni pot-hoc test showed that this reflected a difference between non-separated twins and partly separated twins (p=.034) and not between non-separated and separated twins (p=.341). Hence, the covariates that were used in the Linear Mixed Modeling procedure were urbanization, externalizing problems at age 3, and SES.

Classroom separation and educational achievement

Without adjusting for the confounding factors, a significant

association between classroom separation and CITO scores was present (F

(2, 1931) =7.200, p=.001). The effect size (

r

) of classroom separation was

0.08. After adjusting for confounding effects, there was no significant effect

of classroom separation on CITO scores (

F

(2, 1653) =1.985,

p

=.138), and

(13)

effect between classroom separation and zygosity (

F

(8, 686) =0.576,

p

=.798), thus the association between classroom separation and CITO

scores is the same for male and female MZ, male and female DZ and

opposite-sex twins.

Externalizing problems at age 3 (B= -0.05,

t

(1659)= -3.40,

p

= 0.001)

and SES (B= 2.55,

t

(1647)= 13.70,

p

< 0.000) were significant covariates in

the association between CITO scores and classroom separation.

Urbanization was not a significant confounder (B= -0.07,

t

(1639)= -.54,

p

=

0.59). Effect sizes (

r

) of the covariates were 0.08, 0.32 and 0.01

respectively.

Table 1 presents the mean CITO scores for separated and

non-separated twin pairs per zygosity group, before and after adjusting for

significant covariates.

INSERT TABLE 1

DISCUSSION

(14)

twins who are separated. Important covariates in the relation between classroom separation and CITO scores were socio-economic status and externalizing problems. Our results confirm those of Webbink et al.11 who also found no effect of classroom

separation on cognitive abilities. We further showed that these results are the same for male and female, and MZ and DZ twins. The question whether separation has an influence on internalizing or externalizing problem behaviors of twins was already answered by van Leeuwen et al.3; also for these outcomes there was no important

effect of separation.

A remaining question is whether social development interacts with sharing a classroom with your co-twin. Because twins share their age and developmental needs, they do share friends more often than other siblings.22 An often used

argument of schools in the Netherlands is that separation stimulates the individual development of cognition as well as in social contacts. In the current study a small subsample of 12-year-old twins (from cohorts 1990-1992, N=169 pairs) answered the question ‘do you share the same friends’. For twins who shared a classroom 74% reported ‘yes, we share the same friends’, 20% answered ‘no, we have different friends’, and 6% reported to share ‘some friends’. For twins who were in separated classrooms this was respectively 25%, 50% and 25%, and for twins who partly shared a classroom 50%, 38% and 12%. These outcomes indicate that twins who share a classroom have more mutual friends. However, these results only apply to the primary school period. In this phase friendships might predominantly be based on classroom mates and therefore classroom separation explains the difference in the sharing of friends. Whether or not these shared friendships continue in life needs to be investigated.

(15)

Methodological strengths of this study were the prospective, longitudinal design, the large sample size and the correction for confounding effects like

externalizing problems in the twins, and urbanization levels and SES of the family. The large sample size made it possible to test for interaction effects of separation with sex and zygosity. Lastly, the sample in this study was population based which makes the results representative of the normal twin population in the Netherlands and probably most Western countries.

This study has some limitations.

We had no information on the reason

why in some cases the CITO score of one twin was missing. However,

inspection of the missing cases gave no indication for any systematic

underlying factors that could explain these missing data.

Furthermore, the data were of birth cohorts 1986 to 1993; it is possible that cognitive achievement differs between cohorts, but no cohort effects in CITO scores were found in the current analyses. Data on separation were derived from parental reports and not from a more objective source, like school records. A specific group in the analyses was the ‘partly separated’ group. Nine percent of the mothers reported that her twins had ‘partly’ been separated. We tested whether externalizing problems at age 10 might be a reason to separate twins later in school; this was not the case. One might think of other, for example practical reasons like moving of twin families, or expansion or reduction of a school population that enhances a separation or ‘coming together’ later in school.

Conclusions

(16)
(17)

WHAT IS ALREADY KNOWN ON THIS TOPIC

Two previous studies on behavioral and emotional problems suggest that early separated twin pairs show more internalizing problems compared to non-separated twin pairs in the first grades of primary school. The effect sizes are small however, and the effects have disappeared at the end of primary school. One study reports no differences between separated or non-separated twin pairs on cognitive development but has no information on zygosity.

WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS

We replicate the findings on cognitive development using an educational

achievement test that has high predictive value for future educational achievement. We find no differences in cognitive scores between separated and non-separated twin pairs. Socio economic status and externalizing problems interact with classroom separation and educational achievement in twins, but sex and zygositiy do not (i.e., the effects are the same in male and female, MZ, DZ and opposite-sex twins).

Contributors:

All authors contributed towards the conception and design of the project, interpretation of the results, a critical review of the manuscript, and

approved the final version. TJCP, MB and DIB designed the data analysis plan. TJCP performed the data analysis, wrote the manuscript, and is guarantor. CEM managed the data.

Funding:

This work was supported by NWO 904-57-94, NWO/SPI 56-464-14192; NWO 480-04-004; NIMH, RO1 MH58799-03. TJCP is funded by Sophia Foundation for Scientific Research (SSWO, no. 562).

Competing interests:

All authors declare that the answer to the questions on your competing interest form are all No and therefore have nothing to declare.

(18)

Copyright:

the Corresponding Author has the right to grant on behalf of all authors and does grant on behalf of all authors, an exclusive licence (or non exclusive for government employees) on a worldwide basis to the BMJ Publishing Group Ltd to permit this article (if accepted) to be published in BMJ editions and any other BMJPGL products and sublicences such use and exploit all subsidiary rights.

Provenance and peer review:

Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Acknowledgements

(19)

Reference List

1 Preedy P. Meeting the educational needs of preschool and primary aged twins and higher multiples. In: Sandbank AC, eds. Twin and Triplet Psychology. London: Routledge, 1999:70-99.

2 Geluk A, Hol J. Samen of apart? Meerlingkinderen naar school, peuterspeelzaal of kinderopvang [Together or apart? Multiples to school, playground or

daycare]. NVOM brochure . 2001. Bergen, The Netherlands, NVOM (Dutch Society for Parents of Multiples).

3 van Leeuwen M, van den Berg SM, van Beijsterveldt TCEM, Boomsma DI. Effects of twin separation in primary school. Twin Res Hum Genet 2005;8:384-91.

4 Hay DA. Together or apart? Twin Res 2004;7:iii-iiv.

5 Beauchamp HM, Brooks LJ. The perceptions, policy, and practice of educating twins: A review. Psychol Sch 2003;40:429-38.

6 Hay DA, Preedy P. Meeting the educational needs of multiple birth children. Early Hum Dev 2006;82:397-403.

7 Segal NL. Same or separate classrooms: a twin bill. Twin Res Hum Genet 2006;9:473-8.

8 Hall JG. Twinning. Lancet 2003;362:735-43.

9 Herskind AM, Basso O, Olsen J, Skytthe A, Christensen K. Is the natural twinning rate still declining? Epidemiology 2005;16:591-2.

10 Tully LA, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Kiernan H, Andreou P. What effect does classroom separation have on twins' behavior, progress at school, and reading abilities? Twin Res 2004;7:115-24.

11 Webbink D, Hay D, Visscher PM. Does sharing the same class in school improve cognitive abilities of twins? Twin Res Hum Genet 2007;10:573-80.

12 Bartels M, Rietveld MJ, van Baal GC, Boomsma DI. Heritability of educational achievement in 12-year-olds and the overlap with cognitive ability. Twin Res 2002;5:544-53.

13 Boomsma DI, Vink JM, van Beijsterveldt TCEM, de Geus EJC, Beem AL, Mulder EJCM et al. Netherlands Twin Register: A focus on longitudinal research. Twin Res 2002;5:401-6.

(20)

15 Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt CE, Derks EM, Stroet TM, Polderman TJ, Hudziak JJ et al. Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): a longitudinal multiple

informant study of problem behavior. Twin Res Hum Genet 2007;10:3-11. 16 Rietveld MJ, van D, V, Bongers IL, Stroet TM, Slagboom PE, Boomsma DI.

Zygosity diagnosis in young twins by parental report. Twin Res 2000;3:134-41. 17 Fengler M, Joung I, Mackenbach JP. Sociaal demongrafische kenmerken en

gezondheid: Hun relatief belang en onderlinge relaties (Social demographic characteristics and health: Their relative importance and mutual relations). In: Mackenbach JP, Verkleij H, eds. Volksgezondheid Toekomst Verkenning 1997, II Gezondheidsverschillen. Maarssen, The Netherlands: Elsevier/De Tijdstroom, 1997:49-67.

18 Erikson R, Goldthorpe JH, Portocarero L. Intergenerational Class Mobility in 3 Western European Societies - England, France and Sweden. Br J Sociol 1979;30:415-41.

19 Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry, 1991.

20 Boomsma DI, van Beijsterveld TCEM, Beem AL, Hoekstra RA, Polderman TJC, Bartels M. Intelligence and birth order in boys and girls. Intelligence

2008;36:630-4.

21 Beem AL, Boomsma DI. Implementation of a combined association-linkage model for quantitative traits in linear mixed model procedures of statistical packages. Twin Res Hum Genet 2006;9:325-33.

(21)

Table 1

Mean scores of separated and non-separated MZ and DZ, male, female and opposite sex

twins,

with and without correction of SES and externalizing problem age 3

CITO score CITO score including sign.

covariates* N twin pairs 1. Non-separated MZM 539.34 (7.49) 539.38 (7.40) 286 2. Separated MZM 540.82 (8.30) 540.12 (8.76) 60 3. Partly MZM 540.53 (5.82) 540.16 (5.50) 24 total MZM 370 pairs 1. Non-separated DZM 537.78 (8.31) 538.33 (8.11) 175 2. Separated DZM 539.25 (7.65) 539.06 (7.71) 66 3. Partly DZM 538.96 (7.83) 540.00 (7.62) 28 total DZM 269 pairs 1. Non-separated MZF 537.38 (9.22) 537.62 (9.16) 339 2. Separated MZF 538.86 (7.49) 538.35 (7.46) 89 3. Partly MZF 538.56 (7.66) 538.39 (7.57) 41 total MZF 469 pairs 1. Non-separated DZF 537.03 (8.25) 537.38 (8.43) 217 2. Separated DZF 538.50 (8.77) 538.11 (9.20) 59 3. Partly DZF 538.21 (7.43) 538.15 (7.75) 26 total DZF 302 pairs 1. Non-separated DOS 538.30 (8.62) 538.08 (8.63) 427 2. Separated DOS 539.78 (8.32) 538.81 (8.37) 106 3. Partly DOS 539.48 (7.93) 538.85 (8.11) 60

total DOS 593 pairs

total 2003 pairs

Note:

Non-separated= same school, same classroom; Separated= same school, parallel classrooms; Partly= partly

the same classroom, partly separate classrooms

MZM= monozygotic males; DZM= dizygotic males; MZF= monozygotic females; DZF= dizygotic females; DOS= dizygotic opposite-sex

Références

Documents relatifs

Alemtuzumab versus interferon beta 1a as first-line treatment for patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis: a randomised controlled phase 3 trial. Alemtuzumab for

We hope to motivate further investigation into these structures (e.g., by measuring the in-plane polarization at each wall) and their role in the performance of hybrid

For planar quasicrystals the quasirational rotations commute witu tue elements of tue point symmetry group of tue quasicrystal (ail two-dimensional rotations commute), uence tue

In the following we give an example of K, = (1072) deformation twin in Zr and a K, = (1 122) deformation twin in Ti with a preliminary analysis of the dislocation arrays

logarithm of cells concentrations for measurements carried out in a microfluidic mode with different

We then provide a careful analysis in the case of a ball by: (1) characterizing the solution among all radially symmetric resources distributions, with the help of a new

L’accès à ce site Web et l’utilisation de son contenu sont assujettis aux conditions présentées dans le site LISEZ CES CONDITIONS ATTENTIVEMENT AVANT D’UTILISER CE SITE WEB.

Lack of the antioxidant enzyme methionine sulfoxide reductase A in mice impairs RPE phagocytosis and causes photoreceptor cone dysfunction.. Francesca Mazzoni a , Ying Dun a ,