• Aucun résultat trouvé

Measuring Belief in Fake News in Real-Time (Extended Abstract)

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Partager "Measuring Belief in Fake News in Real-Time (Extended Abstract)"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Measuring Belief in Fake News in Real-Time

?

Invited Talk - Extended Abstract

Kevin Aslett1, William Godel2, Zeve Sanderson1, Nathaniel Persily3, Jonathan Nagler1,2, Richard Bonneau4, and Joshua A Tucker1,2

1 Center for Social Media and Politics, New York University, New York NY 10011, USA

2 Wilf Family Department of Politics, New York University, New York NY 10012, USA

3 Stanford Law School, Stanford University, Stanford CA 94305, USA

4 Center for Genomics and Systems Biology, New York University, New York NY 10003, USA

Interest in the spread of fake news and misinformation online has increased dramatically since the 2016 US presidential election, and the relevance of mis- information to politics has only grown during the Covid-19 pandemic. However, we know little about levels of belief in fake news encountered shortly after pub- lication, as well as what types of people are more likely to believe fake news.

To address this gap in the literature, we fielded two studies in which we repeat- edly asked representative samples of Americans to evaluate popular articles from non-credible and credible sources within 24-48 hours of their publication.

We find that, on average, false or misleading articles are rated as true 33.2%

of the time; moreover, approximately 90% of individuals coded at least one false or misleading article as true when given a set of four false or misleading ar- ticles. While most demographic characteristics co-vary only slightly with the likelihood of correctly identifying fake news stories, we find a very strong re- lationship for ideological-congruence: both conservatives and liberals are much more likely to believe false/misleading information if it reflects their ideologi- cal perspectives than if it does not, and this effect is stronger than previously measured in other studies. We also find that older respondents are less likely to believe false/misleading information relative to younger respondents. This adds important context to the existing findings that older Americans are more likely to share fake news on Facebook [4], suggesting that sharing behavior may be divorced from actual belief.

Finally, using two different experiments, we tested whether a common pre- scription for lowering belief in fake news, encouraging news consumers to search for information about a fake news article, actually reduces belief in fake news.

Paradoxically, we find that encouraging respondents to search for information to inform one’s evaluation of an article’s veracity increases the likelihood that an individual believes that fake news article. Evidence from real-time Google

?Copyright 2021 for this paper by its authors. Use permitted under Creative Commons License Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0). Presented at the MISINFO 2021 workshop held in conjunction with the 30th ACM The Web Conference, 2021, in Ljubljana, Slovenia. This talk was delivered by Joshua A. Tucker.

(2)

searches suggests that this pattern is driven by the existence of similar infor- mation elsewhere on the internet, even when the quality of that “supporting”

information is also low. In our two experiments, we find that encouraging respon- dents to search for information only increases belief in false/misleading stories when Google search engines return information from low-quality sources that corroborate claims made in that fake news article.

When running a second study using the same survey instrument, but strictly limiting our articles to those about Covid-19, we find that slightly more respon- dents believed popular fake news about Covid-19 than general fake news articles.

We also find that the salience of partisan divisions remains. Both the strong ef- fect of ideological congruence and age identified in our first study hold when we focus our analysis strictly on fake news articles about Covid-19. Similarly, we again find that encouraging respondents to search for information about fake news articles Covid-19 also increases their belief in this information. This in- dicates that attempts by Google during the Covid-19 pandemic to remove or mitigate data voids may not have been as successful as we might have hoped.

These studies show that belief in online fake news is higher than previously identified [3, 6, 2, 7, 5, 1] and suggests that efforts to encourage people to search for corroborating information may be exacerbating this problem.

References

1. Allen, J., Arechar, A.A., Pennycook, G., Rand, D.: Scaling up fact-checking using the wisdom of crowds (2020)

2. Bronstein, M.V., Pennycook, G., Bear, A., Rand, D.G., Cannon, T.D.: Belief in fake news is associated with delusionality, dogmatism, religious fundamentalism, and reduced analytic thinking. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 8(1), 108–117 (2019)

3. Clayton, K., Blair, S., Busam, J.A., Forstner, S., Glance, J., Green, G., Kawata, A., Kovvuri, A., Martin, J., Morgan, E., et al.: Real solutions for fake news? measuring the effectiveness of general warnings and fact-check tags in reducing belief in false stories on social media. Political Behavior pp. 1–23 (2019)

4. Guess, A., Nagler, J., Tucker, J.: Less than you think: Prevalence and predictors of fake news dissemination on facebook. Science advances5(1), eaau4586 (2019) 5. Pennycook, G., Epstein, Z., Mosleh, M., Arechar, A.A., Eckles, D., Rand, D.: Under-

standing and reducing the spread of misinformation online. Unpublished manuscript:

https://psyarxiv. com/3n9u8 (2019)

6. Pennycook, G., Rand, D.G.: Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition188, 39–50 (2019)

7. Pennycook, G., Rand, D.G.: Who falls for fake news? the roles of bullshit receptivity, overclaiming, familiarity, and analytic thinking. Journal of personality88(2), 185–

200 (2020)

Références

Documents relatifs

We found that (1) the utilization of security features in enterprise repositories, (2) the adaptation of resource description and resource selec- tion for enterprise model, and

seule chose que je puisse admettre , c’est que , si la société des Templiers existait réellement , ce dont je doute beau- coup , du temps de Massillon , Fénélon et des autres

« Mais le souvenir que réveillent parmi nous les services des races antiques, n’en excite pas moins notre vénération ; loin de leur disputer le rang que le zèle et la valeur

Que des hommes oififs don- nent dans ces jeux enfantins , on tolére- ra leur amufement ; mais qu’ils n’entrai- nent point avec eux des gens qui pou- roient pafler pour

Clepip-unige RUN1 (map 10.52%): Weighting: TF*IDF, Length Filtering: yes, Class Filtering: yes Clepip-unige RUN2 (map 10.58%): Weighting TF*IDF, Length Filtering: yes, Class

This paper presents the outcomes of a survey of 117 respondents, investigating users’ prefer- ences for their type of search result (image, news, video) and their type of

In our framework, a tell action of agent a (the sender) towards agent b (the receiver) that ϕ 0 is true is considered as a private announcement in the sense of [22], that is,

Our approach demonstrated (1) the reusability of a large test collection originally created for TREC, (2) an experimental design that specifically considers types of