• Aucun résultat trouvé

Generalising from visual spatial patterns

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Generalising from visual spatial patterns"

Copied!
3
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-01286982

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01286982

Submitted on 11 Mar 2016

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

Generalising from visual spatial patterns

Aisling Twohill

To cite this version:

Aisling Twohill. Generalising from visual spatial patterns. CERME 9 - Ninth Congress of the European Society for Research in Mathematics Education, Charles University in Prague, Faculty of Education;

ERME, Feb 2015, Prague, Czech Republic. pp.506-507. �hal-01286982�

(2)

506 CERME9 (2015) – TWG03

Generalising from visual spatial patterns

Aisling Twohill

Dublin City University, St. Patrick’s College, Dublin, Ireland, aisling.twohill@spd.dcu.ie

In the research project presented, I categorise the strat- egies employed by four Fourth class girls and four Sixth class girls when asked to construct a general term for a visual spatial pattern. The approach adopted by each girl is categorised as either explicit or recursive, and the mode of generalisation is categorised as either linear or numeric.

Keywords: Visual spatial pattern, generalisation strategies, reasoning.

THEORETICAL BACKGROUND

Generalisation is considered by many to be a highly significant component of algebraic reasoning (Kaput, 2008). Patterning plays a key role in supporting chil- dren’s developing skills in generalisation, and inter- nationally visual spatial patterning tasks have been utilised in many research projects to investigate chil- dren’s success in generalising (e.g., Rivera & Becker, 2011).

Lannin (2004) presents two approaches to the con- struction of generalisations from patterns. An ‘ex- plicit’ approach involves identifying a rule for the relationship between a term and its position in the pattern, whereas a ‘recursive’ approach focuses on a relationship between successive terms. Rivera and Becker (2011) discuss the tendency of some children in their research to adopt a ‘numerical’ rather than ‘figu- ral’ mode of generalising, which in some cases caused difficulty in the children’s own reasoning about the generalisations they had constructed.

Within the algebra strand of the Irish Primary School Mathematics Curriculum (PSMC) it is proposed that children be facilitated in establishing rules for number sequences, but explicit approaches are not mentioned, and examples given all indicate constant differences between terms (Government of Ireland, 1999). Generalisation is not mentioned and visual

spatial patterning is not suggested for consideration, beyond repeating patterns of shapes aimed at the youngest children in primary school. In light of this, the research discussed in this paper aims to explore the responses of primary school children in Ireland when asked to construct generalisations from a visual spatial pattern.

METHOD

Individual clinical interviews were used to gather data on children’s constructions of generalisations.

Four girls were randomly selected from each of Fourth class and Sixth class in an Irish primary school. The cohorts from the two classes had mean ages of 10.08 years and 12.19 years respectively. Pseudonyms are used. The girls were asked to describe, extend and construct near and far generalisations from the pat- tern terms shown in Figure 1. Suggestive questions were asked to support children in making progress in the solution of tasks.

FINDINGS AND DISCUSSION

During the clinical interview, each girl was asked to identify the 100th term in the pattern, as a far gener- alisation. Each girl’s generalisation, or work towards a generalisation, was deemed to be figural or numeric.

A response was deemed to be figural if the child re- ferred to the position of the diamonds, by referring to the ‘top’ or ‘bottom’ or through gesture and use of the deictic ‘there’. Table 1 summarises the mode of generalisation adopted by each girl and whether the approach adopted in working with the pattern was recursive or explicit. An incomplete generalisation

Figure 1: Diamonds pattern

(3)

Generalising from visual spatial patterns (Aisling Twohill)

507 refers to an instance when a participant did not con-

struct the 100th term.

Within the PSMC the 6th Class girls may have en- countered many number sequences which favour recursive thinking. Lannin (2004) suggests that such immersion may cause difficulty in considering an ex- plicit approach. Additionally, the numeric approach of Aoife and Rachel seemed to inhibit their thinking about the general term, in a manner consistent with the findings of Rivera and Becker (2011).

REFERENCES

Government of Ireland (1999). Primary school curriculum math- ematics. Dublin, Ireland: The Stationery Office.

Kaput, J. J., Blanton, M. L., & Moreno, L. (2008). Algebra from a symbolization point of view. In J. J. Kaput, D. W. Carraher, &

M. L. Blanton (Eds.), Algebra in the early grades (pp. 19-56).

New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Ass.

Lannin, J. K. (2004). Developing mathematical power by using explicit and recursive reasoning. Mathematics Teacher, 98(4), 216-223.

Rivera, F., & Becker, J. R. (2011). Formation of pattern generaliza- tion involving linear figural patterns among middle school students. In J. Cai & E. Knuth (Eds.), Early algebraization (pp.

323-366). Berlin, Germany: Springer.

Name Class

Level

Numeric/figural mode of generalisa- tion

Recursive or explicit approach Validity of generalisation

Natasha 4th Figural Explicit Complete, but flawed nu-

merically

Bella 4th Figural Explicit Complete and valid

Tara 4th Did not respond Did not respond No response

Nikki 4th Numeric Recursive and later explicit Complete and valid

Lisa 6th Numeric Recursive and later explicit Complete and valid

Sarah 6th Figural Did not explain her thinking Complete and valid

Aoife 6th Numeric Recursive Incomplete

Rachel 6th Numeric Recursive Incomplete

Table 1: Classifications of girls’ constructions of a far generalisation

Références

Documents relatifs

For instance, children lower social status attain lower academic achievements [13], while liv- ing with a disability increases the risk of being exposed to bullying at school [2]..

The best accuracy was obtained by using the VLAD approach that has been recently proposed for performing object recognition on a large scale1. Surprisingly, even the BoF approach

Exercice 1 : Quantifier la d´ efinition d’injectivit´ e, en faisant apparaˆıtre

NB : Les d´ emonstrations des th´ eor` emes ou propositions ´ etoil´ es doivent ˆ etre sues. Strat´ egies de

McWhirter, 1998). The procedure we followed in soliciting the drawings  included group discussions and interviews. The girls met with one of the  researchers  as 

During year two, Colleen used the expertise of others to help her to teach visual literacy (for example Sam demonstrated a lesson on examining picture book illustrations) but

To achieve this objective and despite twenty years of research on durability of concrete structures, many obstacles have still to be tackled such as: lack of suitable

we introduce, we always start with an isotropic kernel that depends on a unique range parameter and we finish with an isotropic by group kernel. At each stage we compare different