• Aucun résultat trouvé

Peer Effects, Fast Food Consumption and Adolescent Weight Gain ∗

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Partager "Peer Effects, Fast Food Consumption and Adolescent Weight Gain ∗"

Copied!
39
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Peer Effects, Fast Food Consumption and Adolescent Weight Gain

Bernard Fortin Myra Yazbeck April 2014

Abstract

This paper aims at opening the black box of peer effects in adolescent weight gain. Using Add Health data on secondary schools in the U.S., we investigate whether these effects partly flow through the

eating habitschannel. Adolescents are assumed to interact through a friendship social network. We

first propose a social interaction model of fast food consumption. Our approach allows to control for correlated effects at the network level and to solve the simultaneity (reflection) problem. The model is estimated using maximum likelihood and generalized 2SLS strategies. We also estimate a panel dynamic weight gain production function relating an individual’s Body Mass Index z-score (zBMI) to his fast food consumption and his lagged zBMI. Results show that there are positive but small peer effects in fast food consumption among adolescents belonging to a same friendship school network.

Based on our preferred specification, the estimated social multiplier is 1.15. Our results also suggest that, after one year, an extra day of weekly fast food restaurant visits increases zBMI by 4.71% when ignoring peer effects and by 5.41%, when they are taken into account.

Keywords: Obesity, overweight, peer effects, social interactions, fast food, spatial models.

JEL Codes: C31 I10, I12

We wish to thank Christopher Auld, Charles Bellemare, Luc Bissonnette, Paul Frijters, Guy Lacroix, Paul Makdissi, Kevin Moran, Bruce Shearer and two anonymous referees for useful comments and Yann Bramoull´e, Badi Baltagi, Vincent Boucher, Rokhaya Dieye, Habiba Djeb- bari, Tue Gorgens, Bob Gregory, Linda Khalaf, Lung-Fei Lee, Xin Meng and Rabee Tourky for useful discussions. All remaining errors are ours.

Financial support from the Canada Research Chair in the Economics of Social Policies and Human Resources and le Centre interuniversitaire sur le risque, les politiques ´economiques et l’emploi is gratefully acknowledged.This research uses data from Add Health, a program project directed by Kathleen Mullan Harris and designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and funded by grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 23 other federal agencies and foundations.

CIRP ´EE, IZA, CIRANO and Department of Economics, Universit´e Laval. E-mail: Bernard.Fortin@ecn.ulaval.ca

School of Economics, University of Queensland. Email: m.yazbeck@uq.edu.au

(2)

1 Introduction

For the past few years, obesity has been one of the major concerns of health policy makers in the U.S.

1

It has also been one of the principal sources of increased health care costs. In fact, the increasing trend

2

in children’s and adolescents’ obesity (Ogden et al., 2012) has raised the annual obesity-related medical

3

costs to $147 billion per year. Obesity is also associated with increased risk of reduced life expectancy as

4

well as with serious health problems such as type 2 diabetes (Maggio and Pi-Sunyer, 2003), heart disease

5

(Calabr et al., 2009) and certain cancers (Calle, 2007), making obesity a real public health challenge.

6

Recently, a growing body of the health economics literature has tried to look into the obesity problem

7

from a new perspective using a social interaction framework. An important part of the evidence suggests

8

the presence of peer effects in weight gain. Thus Christakis and Fowler (2007), Trogdon et al. (2008),

9

Renna et al. (2008) and Yakusheva et al. (2014) are pointing to thesocial multiplieras an important element

10

in the obesity epidemics. On the other hand, Cohen-Cole and Fletcher (2008b) found that there is no

11

evidence of peer effects in weight gain. Also, results from a placebo test performed by the same authors

12

(Cohen-Cole and Fletcher, 2008a) indicate that there are peer effects in acne (!) in the Add Health data

13

when one applies the Christakis and Fowler (2007) method discussed later on. As long as it is strictly

14

larger than one, a social multiplier amplifies, at the aggregate level, the impact of any shock that affects

15

obesity at the individual level. This is so because the aggregate effect incorporates, in addition to the

16

sum of the individual direct effects, positive indirect peer effects stemming from social interactions.

17

While the presence of the social multiplier in weight gain has been widely researched, the literature

18

on the mechanisms by which this multiplier flows is still scarce. Indeed, most of the relevant literature

19

attempts to estimate the relationship between variables such as an individual’s Body Mass Index (BMI)

20

and his average peers’ BMI, without exploring the channels at source of this potential linkage.1 The aim

21

1One recent exception is Yakusheva et al. (2011) and Yakusheva et al. (2014) who look at peer effects in weight gain and in weight management behaviours such as eating and physical exercise, using randomly assigned pairs of roommates in freshman year.

(3)

of this paper is to go beyond the black box approach of peer effects in weight gain and try to identify

22

one crucial mechanism through which peer effects in adolescence overweight may flow:eating habits(as

23

proxied by fast food consumption).

24

Three reasons justify our interest in eating habits in analyzing the impact of peer effects on teenage

25

weight gain. First of all, there is important literature that points to eating habits as an important com-

26

ponent in weight gain (e.g., Niemeier et al., 2006; Rosenheck, 2008).2 See also Cutler et al. (2003) which

27

relates the declining relative price of fast food and the increase in fast food restaurant availability over

28

time to increasing obesity in the U.S. Second, one suspects that peer effects in eating habits are likely

29

to be important in adolescence. Indeed, at this age, youngsters have increased independence in general

30

and more freedom as far as their food choices are concerned. Usually vulnerable, they often compare

31

themselves to their friends and may alter their choices to conform to the behaviour of their peers. There-

32

fore, unless we scientifically prove that obesity is a virus, it is counter intuitive to think that one can

33

gain weight by simply interacting with an obese person.3 This is why we are inclined to think that the

34

presence of real peer effects in weight gain can be estimated using behavioural channels such as eat-

35

ing habits. Third, our interest in peer effects in youths’ eating habits is policy driven. There has been

36

much discussion on implementing tax policies to address the problem of obesity (e.g.,Caraher and Cow-

37

burn, 2007; Powell et al., 2013). As long as peer effects in fast food consumption is a source of externality

38

that may stimulate overweight among adolescents, it may be justified to introduce a consumption tax

39

on fast food. The optimal level of this tax will depend, among other things, on the social multiplier of

40

eating habits, and on the causal effect of fast food consumption on adolescent weight.

41

In order to analyze the impact of peer effects in eating habits on weight gain, we propose a two-

42

equation model. The firstlinear-in-meansequation relates an individual’s fast food consumption to his

43

reference group’s mean fast food consumption (endogenous peer effect), his individual characteristics, and

44

2An indirect evidence of the relationship between eating habits and weight gain come from the literature on the (negative) effect of fast food prices on adolescents’ BMI (see Auld and Powell, 2009; Powell, 2009; Powell and Bao, 2009).

3Of course, having obese peers may influence an individual’s tolerance for being obese and therefore his weight manage- ment behaviours.

(4)

his reference group’s mean characteristics (contextual peer effects). This equation also provides an estimate

45

of the social multiplier in fast-food consumption. The second equation is a panel dynamic production

46

function that relates an individual’s BMI adjusted for age (z-score BMI, or zBMI) to his fast food con-

47

sumption, his lagged zBMI and and other control variables. The system of equations thus allows us to

48

evaluate the impact of an eating habits’ exogenous shock on weight gain, when peer effects on fast food

49

consumption are taken into account.

50

Estimating our system of equations raises serious econometric problems. It is well known that the

51

identification of peer effects (first equation) is a challenging task. These identification issues were pointed

52

out by Manski (1993) and discussed among others by Bramoull´e, Djebbari and Fortin (2009) and Blume

53

et al. (2010). On one hand, (endogenous + contextual) peer effects must be identified fromcorrelated(or

54

confounding) factors. For instance, students in a same friendship group may have similar eating habits

55

because they share similar characteristics (i.e.., homophily) or face a common environment (e.g., same

56

school). On the other hand, simultaneity between an adolescent’s and his peers’ behaviour (referred to

57

asthe reflection problemby Manski) may make it difficult to identify separately the endogenous peer effect

58

and the contextual effects. This later task is important since the endogenous peer effect is the only source

59

of a social multiplier.

60

We use a new approach based on Bramoull´e et al. (2009) and Lee et al. (2010), and extended by Blume

61

et al. (2013) to address these identification problems and to estimate the peer effects equation. First, we

62

assume that in their fast food consumption decisions, adolescents interact through afriendship network.

63

Each school is assumed to form a network. School fixed effects are introduced to capture correlated

64

factors associated with network invariant unobserved variables (e.g., similar preferences due to self-

65

selection in schools, same school nutrition policies, distance from fastfood restaurants). The structure of

66

friendship links within a network is allowed to be stochastic and endogenous but is strictly exogenous,

67

conditional on the school fixed effects and observable individual and contextual variables. The possi-

68

bility that friends select each other using unobservable traits that may be correlated with their fast food

69

(5)

consumption decisions is an important issue and is discussed later on.

70

To solve the reflection problem, we exploit results by Bramoull´e et al. (2009) who show that if there

71

are at least two agents who are separated by a link of distance 3 within a network (i.e., there are two

72

adolescents in a school who are not friends but are linked by two friends), both endogenous and con-

73

textual peer effects are identified. Finally, we exploit the similarity between the linear-in-means model

74

and the spatial autoregressive (SAR) model with or without autoregressive spatial errors.4 The model is

75

estimated using a maximum likelihood (ML) approach as in Lee et al. (2010) and Lin (2010). We also es-

76

timate the model with a distribution free approach: generalized spatial two-stage least square (GS-2SLS)

77

proposed in Kelejian and Prucha (1998) and refined in Lee (2003).

78

The estimation of the production function (second equation) also raises serious econometric issues.

79

First, fast food consumption is likely to be an endogenous variable correlated with the individual error

80

term. Moreover, time-invariant unobserved effects potentially correlated with explanatory variables

81

may influence the individual zBMI. Also, the short and the long term impacts of fast food consumption

82

on zBMI may be different, suggesting the introduction of lagged zBMI as an explanatory variable. In

83

order to deal with these problems, we estimate a dynamic AR(1) model using an IV fixed effect approach.

84

Predetermined variables from previous periods are used as excluded instruments.

85

To estimate our model, we use four waves of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health

86

(Add Health).5 We define peers as the nominated group of individuals reported as friends within the

87

same school. The consumption behaviour is depicted through the reported frequency (in days) of fast

88

food restaurant visits in the past week. Results suggest that there is a positive but low endogenous peer

89

effect in fast food consumption among adolescents in general. Based on our preferred specification, the

90

estimated social multiplier is 1.15. Moreover, the production function estimates indicate that there is a

91

positive significant impact of fast food consumption on zBMI. Combining these results, we find that,

92

4Our approach is more general than the SAR model as the latter usually ignores contextual effects and spatial fixed effects.

5Note that for the first equation we use wave II and for the second equation we use all four waves.

(6)

after one year, an extra day of weekly fast food restaurant visits increases zBMI by 4.71% when ignoring

93

peer effects and by 5.41%, when they are taken into account.

94

The remaining parts of this paper will be laid out as follows. Section 2 provides a critical survey of

95

the literature on peer effects in obesity as well as its decomposition into the impact of peer effects on

96

fast food consumption and the impact of fast food consumption on obesity. Section 3 presents our two-

97

equation model and our estimation methods. In section 4, we give a brief overview of the Add Health

98

Survey and we provide descriptive statistics of the data we use. In section 5, we discuss estimation

99

results. Section 6 concludes.

100

2 Previous literature

101

In recent years, a number of studies found strong ”social networks effects” in weight outcomes. In

102

a widely debated article, Christakis and Fowler (2007), using a 32-year panel dataset on adults from

103

Framingham, Massachusetts and based on a logit specification, found that an individual’s probability of

104

becoming obese increased by 57% if he or she had a friend who became obese in a given interval. How-

105

ever, their analysis has been criticized for suffering from a number of limitations (see Cohen-Cole and

106

Fletcher, 2008b; Lyons, 2011; Shalizi and Thomas, 2011).6 In particular, it ignores potential spurious cor-

107

relations between two friends’ BMI resulting from the fact that they are exposed to a same environment.

108

Both Shalizi and Thomas (2011) and Lyons (2011) show that the relying on link asymmetries does not rule

109

out shared environment as it claims. Also, the simultaneity problem between these two outcomes is not

110

directly addressed by allowing the peer’s obesity to be endogenous. Moreover, by introducing lagged

111

obesity variables, it only partly takes into account the problem of selection that may occur as obese in-

112

dividuals may have a higher probability to become friends (for details see pages 217-218 in Shalizi and

113

Thomas, 2011). Finally, by focusing on dyads over time, it introduces an upward bias resulting from the

114

6For a response to these criticisms and others, see Fowler and Christakis (2008), VanderWeele (2011) and Christakis and Fowler (2013).

(7)

unfriendingproblem as defined by Noel and Nyhan (2011). The basic idea behind this argument is that

115

people who share similar characteristics are more likely to maintain social ties (homophily).7

116

Using Add health data, Trogdon et al. (2008) include school fixed effects to account for the fact that

117

students in a same school share a same surrounding. They also estimate their BMI peer model with

118

an instrumental variable approach. They use information on friends’ parents’ obesity and health and

119

friends’ birth weight as instruments for peers’ BMI. They find that an one point increase in peers’ average

120

BMI increases own BMI by 0.52 point. Using a similar approach and based on Add Health dataset, Renna

121

et al. (2008) also find positive peer effects. These effects are significant for females only (= 0.25 point).

122

These analyses raise a number of concerns though. In particular, they assume no contextual variables

123

reflecting peers’ mean characteristics. This rules out the reflection problem by introducing non-tested

124

restriction exclusions. As a result, the peer effect estimates may be inconsistent. Moreover, it is not clear

125

that their instruments is truly exogenous as peers’ parents obesity status or health may be correlated with

126

unobserved variables influencing own BMI. Also, their instruments aread hocas they are not explicitly

127

derived from the reduced form of the model. In our approach, we introduce school fixed effects as well

128

as, for each individual variable, the corresponding contextual variable at the reference group level. We

129

can thus identify both endogenous and contextual peer effects.

130

Using the same dataset, Cohen-Cole and Fletcher (2008b) exploit panel information (wave II in 1996

131

and wave III in 2001) for adolescents for whom at least one of same-sex friend is also observed over

132

time. Compared with Christakis and Fowler’s approach, their analysis introduces time invariant and

133

time dependent environmental variables (at the school level). Friendship selection is controlled for by

134

individual fixed effects. The authors find that peer effects are no longer significant with this specifica-

135

tion. As in Trogdon et al. (2008), their analysis ignores contextual variables, contrary to our approach.

136

Moreover, the friendship network they used in estimations is incomplete, which may underestimate the

137

7Steeg and Galstyan (2011) show that there is a test for ruling out homophily related to the use of longitudinal social networks. The intuition behind the test being that if an individual reproduces the same sequence of events as his friend, it is unlikely that homophily is a source of this replication.

(8)

endogenous peer effect (see Stinebrickner and Stinebrickner, 2006).

138

All the studies discussed up to this point focus on peer effects in weight outcomes without analyzing

139

quantitatively the mechanisms by which they may occur. The general issue addressed in this paper is

140

whether the peer effects in weight gain among adolescents partly flow through theeating habitschannel.

141

This raises in turn two basic issues: a) are there peer effects in fast food consumption ?, and b) is there a

142

link between weight gain (or obesity) and fast food consumption ? In this paper, we address both issues.

143

The literature on peer effects in eating habits (first issue) is recent and quite limited. In a recent natural

144

experiment, Yakusheva et al. (2011) estimate peer effects in explaining weight gain among freshman

145

girls using a similar set up but in school dormitories. Also, they test whether some of the student’s

146

weight management behaviours (i.e., eating habits, physical exercise, use of weight loss supplements)

147

can be predicted by her randomly assigned roommate’s behaviours. Their results provide evidence of

148

the presence ofnegativepeer effects in weight gain. Their results also suggestpositivepeer effects in eating

149

habits, exercise and use of weight loss supplements. In a subsequent paper, Yakusheva, Kapinos and

150

Eisenberg (2014) investigate the presence of peer effects in weight gain exploiting random assignment

151

of roommates during first year of college. The authors find evidence that suggests that peer effects

152

in weight gain are predominantly significant among females. They also try to identify the possible

153

mechanisms through which these effects occur. The authors conclude that eating related disorders and

154

exercise can be excluded as potential mechanisms. However, they could not test whether these effects

155

are mediated through eating habits.

156

Two caveats in Yakusheva et al. (2011) is its focus on girls and its limited sample (e.g., recruited par-

157

ticipants, freshman level students). Moreover the estimates of both studies are likely to underestimate

158

social interactions effects as co-eaters or roommates may not reflect thetruesocial network influencing

159

students’ weight management behaviours (Stinebrickner and Stinebrickner, 2006). Finally, these studies

160

do not estimate the causal links between behaviours and weight gain. Our paper finds its basis in this

161

literature as well as the literature on peer effects and obesity discussed above. However, while works by

162

(9)

Yakusheva et al. (2011) and Yakusheva et al. (2014) rely upon experimental data, we use observational

163

non-experimental data. Thus, peers are not limited to assigned dyads. Rather, they are considered to

164

have social interactions within a school network. This allows for the construction of a social interac-

165

tion matrix that reflects how social interaction between adolescents in schools occurs in a more realistic

166

setting (as in Trogdon et al., 2008; Renna et al., 2008). An additional originality of our paper lies in the

167

fact that it relies upon a linear-in-means approach when relating an adolescent’s behaviour to that of

168

his peers. Also, the analogy between the forms of the linear-in-means model and the spatial autoregres-

169

sive (SAR) model allows us to exploit the particularities of this latter model, in particular the natural

170

instruments that are derived from its structural form.

171

Regarding the second issue, i.e., the relationship between weight gain (or obesity) and fast food

172

consumption, it is an empirical question that is still on the debate table.8 There is no clear evidence in

173

support of a causal link between fast food consumption and obesity. Nevertheless, most of the literature

174

in epidemiology find evidence of a positive correlation between fast food consumption and obesity (see,

175

Anderson et al., 2011; Rosenheck, 2008).

176

The economic literature reveals to be conservative with respect to this question. It focuses the impact

177

of “exposure” to fast food on obesity. Dunn et al. (2012), using an instrumental variable approach,

178

investigates the relationship between fast food availability and obesity. He finds that an increase in

179

the number of fast food restaurants has a positive effect on the BMI among non-whites. Alviola IV

180

et al. (2014) using a similar approach provides evidence that the number of fast food restaurant has a

181

significant impact on school obesity rates. Similarly, Currie et al. (2010) find evidence that proximity to

182

fast food restaurants has a significant effect on obesity for 9th graders. On the other hand, Chen et al.

183

(2009) found a small but statistically significant effect in favour of a relationship between BMI values

184

and the density of fast food restaurants. Finally, Anderson and Matsa (2011), exploiting the placement

185

8The literature on the impact of physical activity on obesity is also inconclusive. For instance, Berentzen et al. (2008) provide evidence that decreased physical activity in adults does not lead to obesity.

(10)

of Interstate Highways in rural areas to obtain exogenous variation in the effective price of restaurants,

186

did not find any causal link between restaurant consumption and obesity.

187

The factors underlying fast food consumption were also investigated. Chou et al. (2005) find a strong

188

positive correlation between exposure to fast food restaurant advertising and the probability that chil-

189

dren and adolescents are overweight. This effect seems to be stronger and more significant for girls

190

(Chou et al., 2005). In fact, this influence can be clearly seen as children are more likely to pick up items

191

that are in “Mac Donald’s” packaging (Robinson et al., 2007). More generally, Cutler et al. (2003) and

192

Bleich et al. (2008) argue that the increased calorie intake (i.e., eating habits) plays a major role in ex-

193

plaining current obesity rates. Importantly, weight gain prior to adulthood set the stage for weight gain

194

in adulthood.

195

While most of the economics literature analyses the relationship between adolescents’ fast food con-

196

sumption and their weight gain using an indirect approach (i.e, effect to fast food exposure), we adopt a

197

direct approach in this paper. More precisely, we estimate a dynamic model of weight gain as a function

198

of fast food consumption and lagged weight gain.

199

3 The econometric model

200

In this section, we first propose a linear-in-means peer effects model of the adolescent’s fast food con-

201

sumption (first equation) and discuss the econometric methods we use to estimate it. We then present

202

our dynamic weight gain production function which relates the adolescent’s zBMI level to his fast food

203

consumption (second equation).

204

(11)

3.1 A model of peer effects in fast food consumption

205

Assume a set of N adolescentsithat are partitioned in a set ofLnetworks. A network is defined as a

206

structure (e.g., school) in which adolescents are potentially tied by a friendship link. Each adolescenti

207

in his network has a set of nominated friendsNiof sizenithat constitute his reference group (or peers).

208

We assume thatiis excluded from his reference group. Since peers are defined as nominated friends,

209

the number of peers will not be the same for every network member. LetGl(l = 1, . . . , L) be the social

210

interaction matrix for a networkl. Its elementglij takes a value of n1

i wheniis friend withj, and zero

211

otherwise. Therefore, theGlmatrix is row normalized (assuming no isolated adolescent, that is, with no

212

group of friends). We defineylias the fast food consumed by adolescentiin networkl,xlirepresents the

213

adolescenti’s observable characteristics, ylthe vector of fast food consumption in network l, andxlis

214

the corresponding vector for individual characteristics. To simplify our presentation, we look at only one

215

characteristic (e.g., adolescent’s mother education).9The correlated effects are captured through network

216

fixed effects (theαl’s). They take into account unobserved factors such as preferences of school, school

217

nutrition policies, or presence of fast food restaurants around the school. Theεli’s are the idiosyncratic

218

error terms. They capture i’s unobservable characteristics that are not invariant within the network.

219

Formally, one can write the linear-in-means model for adolescentias follows:

220

ylil+β P

j∈Niylj

ni +γxli+δ P

j∈Nixlj

nili, (1)

where

P

j∈Niylj

ni and

P

j∈Nixlj

ni are respectively his peers’ mean fast food consumed and characteristics. In

221

the context of our paper,β is theendogenous peer effect. It reflects how the adolescent’s consumption of

222

fast food is affected by his peers’ mean fast food consumption. It is standard to assume that|β|<1. The

223

contextual peer effectis represented by the parameter δ. It captures the impact of his peers’ mean char-

224

acteristic on his fast food consumption. It is important to note that theGl matrix and thexl’ vector are

225

allow to be stochastic but are assumed strictly exogenous conditional onαl, that is,E(εli|xl,Gl, αl) = 0.

226

This assumption is flexible enough to allow for correlation between the network’s unobserved common

227

9Later on, in section 3.1.1, we will generalize the model to account for many characteristics.

(12)

characteristics (e.g., school’s cafeteria quality) and observed characteristics (e.g., mother’s education).

228

Nevertheless, once we condition on these common characteristics, mother’s education is assumed to be

229

independent of the idiosyncratic error terms. Let Il be the identity matrix for a network l andιl the

230

corresponding vector of ones, the model (1) for networklcan be rewritten in matrix notation as follows:

231

yl = αlιl+βGlyl+γxl+δGlxll, for l= 1, ..., L. (2) Note that model (2) is similar to a SAR model (e.g., Cliff and Ord, 1981) generalized to allow for contex-

232

tual and fixed effects (hereinafter referred to as the GSAR model). Since|β|<1,(Il−βGl)is invertible.

233

Therefore, in matrix notation, the reduced form of the model can be written as:

234

yl = αl/(1−β)ιl+ (Il−βGl)−1(γIl+δGl)xl+ (Il−βGl)−1 εl, (3) where we use the result that(I−βGl)−1 = P

k=0βkGkl, so that the vector of intercepts isαl/(1−β)ιl,

235

asuming no isolated adolescents.10

236

Equation (3) allows us to evaluate the impact of a marginal shock in αl (i.e., a common exogenous

237

change in fast food consumption within the network) on an adolescenti’s fast food consumption, when

238

the endogenous peer effect is taken into account. One has∂(E(yli|·)/∂αl = 1/(1−β). This expression

239

is defined as the social multiplier in our model. When β > 0 (strategic complementarities in fast food

240

consumption), the social multiplier is larger than 1. In this case, the impact of the shock is amplified by

241

social interactions due for instance to a conformity or an imitation effect.

242

We then perform a panel-likewithintransformation to the model. More precisely, we average equa-

243

tion (3) over all students in networkland subtract it fromi’s equation. This transformation allows us to

244

address problems that arise from the fact adolescents are sharing the same environment or preferences.

245

LetKl = Il−Hl be the matrix that obtains the deviation from networkl mean withHl=n1

llι0l).The

246

network within transformation will eliminate the correlated effectαl. Pre-multiplying (3) byKl yields

247

10When an adolescent is isolated, his intercept isαl.

(13)

the reduced form of the model for networkl, in deviation:

248

Klyl=Kl(Il−βGl)−1(γIl+δGl)xl+Kl(Il−βGl)−1εl. (4)

3.1.1 Identification

249

Our model raises two basic identification problems.

250

- Simultaneity

251

Simultaneity between individual and peer behaviour (thereflection problem) may prevent separating con-

252

textual effects from endogenous effects. This problem has been analyzed by Bramoull´e et al. (2009) when

253

individuals interact through social networks. Let us defineGthe block-diagonal matrix with theGl’s

254

on its diagonal. Assume first the absence of fixed network effects (i.e., αl = α for alll). In this case,

255

Bramoull´e et al. (2009) show that the structural parameters of the model (2) are identified if the matrices

256

I,G,G2are linearly independent. This condition is satisfied when there are at least two adolescents who

257

are separated by a link of distance 2 within a network. This means that they are not friends but have

258

a common friend (intransitive triad).11The intuition is that this provides exclusion restrictions in the

259

model. More precisely, the friends’ friends mean characteristics can served as instruments for the mean

260

friends’ fast food consumption. Of course, when fixed network effects are allowed, the identification

261

conditions are more restrictive. Bramoull´e et al. (2009) show that, in this case, the structural parameters

262

are identified if the matricesI,G,G2 andG3are linearly independent. This condition is satisfied when

263

at least two adolescents are separated by a link of distance 3 within a network, i.e., we can find two

264

adolescents who are not friends but are linked by two friends. In this case,glij3 >0whilegij2 = gij = 0.

265

Hence, no linear relation of the formG3 = λ0I+λ1G+λ2G2 can exist. This condition holds in most

266

friendship networks and, in particular, in the data we use.12

267

11More generally, the model is identified when individuals do not interact in groups and are not included in their own reference group.

12Identification fails, however, for a number of non trivial networks. This is notably the case forcomplete bipartite networks.

In these graphs, the population of students is divided in two groups such that all students in one group are friends with all students in the other group, and there is no friendship links within groups. These include star networks, where one student, at the centre, is friend with all other students, who are all friends only with him.

(14)

- Correlated effects

268

The presence of confounding unobservable variables affecting fast food consumption and correlated

269

with the explanatory variables raises difficult identification problems. First, since adolescents are not

270

randomly assigned into schools, endogenous self-selection through networks may be the source of po-

271

tentially serious biases in estimating (endogenous + contextual) peer effects. Indeed, if the variables

272

that drive this process of selection are not fully observable, correlations between unobserved network-

273

specific factors and the regressors are potentially important sources of bias. In our approach, we assume

274

that network fixed effects capture these factors. This is consistent with two-step models of link forma-

275

tion. Each adolescent joins a school in a first step, and forms friendship links with others in his school

276

in a second step. In the first step, adolescents self-select into different schools with selection bias due

277

to specific school characteristics. In a second step, link formation takes place within schools randomly

278

or based on observable individual characteristics only. Recall also that network fixed effects take into

279

account common unobservable variables at the school level that may influence fast food consumption

280

(e.g., availability of fast food restaurants).

281

Of course, one limitation of using network fixed effects is that it ignores the possibility that the

282

links formation within a network depend on omitted variables. The matrix G may be endogenous

283

even when controlling for the network fixed effects and observable characteristics. Thus friends may

284

select each other using unobservable traits that may be correlated with fast food consumption (e.g., im-

285

pulsivity, a specific taste for sugar- and fat-rich food,etc). Recently, some researchers (e.g., Hsieh and

286

Lee, 2011; Goldsmith-Pinkham and Imbens, 2013; Liu et al., 2013; Badev, 2013) have made attempts to

287

develop econometric models allowing for the joint estimation of network formation and network interac-

288

tions.13 However, empirical results using Add Health data and focusing on outcomes such as smoking,

289

sleeping behaviour, and scholar performance, do not seem to detect much difference in peer effects when

290

13Of course, strong assumptions are required to identify peer effects in these static models. For instance, the probability of a link between two students is assumed to be independent from the presence of links between other students. Note that strong assumptions are also needed even for dynamic networks (see Shalizi and Thomas, 2011).

(15)

networks are assumed exogenous and when they are allowed to be endogenous.14 One reason may be

291

that this data base includes a very large number of observable characteristics.

292

One specification of our model also allows the error terms to be (first-order) autocorrelated within

293

networks. Therefore its structure becomes analogous to that of a generalized spatial autoregressive

294

model with network autoregressive disturbances (hereinafter referred to as the GSARAR model). This

295

model implies that in addition to the endogenous and contextual effects, some unobserved characteris-

296

tics of the friends are also interdependent. In this case, the error terms in (2) can be written as:

297

εl = ρGlεll, (5)

where the innovations,ξl, are assumed to bei.i.d.(0, σ2Il)and|ρ|<1. Given these assumptions, we can

298

write:

299

εl = (Il−ρGl)−1ξl. (6) Allowing for many characteristics and performing a Cochrane-Orcutt-like transformation on the struc-

300

tural model in deviation, the latter is given by the following structural form:

301

KlMlyl=βKlMlGlyl+KlMlXlγ+KlMlGlXlδ+νl, (7) whereXlis the matrix of adolescents’ characteristics15in thelth network,Ml= (I−ρGl)andνl=Klξl.

302

The elimination of fixed network effects using a withintransformation leads to a singular variance

303

matrix such that E(νlνl0 | Xl,Gl) = KlK0lσ2 = Klσ2. To resolve this problem of linear dependency

304

between observations, we follow a suggestion by Lee et al. (2010) and applied by Lin (2010). Let[Ql Cl]

305

be the orthonormal matrix ofKl, whereQlcorresponds to the eigenvalues of 1 andClto the eigenvalues

306

of 0. The matrixQlhas the following properties:Q0lQl=In

l,QlQ0l =KlandQ0lι= 0, wherenl =nl−1

307

14Note that Badev (2013) obtains that neglecting the endogeneity of the friendship network leads to a downward bias of 10% to 15% in endogenous peer effects regarding adolescent smoking rates.

15Following the linear-in-means model, we allow the peers’ mean characteristic corresponding to each individual’s char- acteristic to have a potential effect on his fast food consumption. Therefore, we do not imposead hoc(identifying) exclusion restrictions to the model. Admittedly, these peers’ characteristics may also act as proxy variables that help to control for the selection effect.

(16)

with nl being the number of adolescents in thelth network. Pre-multiplying (7) by Q0l, the structural

308

model can now be written as follows:

309

Mlyl =βMlGlyl +MlXlγ+MlGlXlδ+νl, (8) whereMl =Q0lMlQl,yl =Q0lyl,Gl =Q0lGlQl,Xl =Q0lXl, andνl =Q0lξl. With this transformation,

310

our problem of dependency between the observations is solved, since we haveE(νlνl∗0|Xl,Gl) =σ2In

l.

311

Following Lee et al. (2010), we propose two approaches to estimate the peer effects model (8): a max-

312

imum likelihood approach (ML) and a generalized spatial two stage least squares (GS-2SLS) approach.

313

The ML approach imposes more structure (normality) than GS-2SLS. Therefore, under some regularity

314

conditions, ML estimators are more asymptotically efficient than GS-2SLS ones when the restrictions it

315

imposes are valid.

316

3.1.2 Maximum Likelihood (ML)

317

Assuming thatνl is anl-dimensional normally distributed disturbance vector, the log-likelihood func-

318

tion is given by:

319

lnL = −n

2 ln (2πσ2) +

L

X

l=1

ln|In

l −βGl| +

L

X

l=1

ln|In

l −ρMl| − 1 2σ2

L

X

l=1

νl∗0νl, (9)

wheren =

L

P

l=1

nl =N−L, and, from (8),νl =Ml(yl − βGlyl − Xlγ − GlXlδ). Maximizing (9)

320

with respect to(β,γ00, ρ, σ)yields the maximum likelihood estimators of the model. Interestingly, the

321

ML method is implemented after the elimination of the network fixed effects. Therefore, the estimators

322

are not subject to the incidental parameters problem that may arise since the number of fixed effects

323

increases with the the size of the networks sample.

324

(17)

3.1.3 Generalized spatial two stage least squares (GS-2SLS)

325

To estimate the model (8), we also adopt a generalized spatial two-stage least squares procedure pre-

326

sented in Lee et al. (2010). This approach provides a simple and tractable numerical method to obtain

327

asymptotically efficient IV estimators within the class of IV estimators. In the case of our paper this

328

method will consist of a two-step estimation.16 To simply the notation, Let X be a block-diagonal

329

matrix withXl on its diagonal, G be a block-diagonal matrix with Gl on its diagonal, and y the

330

concatenated vector of theyl’s over all networks.

331

Now, let us denote by X˜ the matrix of explanatory variables such thatX˜ = [Gy X GX].

LetPbe the weighting matrix such thatP=S(S0S)−1S0, andSa matrix of instruments such thatS = X GX G∗2X

. In the first step, we estimate the following 2SLS estimator:

θˆ1 = (X˜∗0P ˜X)−1∗0Py,

whereθˆ1 is the first-step 2SLS vector of estimated parameters(ˆγ10,δˆ10,βˆ1)of the structural model. This

332

estimator is consistent but not asymptotically efficient within the class of IV estimators.

333

Now, in the second step, we estimate a 2SLS using a new matrix of instrumentsZˆ given by:

Zˆ = [Gˆy X GX],

whereGˆyis computed from the first-step 2SLS reduced form (pre-multiplied byG):

334

Gˆy =G(I−βˆ1G)−1(Xˆγ1+GXδˆ1). (10) We then estimate:

θˆ2 = (Zˆ0)−1Zyˆ .

This estimator can be shown to be consistent and asymptotically best IV estimator. Its asymptotic vari-

335

ance matrix is given byN[Z0R−1Z]−1. The matrixRis consistently estimated byRˆ =s2ˆZN0ˆZ, where

336

16Note that for this particular case we imposeρ= 0and thusMl=Il.

(18)

s2 =N−1PN

i=1i2anduˆi are the residuals from the second step. It is important to note that, as in Kele-

337

jian and Prucha (1998), we assume that errors are homoscedastic. The estimation theory developed by

338

Kelejian and Prucha (1998) under the assumption of homoscedastic errors does not apply if we assume

339

heteroscedastic errors (Kelejian and Prucha, 2010).

340

3.2 A weight gain production function

341

In this section, we propose a dynamic weight gain production function that relates an individual’s zBMI

342

in timetto his lagged zBMI, his lagged fast food consumption as well as his own characteristics in period

343

t. Letybitbe an individual i’s zBMI level at timet, andyfit−1 be the individual’s fast food consumption

344

at timet−1. Then, for a given vector of characteristicsx˜it, the weight gain production function can be

345

formally expressed as follows (for notational simplicity we suppressl):

346

ybit01yi,t−1b2yit−1f3itit, (11)

where

ηitiit,

withµirepresenting the individuali’s time-invariant error component (fixed effect) andζit, his idiosyn-

347

cratic error that may change across t. It is assumed that |π1| < 1 and thatζit is serially uncorrelated

348

(A test of this latter assumption is provided in the empirical section). As discussed earlier, our interest

349

in this production function goes beyond a mere association between fast food consumption and weight

350

gain. We are particularly interested to analyze the magnitude of a change in zBMI resulting from a

351

common exogenous shock on fast food consumption within the network, when peer effects are taken

352

into account. Our two equation model allows us to compute this result. Partially differentiating (11)

353

with respect toyit−1f and using the social multiplier [= 1/(1−β)]yields the magnitude of a short run

354

change in zBMI (i.e., forybi,t−1given) resulting from a common marginal shock on fast food consumption:

355

∂E(yitb|·)/∂αl = 1−βπ2 . This expression entails two components: the impact of the fast food consumption

356

on the zBMI (=π2) and the multiplier effect (= 1−β1 ).

357

(19)

At this point it is important to recall that the first differences OLS estimator of eq.(11) to get rid of

358

the fixed effects is inconsistent becauseybi,t−1 is correlated withζi,t−1 whileyit−1f may also be correlated

359

withζi,t−1. However, an IV variant leads to consistent estimates. Formally, assuming that all character-

360

istics are time-invariant (except age whose coefficient is proportional to the intercept of the transformed

361

model), the first differences model can be written as follows:

362

∆ybit= ˜π01∆ybi,t−12∆yit−1f + ∆ζit, (12)

where∆is the first difference operator. To resolve the problem of correlation between the right hand side

363

variables and the error term one can instrument for∆yi,t−1b and∆yfi,t−1. Following Anderson and Hsiao

364

(1981), we instrument∆yi,t−1b by the lagged first difference variable∆ybi,t−2. To instrument∆yi,t−1f , we

365

use lagged changes in time use variables (work hours, sleep hours, leisure hours), in cigaret consumption

366

and in income, between wave II and wave I. The latter variables are predetermined in the model and

367

therefore are weakly exogenous (this is tested in our empirical section). Moreover they are likely to

368

be correlated with change in fast food consumption between wave III and wave II (also tested in our

369

empirical section).17 The model as laid out in equation (12) is estimated using generalised method of

370

moments (GMM).

371

4 Data and Descriptive Statistics

372

The Add Health survey is a longitudinal study that is nationally representative of American adoles-

373

cents in grades 7 through 12. It is one of the most comprehensive health surveys that contains fairly

374

exhaustive social, economic, psychological and physical well-being variables along with contextual data

375

on the family, neighbourhood, community, school, friendships, peer groups, romantic relationships,etc.

376

In wave I (September 1994 to April 1995), all students (around 90 000) attending the randomly selected

377

high schools were asked to answer a short questionnaire. An in-home sample (core sample) of approx-

378

17As reported earlier, Jeffery and French (1998) provides evidence that leisure time, such as hours of TV viewing, positively influence the BMI of women.

(20)

imately 20 000 students was then randomly drawn from each school. These adolescents were asked

379

to participate in a more extensive questionnaire where detailed questions were asked. Information on

380

(but not limited to) health, nutrition, expectations, parents’ health, parent-adolescent relationship and

381

friends nomination was gathered.18 This cohort was then followed in-home in the subsequent waves in

382

1996 (wave II), 2001 (wave III) and 2008-2009 (wave IV). The extensive questionnaire was also used to

383

construct the saturation sample that focuses on 16 selected schools (about 3000 students). Every student

384

attending these selected schools answered the detailed questionnaire. There are two large schools and

385

14 other small schools. All schools are racially mixed and are located in major metropolitan areas except

386

one large school that has a high concentration of white adolescents and is located in a rural area. Con-

387

sequently, fast food consumption may be subject to downward bias if one accepts the argument that the

388

fast food consumption among white adolescents is usually lower than that of black adolescents.

389

In this paper we use the saturation sample of wave II in-home survey to investigate the presence of

390

peer effects in fast food consumption.19 One of the innovative aspects of this wave is the introduction

391

of the nutrition section. It reports among other things food consumption variables (e.g., fast food, soft

392

drinks, desserts,etc.). This allows us to depict food consumption patterns of each adolescent and relate

393

it to that of his peer group. In addition, the availability of friend nomination allows us to retrace school

394

friends and thus construct friendship networks.To estimate the weight gain production function, we

395

considered information from wave I, wave II, wave III and wave IV.

396

We exploit friends nominations to construct the network of friends. Thus, we consider all nominated

397

friends as network members regardless of the reciprocity of the nomination. If an adolescent nominates

398

a friend then a link is assigned between these two adolescents (directed network with non symmetric

399

links).

400

18Adolescents were asked to nominate up to five female friends and five male friends.

19It includes all meals that are consumed at a fast food restaurant such as McDonald’s, Burger King, Pizza Hut, Taco Bell and other fast food outlets.

(21)

4.1 Descriptive statistics

401

In our peer effects model, the dependent variable of interest is fast food consumption, as approximated

402

by the reported frequency (in days) of fast food restaurant visits in the past 7days. Table 1 reports respec-

403

tively the mean and the standard deviation of the endogenous variable, the covariates used and other

404

relevant characteristics. We note that on average, adolescents’ fast food consumption is fairly within

405

the range of 2.33 times/week. This is consistent with the frequency reported by the Economic Research

406

Service of the United States Department of Agriculture. Around 62% of the adolescents consumed fast

407

food twice or more in the past week and 44% of the adolescents who had consumed fast food did so 3

408

times in the past week.

409

The covariates of the fast food peer effect equation include the adolescent’s personal characteristics,

410

family characteristics as well as the corresponding contextual social effects.The personal characteristics

411

are gender, age, ethnicity (white or other) and grade. We observe that 50% of the sample are females,

412

that the mean age is 16.3 years and that 57% are white. Family characteristics are dummies for mother

413

and father education. We observe that around 45% of mothers and fathers have at least some college

414

education. To control further for parents’ income we use child allowance as a proxy. An adolescent’s

415

allowance is on average 8.28 $ per week, around 50% of the adolescents in our the sample have a weekly

416

allowance. At this point, it is important to highlight that since we use cross section data, we do not

417

have to control for fast food prices as they are taken into account by network fixed effects. As for the

418

weight gain production function, the dependent variable that we use is the variation in the zBMI between

419

waves III and IV. The zBMI variables for each wave, the fast food variables for wave II and III and the

420

instruments are detailed in Table 4.20A large part of changes in the variables between waves is explained

421

by age.

422

20It is important to note that information on fast food consumption was not collected in wave I.

Références

Documents relatifs

In the Central African Republic, in order to simplify this method, the total amount of food entering a Ngbaka village was weighed at characteristic periods of the

Overall, after adjusting for several po- tential confounders (i.e., sex, age, food insecurity, alcohol consumption, smoking, physical activity, obesity, carbonated soft

The genetic correlations of weight gain at different periods of age for males, females, and the combined sexes estimated from the sire, dam, and sire-dam components

The designations employed and the presentation of material in this information product do not imply the expression of any opinion whatsoever on the part of the Food and Agriculture

Here, we studied three European sea bass populations: Atlantic Ocean (AT), West Mediterranean sea (WM) and East Mediterranean sea (EM) at two temperatures (18 and 24°C) in

Echos du Musée d'art et d'histoire 1) L'exposition SCULPTURE BOURGUIGNONNE - FIN DU MOYEN AGE a fermé ses portes après avoir reçu 15 358 visiteurs. Nous considérons ce résultat

The purpose of this study was to identify mutations in one of the genes, PRPF31, in French patients with autosomal dominant RP, to perform genotype-phenotype correlations of

L’accès à ce site Web et l’utilisation de son contenu sont assujettis aux conditions présentées dans le site LISEZ CES CONDITIONS ATTENTIVEMENT AVANT D’UTILISER CE SITE WEB.