• Aucun résultat trouvé

Tablet-based visuo-spatial training tool for preschoolers

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Tablet-based visuo-spatial training tool for preschoolers"

Copied!
1
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Tablet-­‐based  visuo-­‐spa0al  training  tool  for  

preschoolers  

In  the  context  of  numerical  development,  visuo-­‐spa7al   skills  are  assumed  to  provide  an  early  founda7on  for  

mathema7cs  learning  [1,2,3].  Recently,  the  importance   of  these  abili7es  in  preschool  years  has  been  stressed   out  [e.g.  7].  

Nevertheless,  rarely  any  specific  visuo-­‐spa7al  training   tools  are  available  for  the  preschool  seFng.  

We  have  developed  a  tablet-­‐based  visuo-­‐spa7al  

interven7on  tool  for  preschoolers  and  implemented  it   in  5  Luxembourgish  kindergarten  classrooms.    

   

               

INTRODUCTION  

RESULTS  

METHOD  

 Near  transfer  effects  on  trained  visuo-­‐spa7al  abili7es  could  be  observed,   but  no  gains  on  measures  of  mental  transforma7on  skills  and  early  

mathema7cal  abili7es  could  be  observed.    

 Possible  explana7on:  transfer  to  mathema7cal  skills  might  only  occur  when   formal  math  instruc7on  has  begun  as  visuo-­‐spa7al  abili7es  are  thought  to   provide  an  early  founda7on  for  later  mathema7cs  performance.    

DISCUSSION  

BIBLIOGRAPHY  

Véronique  Cornu,  Tahereh  Pazouki  and  Romain  Mar0n  

Luxembourg  Centre  for  Educa7onal  Tes7ng  (LUCET),  University  of  Luxembourg,  Esch-­‐sur-­‐Alze^e  (Luxembourg)  

TRAINING  TOOL  

Training  tasks:  

²  Find  shape  in  a  complex  figure  

²  Tangram  

²  Complete  the  figure  

²  Copying  tasks    

²  Find  the  pair  /  Odd  one  out  

²  Row  comple7on  

²  Close  the  shape  

²  Symmetry  

²  Line  and  figure  bisec7on    

²  Dot  connec7on  

²  Rota7on    

Tablet  workspace  is  

conceptualized  as  an  electronic   blackboard  that  can  be  used  in   combina7on  with  external  

material  such  as  booklets.  

•  Par0cipants  &  Design  

10  kindergarten  classrooms  from  two  schools  in  

Luxembourg  were  recruited.  Half  of  the  classrooms   were  assigned  to  a  “teaching  as  usual”  control  

condi7on,  whereas  the  other  five  classrooms  received   20  sessions  (two  sessions  per  week)  of  visuo-­‐spa7al  

training  (20  minutes  per  session).  A  total  of  N=125  

children  (61  girls)  par7cipated  in  the  study  with  a  mean   age  of  M  =  5.49  years  (SD  =  .63).    

   Interven0on  group  (IG):  n  =  68    Control  group  (CG):  n  =  57  

   

•  Assessment  

 A  specific  assessment  ba^ery  targe7ng  visuo-­‐spa7al,  

symbolic  and  non-­‐symbolic  early  numerical  abili7es  has  been   compiled.  All  tests,  besides  the  non-­‐symbolic  magnitude  

comparison  task,  were  administered  in  paper-­‐pencil  version.  

Tests  were  administered  during  the  three  weeks  before  and   aier  the  interven7on.  

Find  the  pair  

Find  shape  

Pretest  

IG:  tablet-­‐based  

Interven0on  

CG:  teaching  as  

usual  

Pos^est  

Tests  assessing  

“near  transfer”  

• Spa7al  rela7ons   [4]  

• Posi7on  in  space   [4]  

Test  assessing    

“Intermediate   transfer  “  

• Children’s  mental   transforma7on   task  (CMTT)  –  

shortened  12  item   version  [6]  

Tests  assessing    

“Far  transfer”  

• Coun7ng  tasks  

• Number  naming  

• Magnitude   comparison  

(symbolic  &  non-­‐

symbolic)  

• Arithme7c  [5]  

• Number  line  

es7ma7on  [0-­‐20]  

10  weeks  

Significant  near  transfer  effects  could  be  observed  in  the  interven7on     group  compared  to  the  control  group.  

Measures  of  verbal  and  non-­‐verbal  intelligence  were  entered  as   covariates  into  the  model.  

If  we  remove  children  scoring  very  high  on  tests  of  visuo-­‐spa7al  abili7es  at   pretest  (>  4th  quar7le)  from  our  sample,  7me  x  group  interac7on  reaches   sta7s7cal  significance  for  both  measures  (N  =  101;  nIG  =  52,  nCG  =  49).  

No  intermediate  and  no  far  transfer  effects  could  be  observed  at  this   stage..  

0   1   2   3   4   5   6   7  

Pre   Post  

Mean  score  

Spa0al  rela0ons  

IG   CG  

Time  x  group:    

F(1,97)  =  5.0  

p  =  .028*,  ηp2  =  .05  

 

0   1   2   3   4   5   6   7   8  

Pre   Post  

Mean  score  

Posi0on  in  space  

IG   CG  

Time  x  group:  

F(1,97)  =  6.67,    

p  =  .011**,  ηp2  =  .06  

 

0   1   2   3   4   5   6   7  

Pre   Post  

Mean  score  

Spa0al  rela0ons  

IG   CG  

Time  x  group:    

F(1,121)  =  2.34,    

p  =  .13  (n.s.),  ηp2  =  .02  

0   1   2   3   4   5   6   7   8  

Pre   Post  

Mean  Score  

Posi0on  in  space  

IG   CG  

Time  x  group:    

F(1,121)  =  5.45,     p  =  .02*,  ηp2  =  .04  

 

1.  Ansari,  D.,  Donlan,  C.,  Thomas,  M.  S.,  Ewing,  S.  A.,  Peen,  T.,  &  Karmiloff-­‐Smith,  A.  (2003).  What  makes  coun7ng  count?  Verbal  and  visuo-­‐spa7al  contribu7ons  to  typical  and  atypical  number  development.  Journal  of  Experimental  Child  Psychology,  85(1),  50-­‐62.  doi:  10.1016/S0022-­‐0965(03)00026-­‐2   2.  Assel,  M.,  Landry,  S.,  Swank,  P.,  Smith,  K.,  &  Steelman,  L.  (2003).  Precursors  to  mathema7cal  skills:  examining  the  roles  of  visual-­‐spa7al  skills,  execu7ve  processes,  and  paren7ng  factors.  Applied  Developmental  Science,  7(1),  27-­‐38.  doi:  10.1207/S1532480XADS0701_3  

3.  Cheng,  Y.-­‐L.,  &  Mix,  K.S.  (2014).  Spa7al  training  improves  children’s  mathema7cs  ability.  Journal  of  CogniGon  and  Development,  15(1),  2-­‐11.  doi:  10.1080/15248372.2012.725186   4.  Fros7g,  M.  (1973).  Test  de  développement  de  la  percepGon  visuelle  [Developmental  test  of  visual  percepGon].  Paris:  Les  édi7ons  du  centre  de  psychologie  appliquée.  

5.  Grégoire,  J.,  Noël,  M.,  &  Van  Nieuwenhoven,  C.  (2004).  Tedi-­‐Math.  Antwerpen:  Harcourt.  

6.  Levine,  S.  C.,  Hu^enlocher,  J.,  Taylor,  A.,  &  Langrock,  A.  (1999).  Early  sex  differences  in  spa7al  skill.  Developmental  Psychology,  35(4),  940–949.  h^p://doi.org/10.1037/0012-­‐1649.35.4.940  

7.  Verdine,  B.  N.,  Irwin,  C.  M.,  Golinkoff,  R.  M.,  &  Hirsh-­‐Pasek,  K.  (2014).  Contribu7ons  of  execu7ve  func7on  and  spa7al  skills  to  preschool  mathema7cs  achievement.  Journal  of  Experimental  Child  Psychology,  126(0),  37–51.  h^p://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.02.012    

   

Références

Documents relatifs

Virtual Interactive Tablet to Support Vocational Training in Immersive Environment.. Pierre Gac, Paul Richard, Yann Papouin, Sébastien George,

the TCP position cocedinatee that are recorded in an ASCII text file can be used to reduce online programming tim e since these may be downloadable 88 a point array to the actual

4 (a)-(d) Common Mightability (a) Common reachable region in 3D for human and another mobile robot Jido, having one arm, from their current states, (b) common reach- able region

Figure 4 : synthetic patterns for 5 classes used to test the local descriptor and the dissimilarity measure These figures were generated by respecting some constraints to

The solution verification engine is designed in such a way that it is completely isolated from the rest of the system and therefore, it acts as an independent component. This

[r]

For visual object recognition, viewpoint and orientation have very similar connotations; however, the implications for spatial cognition may be different, particularly with respect

In his framework, constructions that describe motion are classified according to whether they express PATH/MANNER components in surface elements: verb roots or satellites.1 Depending