• Aucun résultat trouvé

Construction generalization in children with developmental language disorders

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Construction generalization in children with developmental language disorders"

Copied!
1
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Method

Training phase involving a novel construction with a SOV structure and a meaning of appearance:

- Each sentence is associated with a video - A sequence of 8 stimuli is presented twice

- Two training conditions are created, one with a high variability input (no words in common), one following the progressive alignment principle (some words are common at first and variability is progressively introduced).

Test phase involving novel construction items and transitivity items:

- Each sentence is associated with two videos

- The stimuli were different from the testing phase

- Half of the sentences are SOV structures and the other half are SVO structures

- Children must choose the video which pictures the sentence presented.

Group Age Sentence comprehension Non-verbal IQ

DLD (N=30) 9;11 years old 14 erros 94

TD (N=30) 7;7 years old* 13 errors 99

Introduction

Constructions associate a form with a function, and construction generalization leads children to abstract schemas which can be used productively

(Goldberg, 1995; Schmid, 2017). This process occurs through analogical reasoning, as analogies are performed between utterances to abstract a common

relational structure (Bybee, 2010; Tomasello, 2009).

Children with Developmental Language Disorders (DLD) have difficulty in analogical reasoning and generalization (Hsu & Bishop, 2010; Leroy et al., 2014a).

Analogies and generalization are facilitated by an input involving similarity, but also variability, especially in children with DLD (Casenhiser & Goldberg, 2005; Leroy et al., 2014b; Plante et al., 2014; Wonnacott et al., 2012). These variables are united in progressive alignment, which refers to a progressive introduction of

variability and which has been shown to facilitate analogies in typically-developing (TD) children (Kotovsky & Gentner, 1996).

Are children with DLD impaired when generalizing a novel

construction?

Do children with and without DLD benefit from an input

involving progressive alignment?

? ?

?

Discussion

Children with DLD perform similarly as language-matched peers in a novel construction generalization task.

Progressive alignment (which involves similarity at first and the progressive introduction of variability) facilitates construction generalization in

children with or without DLD.

Progressive alignment led to better results than high variability even for transitivity items, which were not included in the training phase. This means that learning a novel construction through progressive alignment does not disrupt the mastery of known constructions, but quite the reverse.

Progressive alignment could represent an interesting trail in intervention with DLD children in order to improve their generalization ability.

Construction generalization in children with Developmental

Language Disorders

Magali Krzemien, Esther Seret, Christelle Maillart

Department of Speech and Language Therapy, University of Liège, Belgium

Results

DLD children performed similarly as their peers.

Progressive alignment led to better results than high variability.

Total scores were above chance level for the progressive alignment condition, but not for the high variability condition.

Correspondence: Magali KRZEMIEN, Department of Speech and Language Therapy, University of Liège, Rue de l’Aunaie 30, B38, 4000 Liège, BELGIUM, mkrzemien@uliege.be

L’oiseau le jardin tuffe. La fée lonte l’enfant. 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80

Progressive alignment High variability Progressive alignment High variability

TD DLD Per cen tag e of corr ect responses Group

Transitivity items Novel construction items

Références

Documents relatifs

ةتماخ

For multi-family units, there are mandatory requirements in Re sidential Standards (Supplement No. 5 to the National Building Code of Canada) covering party walls and floors

The crucial principle which is the purpose of reading is for pleasure, was cited among the “Top ten principles for Teaching Extensive Reading” in Day and Bamfort

In the early 1980s Sargent’s articles collected in Rational Expectations and Inflation (Sargent, 2013) he used the rational expectations and the Lucas critique argument to

Multiple features were extracted, some due to their previous success in detection of depression [Cummins et al., 2015a] and the overlap of symptoms in free speech between

Index Terms— action recognition, spatio temporal fea- ture, bag of features, dominant point, semi dense point track- ing, trajectory

We defined a set of 4 conservation metrics that aim to demonstrate impacts of conservation to date; identify dependence of species’ survival on conservation actions; quantify

To evaluate the prevalence of EVSP deficit in each clinical group, we computed the number of individuals in this group who performed with a score inferior to the lower outlier limit