• Aucun résultat trouvé

Effects of multiple interventions for reducing vocal stereotypy: Developing a sequential intervention model

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Effects of multiple interventions for reducing vocal stereotypy: Developing a sequential intervention model"

Copied!
17
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Effects

of

multiple

interventions

for

reducing

vocal

stereotypy:

Developing

a

sequential

intervention

model

Marc

J.

Lanovaz

a,

*

,

John

T.

Rapp

b

,

Isabella

Maciw

a

,

E´milie

Pre´gent-Pelletier

a

,

Catherine

Dorion

a

,

Ste´phanie

Ferguson

a

,

Sabine

Saade

a

a

E´coledepsychoe´ducation,Universite´ deMontre´al,C.P.6128,succ.Centre-Ville,Montre´al,QC,H3C3J7Canada

bDepartmentofPsychology,AuburnUniversity,229CaryHall,Auburn,AL36849-5214,UnitedStates

1. Introduction

Individualswithautismspectrumdisordersoftenengageinvariousformsofvocalstereotypy(e.g.,repeatingpreviously heardwords,producingmeaninglesssounds),whichmaybedisruptivetoothersandinterferewithsocialinclusion(Lanovaz & Sladeczek, 2012; MacDonald et al., 2007; Matson,Dempsey, & Fodstad, 2009; Mayes & Calhoun, 2011). Response interruptionandredirection(RIRD;e.g.,Ahearn,Clark,MacDonald,&Chung,2007;Schumacher&Rapp,2011),responsecost (e.g.,Falcomata,Roane,Hovanetz,Kettering,&Keeney,2004;Watkins&Rapp,2014),noncontingentmusic(e.g.,Lanovaz& Sladeczek,2011;Saylor,Sidener,Reeve,Fetherston,&Progar,2012),anddifferentialreinforcementofotherbehavior(DRO;

Rozenblat,Brown,Brown,Reeve,&Reeve,2009;Taylor,Hoch,&Weissman,2005)areexamplesofinterventionsthathave amassedvaryinglevelsofempiricalsupportforthetreatmentofvocalstereotypyintheresearchliterature.Despitethe availabilityofseveralinterventions,fewstudieshavecomparedtwoormoreinterventionstogether(Shabani&Lam,2013).

ARTICLE INFO

Articlehistory:

Received30October2013

Receivedinrevisedform23January2014 Accepted28January2014 Keywords: Differentialreinforcement Interventionmodel Music Prompting Stereotypy ABSTRACT

Despitetheavailabilityofseveralinterventionsdesignedtoreduceengagementinvocal stereotypy,fewstudieshavecomparedtwoormoreinterventionstogether.Consequently, practitionershavelimitedamountofdatatomakeinformeddecisionsonwhetheran interventionmaybemoresuitablethananothertobegintreatingvocalstereotypy.The purposeofthestudywastoaddressthislimitationbyexaminingthedirectandcollateral effectsofmultipleinterventionsin12individualswithautismandotherdevelopmental disabilitiesinordertoguidethedevelopmentofasequentialinterventionmodel.Using single-case experimentaldesigns, we conducted aseries of four experiments which showedthat(a)noncontingentmusicgenerallyproducedmoredesirableoutcomesthan differentialreinforcementofalternativebehavior,(b)differentialreinforcementofother behaviorreducedvocalstereotypyintwoparticipantsforwhomnoncontingentmusichad failedtodoso,(c)theadditionofsimplepromptingproceduresmayenhancetheeffectsof theinterventions,and(d)theeffectsofnoncontingentmusicmaypersistduringsessions withextendeddurations.Basedontheseresults,weproposeasequentialintervention modeltofacilitatetheinitialandsubsequentselectionofaninterventionmostlikelyto reducevocalstereotypywhileproducingdesiredcollateraloutcomes.

ß2014TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierLtd.

* Correspondingauthor.Tel.:+15143436111x81774. E-mailaddress:marc.lanovaz@umontreal.ca(M.J.Lanovaz).

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Research

in

Autism

Spectrum

Disorders

J ou rna l hom e pa ge : h tt p: / / e e s . e l se v i e r . com / R AS D / de f a ul t . a s p

1750-9467ß2014TheAuthors.PublishedbyElsevierLtd.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.01.009

Open access under CC BY license.

(2)

Consequently,practitionershavelimitedamountofdatatomakeinformeddecisionsonwhetheraninterventionmaybe moresuitablethananothertobegintreatingvocalstereotypy.

Inanotableexception,Love,Miguel,Fernand,andLaBrie(2012)comparedtheeffectsofRIRDandnoncontingentaccess totoysthatproduceauditorystimulationonengagementinvocalstereotypyandappropriatevocalizationsintwo school-agedboyswithautism.Theirresultsindicatedthatbothinterventionsreducedvocalstereotypytosimilarlevels,butthat RIRDproducedlargerincreasesinappropriatevocalizations.Oneofthemainstrengthsofthestudywasthattheresearchers measuredtheeffectsoftheinterventiononotherbehavior.Measuringvocalstereotypyalonewouldhaveindicatedthatboth interventionswereequallyeffectivewhereasconsideringtheappropriatevocalizationssuggestedthatRIRDproduceda moredesirable outcome. In somesettings,individualswithdevelopmentaldisabilities maybeexpected toengage in alternativebehaviorotherthanappropriatevocalizations.Forexample,thevocalizationsmaybedisruptivetoothers(e.g., classmates,colleagues)orinterferewithotheralternativebehavior(e.g.,completingatask).Thenagain,otherindividuals maybeunavailabletorespondtotheappropriatevocalizations.PractitionersshouldalsonotethatRIRDoftenrequiresthe ongoingimplementationofapunishmentcontingency(e.g.,Carroll&Kodak,inpress;Cassella,Sidener,Sidener,&Progar, 2011),whichmaybechallengingincertainsettingsorwhenthecontingentdemandsevokeaggressivebehavior.

Two interventions that may be appropriate alternatives in such settings are noncontingent access to music and differential reinforcement of alternative behavior (DRA). Noncontingent music involves playing preferred music continuouslythroughexternalspeakersorheadphones(e.g.,Lanovaz&Sladeczek,2011;Sayloretal.,2012).Themain advantageofnoncontingentmusicisthatitisarguablythesimplestinterventiontoimplementforvocalstereotypy.The practitioneronlyneedstoturnonpreferredmusic,whichallowshertoattendtoothertasksduringthistime.Moreover,the interventionmaynotbedisruptivetootherswhenheadphonesareusedtoprovidethemusic.Whethernoncontingentmusic willinterferewithaperson’sownappropriatebehaviorremainsunclearintheresearchliterature.Burleson,Center,and Reeves(1989)foundthatbackgroundmusicincreasedtaskaccuracyinchildrenwithautism.Inanotherstudy,Lanovaz, Sladeczek,andRapp(2012)reportedmixedresultsonthefunctionalplayoffourchildren:musicincreasedfunctionalplayin one participant,reduced functional play in another, and produced no effecton thesame behavior of theremaining participants.

Asecondconcernisthatplayingnoncontingentmusicmayincreaseengagementinuntargetedformsofmotorstereotypy (Rapp,2005;Rappetal.,2013). Froma clinicalstandpoint,reducing oneformofstereotypywithaninterventionthat increasesa secondformwouldbecounterproductive.Aneffectiveinterventionshouldreduce,oratleastnotincrease, untargetedmotorformsof stereotypy.Finally,researchershavegenerallyassessedtheeffects ofnoncontingentmusic during5-to10-minbriefsessions(e.g.,Rappetal.,2013;Sayloretal.,2012).Resultsofastudyconductedusingitemsthat weremanipulatedbyparticipantsindicatedthattheeffectsofnoncontingentaccessmaynotcontinueduringextended sessionsbecauseindividualsmaystoptoengagewiththeitemsfollowingrepeatedexposure(Lindberg,Iwata,Roscoe, Worsdell,&Hanley,2003).Thatsaid,theeffectsofextendedapplicationofmusicmaydifferbecausetheindividualdoesnot needtoengageinaresponsetoaccesstheauditorystimulation;themusicplaysthroughouttheentiresessionregardlessof theindividual’sbehavior.

AnotherpotentialtreatmentisDRA,whichisoneofthebehavioralinterventionswiththemostempiricalsupportto reduceengagementinstereotypy(DiGennaroReed,Hirst,&Hyman,2012;Rapp&Vollmer,2005).Themainadvantageof DRAisthattheinterventionmaysimultaneouslystrengthenanappropriatebehavior,minimizingtheprobabilitythatitwill bereplacedbyanotherformofstereotypy(Lanovaz,Robertson,Soerono,&Watkins,2013).However,mostpriorstudieshave examinedtheeffectsofDRAonmotorstereotypy.Giventhatengagementinvocalstereotypyisnotnecessarilyincompatible withmanyalternativebehavior(e.g.,playing,completingatask),theeffectsofDRAmaydifferfromthoseobservedwith motorformsofthebehavior.Inarecentexception,Lanovaz,Rapp,andFerguson(2013)foundthatreinforcinganappropriate behaviorassociated withlow levelsof vocalstereotypy (i.e.,sitting) producedreductions in vocalstereotypyfor one participant.Inappliedsettings,thealternativebehaviortargetedforincreasemaynotnecessarilybeassociatedwithlow levels of stereotypy. Assuch, it remains unclear whether strengthening an appropriate behavior, independent of its associationwithlowlevelsofvocalstereotypy,wouldalsoproducedesirableoutcomes.

Basedonthepreviouslimitations,themainpurposeofthestudywastoinvestigatethedirectandcollateraloutcomesof multipleinterventionsinindividualswithautismandotherdevelopmentaldisabilitiesinordertoguidethedevelopmentof asequentialinterventionmodelforvocalstereotypy.WefirstexaminedtheeffectsofnoncontingentmusicandDRAon engagementinvocalstereotypy,motorstereotypy,andappropriatealternativebehavior.Thestudyalsoaimedtoidentify potentialmodificationswhentheinterventionsdidnotreduceengagementinvocalstereotypy,orproducedoneormore undesirablecollateraleffects.Lastly,weexaminedpotentiallimitationsinordertoassistpractitionersinmakinginformed decisionswhenselectinganinterventiontoreduceengagementinvocalstereotypy.

2. Generalmethod

2.1. Participants,datacollection,responsedefinitions,andinterobserveragreement

Twelveindividualswithautismandotherdevelopmentaldisabilitiesparticipatedinoneortwoexperiments.Fourofthe participants(i.e.,David,Eric,FredandGreg)hadbeeninvolvedinotherexperimentsontheassessmentandtreatmentof stereotypyconductedbythefirsttwoauthors(seeLanovaz,Rapp,&Ferguson,2012;Rappetal.,2013).Eachparticipant

(3)

engagedinvocalstereotypyandfiveparticipantsalsoengagedinoneormoreformsofmotorstereotypy.Basedonthe environmentin whichtheinterventionsweretobeimplemented,wealsotargetedoneappropriatebehaviorforeach participantduringthestudy.Table1presentseachparticipant’sage,diagnosis,andresponseforms.Weonlyreportmotor formsofstereotypywhenthemeanpercentageofengagementwasatleast10%duringbaselinesessions.

Trainedresearchassistantsvideotapedeachsessionandsubsequentlyscoredthedurationofeachformofstereotypyand appropriatebehavior.Table2presentsthedefinitionusedtomeasureeachresponseform.Weuseda2-soffsetcriterionto measurevocalstereotypyforeachparticipant.ForJacob,wemeasuredtheproductofhisappropriatebehavior(i.e.,task completion)ratherthanthedurationbycountingthenumberofitemsthathehadtransferredfromonecontainertoanother attheendofthesession.Asecondresearchassistantmeasuredinterobserveragreement(IOA)forapproximately35%of sessionsforeachparticipantusingtheblock-by-blockmethodwith10-sintervals.ThemeanIOAscoresandrangesforeach participantarepresentedinTable1.

Table1

Characteristicsoftheparticipants.

Participant Age Diagnosis ResponseForms IOAscores Nicholas 12 Autism Vocalstereotypy

Pacing On-taskbehavior M=87%(range:83–94%) M=93%(range:82–99%) M=91%(range:83–97%) Zoe 36 ProfoundID DownSyndrome VocalStereotypy Rocking Fingerwiggling Facetouching Objectmanipulation M=91%(range:80–98%) M=93%(range:86–100%) M=87%(range:78–95%) M=90%(range:85–93%) M=93%(range:83–100%) Kyle 4 Autism Vocalstereotypy

On-taskbehavior M=90%(range:83–95%) M=91%(range:83–100%) Morgan 6 GDD Languagedisorder Vocalstereotypy On-taskbehavior M=93%(range:88–99%) M=90%(range:84–96%) Lucas 37 Autism Vocalstereotypy

Magazineviewing

M=90%(range:80–100%) M=93%(range:88–97%) Ryan 7 Autism Vocalstereotypy

Functionalplay

M=88%(range:71–93%) M=97%(range:94–100%) Yasmine 63 ProfoundID Vocalstereotypy

On-taskbehavior

M=74%(range:65–79%) M=96%(range:87–100%) David 6 Autism Vocalstereotypy

Functionalplay

M=96%(range:93–98%) M=93%(range:83–100%) Jacob 5 Autism Vocalstereotypy

Mouthing Objecttapping Taskcompletion M=86%(range:82–91%) M=95%(range:93–100%) M=93%(range:87–98%) N/A

Eric 4 Autism Vocalstereotypy Mouthing Functionalplay

M=95%(range:91–100%) M=98%(range:97–100%) M=76%(range:66–83%) Fred 9 Autism Vocalstereotypy

Objecttapping Functionalplay

M=93%(range:87–98%) M=89%(range:85–92%) M=86%(range:81–91%) Greg 6 Autism Vocalstereotypy

Functionalplay

M=90%(range:86–94%) M=90%(range:82–94%) Notes:GDD,globaldevelopmentaldelay;ID,intellectualdisability;IOA,interobserveragreement.

Table2

Responsedefinitions.

Responseform Definition

Vocalstereotypy Acontextualsoundsorwordsproducedbythevocalapparatus Pacing Walkinginacircularmotion

Bodyrocking Twoormoreforwardandbackwardtorsomovements

Fingermoving Backandforthmotionoffingerswithorwithoutholdinganobject Facetouching Contactbetweenthefingersandfaceorneck

Mouthing Insertionofabodypartornon-edibleobjectpasttheplaceofthemouth Objecttapping Twoormoremovementsofthefingerorhandmakingcontactwithasurface On-taskbehavior Usingtaskmaterialsinamannerconsistentwiththeirintendedfunction Objectmanipulation Holdinganobjectinoneorbothhands

Magazineviewing Lookingatapageofamagazineforatleast3swithoutturningthepageorlookingelsewhere Functionalplay Usingplaymaterialsinamannerconsistentwiththeirintendedfunction

(4)

2.2. Preliminaryassessments

2.2.1. Seriesofno-interactionconditions

Priortothestartofthecurrentstudy,weconductedaseriesof8–21no-interactionconditionstoexaminewhethereach participant’srepetitivevocalizationspersistedintheabsenceofsocialconsequences.Duringeach5-mincondition,the participantshadtheopportunitytoengageinthetargetappropriatebehaviorthatwouldbemeasuredinthesubsequent experiments(e.g., playing, completinga task),but we providedno social consequences. Persistence of the repetitive vocalizationsacrosstheconditionsindicatedthatthebehaviorwasatleastpartlyautomaticallyreinforced(Querimetal., 2013).Weexcludedparticipantswhoserepetitivevocalizationsdidnotpersistacrosstheseriesofno-interactionconditions orthatdidnotoccurforatleast15%ofthetime.Assuch,thevocalstereotypyofallparticipantsinthecurrentstudypersisted duringtheseriesofno-interactionconditions.Thedetailedresultsoftheassessmentarepublishedelsewhereforsome participants(Lanovaz,Rapp,etal.,2012)andavailablefromthefirstauthorfortheothers.

2.2.2. Stimuluspreferenceassessment

Dependingonthetypeofstimuliinvolvedintheirinterventions,eachindividualparticipatedinpreferenceassessments foredibles,music,orboth.Theresearchassistantselectedfivetoeightstimulipresentedduringeachpreferenceassessment incollaborationwiththeindividual’scaregiver.Toassesspreferenceforedibleitems,weusedthepaired-choicestimulus preferenceassessment (Fisheret al.,1992). For music, weconducteda modified paired-choicepreference assessment (Horrocks&Higbee,2008;Lanovaz,Rapp,etal.,2013).Thestimulusselectedthemostoftenduringeachassessmentwas usedasthereinforcerorpreferredstimulusduringtheinterventions.ForJacob,theexperimenterselectedthemusical stimulusincollaborationwiththecaregiverbecausehisresultsindicated thatheselectedsongsregardlessof musical preference(i.e.,basedonthesideofpresentation).

3. Experiment1:directandcollateraleffectsofnoncontingentmusicanddifferentialreinforcementofalternative behavior

3.1. Participants,materials,andsettings

Nicholas,Zoe,Kyle,Morgan,Lucas,Ryan,andYasmineparticipatedinthefirstexperiment.Weconductedthesessionsin settingsinwhichtheparticipantstypicallyengagedintheirappropriatetargetbehavior.Duringtheirsessions,Nicholas, Kyle,andMorgan,hadaccesstomaterialstocompletefinemotoractivities(e.g.,puzzles,beadsandthreads,pushpins, tracing)whereas Yasmine had clothesto fold. We selectedthesetasks because theparticipantscould performthem independentlyandcompletethemwithinthedurationofthesession.Theotherparticipantsengagedinobjectmanipulation orfunctionalplay:Zoehadcontinuousaccesstoitemsthatprovidedsensorystimulation,Lucastomagazines,andRyanto age-appropriatetoys.

3.2. Procedures

Tocomparetheeffectsofthetwointerventions,wealternatedbaseline,noncontingentmusic,andDRAconditionswithin amultielementdesign.WiththeexceptionofYasminewhosesessionswere15mininduration,wemeasuredeachresponse formfor10minduringandfor10minaftertheintervention.Forindividualsengagingintasks,thesessionwasterminatedif theindividualfinishedhisorherseriesoftasksbeforetheendofthe10-minsession.Thepost-interventionsessionswerethe sameasbaseline(seebelow),regardlessoftheprecedingintervention.Wedidnotmeasurepost-interventioneffectsfor Yasminebecausehertaskalreadylasted15minandshehadlimitedavailabilities.

Atthestartofeachbaselinesession,theparticipantswerepromptedtoengageintheirappropriatebehavior(e.g.,the researchassistantsaid,‘‘doyourtask’’or‘‘youcanplaynow’’).Nofurtherconsequenceswereprovidedduringtheentire durationofthesession.Thenoncontingentmusicconditionwassimilartobaselinewiththeexceptionthattheparticipant’s mostpreferredsongplayedcontinuouslyforthedurationofthesessionthroughexternalspeakersinthebackground.During theDRAcondition,weprovidedareinforceronavariable-interval(VI)schedulecontingentonengagementinthetarget appropriatebehavior.Initially,thedurationoftheintervalwas8sforMorgan.ForLucas,wehadinitiallystartedwith15s andchangedto8s,whichallowedustoexaminewhetherthedenserscheduleproducedmoredesirableoutcomes.Our preliminarydataindicatedthatthedenseschedulesmayhaveinterruptedengagementinappropriatebehaviorandbe unpracticaltoimplementinappliedsetting;asaresult,weonlyused15-sintervalsfortheotherparticipants.Weprovided edibleitemsasreinforcersforallparticipantsexceptNicholasbecausehedidnotselectasingleedibleitemduringthe preferenceassessment.Instead,weusedmusicasareinforcer,whichweprovidedona15-sVIschedule.WhenNicholasmet thereinforcementschedulerequirement,weturnedonthemusicuntiltheendoftheongoinginterval.

3.3. Resultsanddiscussion

Foreachexperiment,wepresenttheimmediateeffectsoftheinterventionsforeachparticipantinagraphicalformat. However,thegraphsdepictingthesubsequenteffectswerepresentedonlywhentheinterventionproducedbothimmediate

(5)

andsubsequentchangesinatleastoneresponseform.Otherwise,wereportthemeansandrangesintables.Notethatthe subsequentgraphsthatwerenotincludedinthecurrentpaperareavailablefromthefirstauthor.Table3presentsthemeans andrangesforeachparticipant’sresponseforms.Fig.1showsthepercentageoftimeNicholas(threeupperpanels)andZoe (fivelowerpanels)engagedinstereotypyandappropriatebehaviorduringtheinterventions.ForNicholas,noncontingent musicreducedvocalstereotypyandpacingwhileincreasingon-taskbehaviorwhereasDRAdidnotproduceconsistent effectswhencomparedtobaseline.Incontrast,DRAreducedvocalstereotypy(thoughnottoclinicallysignificantlevels), body rockingand fingermoving,and marginallyincreasedobjectmanipulation for Zoe.Noncontingent music didnot produceclearchangesinherresponseforms.Forbothparticipants,post-interventionlevelsofeachresponseformremained similaracrossconditions(datanotdepicted).

Fig.2showstheresultsoftheanalysesforKyle(fourupperpanels),Morgan(twolowermiddlepanels),andLucas(two lowerpanels).ForKyle,noncontingentmusicreducedimmediateengagementinvocalstereotypyandmaintainedhigher levels of on-taskbehavior than theother conditions.The intervention alsoreduced subsequent engagementin vocal stereotypy,buttheeffectsappearedtofadeovertime.Incontrast,DRAdidnotproducesystematicchangesineithervocal stereotypyoron-taskbehavior.ForMorgan,bothinterventionsdecreasedengagementinvocalstereotypycomparedto

Table3

MeansandrangesforeachparticipantacrossconditionsforExperiment1.

Participants Intervention Post-intervention

Baseline Music DRA Baseline Music DRA Nicholas Vocalstereotypy 51% 21%# 44% 45% 35% 40% (17–87%) (1–45%) (15–75%) (31–66%) (3–72%) (9–63%) Pacing 19% 7%# 11% 7% 9% 6% (2–30%) (0–14%) (0–27%) (0–20%) (0–16%) (0–21%) On-taskbehavior 12% 35%" 9% 21% 30% 29% (2–58%) (3–82%) (0–32%) (4–32%) (0–81%) (0–77%) Zoe Vocalstereotypy 87% 80% 57%# 90% 91% 75% (64–98%) (52–97%) (10–91%) (78–99%) (79–99%) (24–99%) Rocking 65% 73% 18%# 73% 82% 38% (0–95%) (1–98%) (0–66%) (0–99%) (5–100%) (0–90%) Fingerwiggling 60% 47% 14%# 59% 65% 35% (6–93%) (4–89%) (1–46%) (10–94%) (5–94%) (1–86%) Facetouching 15% 17% 31% 26% 16% 35% (4–69%) (3–60%) (7–75%) (4–82%) (3–75%) (5–81%) Objectmanipulation 3% 4% 9%" 1% 1% 2% (1–9%) (0–19%) (3–23%) (0–4%) (0–7%) (0–5%) Kyle Vocalstereotypy 36% 0%# 26% 38% 18%# 37% (10–53%) (0–2%) (15–38%) (15–57%) (4–35%) (21–49%) On-taskbehavior 26% 41%" 19% 22% 30% 15% (0–57%) (17–59%) (1–45%) (0–59%) (0–58%) (1–37%) Morgan Vocalstereotypy 86% 26%# 14%# 88% 83% 87% (79–99%) (4–58%) (7–23%) (76–97%) (35–96%) (57–98%) On-taskbehavior 42% 35% 28% 31% 26% 28% (21–72%) (15–66%) (20–34%) (17–51%) (3–44%) (5–61%) Lucas Vocalstereotypy 31% 8%# 22% 35% 28% 31% (1–60%) (0–38%) (5–38%) (3–68%) (6–56%) (9–54%) Magazineviewing 9% 6% 5%# 10% 12% 10% (3–16%) (0–15%) (1–16%) (0–23%) (4–20%) (1–19%) Ryan Vocalstereotypy 13% 20% 27% 19% 28% 11% (1–29%) (5–44%) (8–60%) (8–33%) (2–52%) (2–36%) Functionalplay 15% 21% 7% 4% 6% 3% (0–34%) (0–40%) (0–32%) (0–10%) (0–14%) (0–10%) Yasmine Vocalstereotypy 35% 28% 43% (28–44%) (22–34%) (30–65%) – – – On-taskbehavior 95% 96% 84% (76–100%) (94–100%) (87–99%)

Notes:DRA:differentialreinforcementofalternativebehavior,":increasecomparedtobaseline(basedonvisualinspectionofmultielementgraph),#: reductioncomparedtobaseline.

(6)

baseline,butDRAproducedalargermeanreduction.Theinterventionsdidnotincreaseengagementinon-taskbehavior. Post-interventionlevelsalsoremainedsimilaracrossconditions(datanotdepicted).NoncontingentmusicreducedLucas’ vocalstereotypyanddidnotinterferewithmagazineviewing.Contrarily,DRAdidnotsystematicallyalterengagementinhis vocalstereotypy,butdecreasedengagementinmagazineviewing.Wedidnotobserve anyconsistentchangesin post-interventionlevelsforbothresponseforms(datanotdepicted).Fig.3showsthatthetwointerventionsfailedtoproduce systematicchangesinstereotypyandappropriatebehaviorforRyanandYasmine.

Atleast one of thetwo interventionsreduced immediate engagementin vocal stereotypy for 5 of7 participants. Noncontingent music reduced immediate engagement in vocal stereotypyin fourparticipants, increasedappropriate behaviorintwoofthem,andalsoreducedcollateralmotorformsofstereotypyinoneparticipant.Ontheotherhand,DRA reduced immediate engagement in vocal stereotypy in two participants, motor stereotypy in one participant, and appropriatebehaviorinoneparticipant.Fortwooftheparticipants,bothinterventionsfailedtoproducedesirableoutcomes, underliningtheimportanceofexaminingotheralternatives.

4. Experiment2:directandcollateraleffectsofdifferentialreinforcementofotherbehavior

ResearchershaveshownthatDROmaybeaneffectiveinterventiontoreduceengagementinvocalstereotypy(e.g.,

Rozenblatetal.,2009;Tayloretal.,2005).Whenthefirstinterventionfailstoreduceengagementinvocalstereotypy,DRO maythusbeasuitablealternativeinasequentialinterventionmodel.Similarlytootherinterventions,theuseofDROis

Fig.1.PercentageoftimeNicholas(threeupperpanels)andZoe(fivelowerpanels)engagedinvocalstereotypy,motorstereotypy,andappropriatebehavior duringbaseline,noncontingentmusic,anddifferentialreinforcementofalternativebehavior(DRA)sessions.

(7)

limitedinsofarasitseffectsonappropriatebehaviorhavenotbeenthoroughlydocumentedbyresearchers.Thepurposeof thesecondexperimentwastoexaminethedirectandcollateraleffectsofimplementingDROtoreduceengagementinvocal stereotypy.

4.1. Participantsandsettings

RyanandYasmineparticipatedinthisexperimentbecausebothnoncontingentmusicandDRAhadfailedtoreducetheir engagementinstereotypyduringthefirstexperiment.WealsoincludedDavidforwhomnoncontingentmusicdidnot reducevocalstereotypyinapreviousstudy(Lanovaz,Rapp,etal.,2012).ForRyanandYasmine,thesettingsandmaterials werethesameasExperiment1.Davidhadnoncontingentaccesstoage-appropriatetoys.

4.2. Procedures

TheexperimentaldesignanddataanalyseswerethesameasinExperiment1withthefollowingexceptions.Weusedan ABdesignforYasminetoreducethenumberofsessionsconductedwithherduetoherlimitedavailabilities.Giventhather resultsshowedthattheinterventionclearlyincreasedengagementinvocalstereotypy,wedidnotconductareturnto baseline.ThebaselineconditionswerethesameasinExperiment1.DuringDRO,theparticipantreceivedaccesstothe

Fig.2.PercentageoftimeKyle(fourupperpanels),Morgan(twolowermiddlepanels),andLucas(twolowerpanels)engagedinvocalstereotypyand appropriatebehaviorduringandfollowingbaseline,noncontingentmusic,anddifferentialreinforcementofalternativebehavior(DRA)sessions.

(8)

reinforcerwhenheorshehadnotengagedinvocalstereotypyduringtheentireinterval.Iftheparticipantengagedinvocal stereotypyatanypointintime,theintervalwasreset.RyanandYasminereceivedediblereinforcerswhereasDavidreceived accesstothemusicforaperiodequivalenttothedurationoftheinterval.ForDavid,westartedwithafixed-duration8-s interval,butthinneditupto30s.Weusedafixed-duration10-sintervalforRyanandYasmine.

4.3. Resultsanddiscussion

Table4presentsthemeansandrangesoftheresponseformsforthethreeparticipants.Fig.4displaysthemultielementgraphs foreachparticipant.ForDavid,DROreducedbothimmediateandsubsequentengagementinvocalstereotypy,butimmediateand subsequentlevelsoffunctionalplayremainedconsistentlylow.ForRyan,DROalsoreducedimmediateengagementinvocal stereotypy,butproducedmarginalpost-interventionincreasesintheresponseform.Wedidnotobservesystematicchangesin functionalplay.ForYasmine,DROneitherreducedvocalstereotypynorincreasedappropriatebehavior,whichwasalreadynear 100%duringbaseline.OurresultssuggestthatDROmayreducevocalstereotypywhenotherinterventionshavefailedtodoso,but thattheinterventiondoesnotnecessarilyevoke,orproducereallocationtoward,appropriatebehavior.

Table4

MeansandrangesforeachparticipantacrossconditionsforExperiment2.

Intervention Post-intervention

Participants Baseline DRO Baseline DRO

David Vocalstereotypy 39% 6%# 47% 32%# (1–60%) (0–38%) (23–61%) (12–45%) Functionalplay 7% 11% 8% 12% (0–23%) (0–76%) (0–46%) (0–96%) Ryan Vocalstereotypy 35% 18%# 19% 31%" (11–68%) (4–46%) (10–49%) (19–85%) Functionalplay 14% 5% 18% 15% (0–41%) (0–15%) (0–67%) (0–45%) Yasmine Vocalstereotypy 35% 57%" (28–44%) (42–74%) – – On-taskbehavior 95% 98% (76–100%) (95–100%)

Notes:DRO:differentialreinforcementofotherbehavior,":increasecomparedtobaseline(basedonvisualinspectionofgraph),#:reductioncomparedto baseline.

Fig.3.PercentageoftimeRyan(twoupperpanels)andYasmine(twolowerpanels)engagedinvocalstereotypyandappropriatebehaviorduringbaseline, noncontingentmusic,anddifferentialreinforcementofalternativebehavior(DRA)sessions.

(9)

5. Experiment3:addingpromptstoincreaseappropriatebehavior

Priorresearchandourpreviousexperimentssuggestthatinterventionsthatreducestereotypymayfailtoproduce desirableeffectsoncollateralbehavior.Forexample,Rappetal.(2013)havefoundthatwhileprovidingnoncontingent accesstoauditorystimulationmayreduceengagementinvocalstereotypy,theinterventionmayalsoincreaseuntargeted motorformsofstereotypyforsomeindividuals.In ourcurrentstudy,wealsoobservedthatreducingvocalandmotor

Fig.4.PercentageoftimeDavid(fourupperpanels),Ryan(fourmiddlepanels),andYasmine(twolowerpanels)engagedinvocalstereotypyand appropriatebehaviorduringandfollowingbaselineanddifferentialreinforcementofotherbehavior(DRO)sessions.

(10)

stereotypydoesnotnecessarilyproduceresponsereallocationtowardappropriatealternativebehavior(e.g.,Zoe,Morgan). Tothisend,oneofthesimplestinterventionstoincreasealternativebehavioristoprovidepromptingwhenthepersonisnot engagingintheappropriatebehavior(e.g.,Britton,Carr,Landaburu,&Romick,2002;Singh&Millichamp,1987).Moreover, someresearchershavefoundthatpromptingalonemayreduceengagementinmotorformsofstereotypy(Symons&Davis, 1994).Thepurposeofthethirdexperimentwastoexaminethedirectandcollateraleffectsofinterventionswithprompts. 5.1. Participantsandsettings

Zoe,Morgan,Jacob,Eric,Fred,andGregparticipatedinthethirdexperiment.ZoeandMorganwereincludedbecauseboth participantswerestillavailablefollowingExperiment1andlevelsofappropriatebehaviorremainedlowdespitereductions instereotypy.Thesettingoftheirinterventionremainedthesame.WeinvitedJacobtoparticipateinthestudybecausewe hadbeeninformedbyhiseducatorthathewasunabletoengageinindependenttasksunlesshewasprompted.Jacob completedasimpletaskoftransferringitemsfromonecontainertoanotherduringthesessions.Finally,ourresultsfrom previousstudiessuggestedthatnoncontingentmusicalonefailedtoincreasefunctionalplay,increasedmotorstereotypy,or bothforEric,Fred,andGreg(Lanovaz,Rapp,etal.,2012;Rappetal.,2013).Thesethreeparticipantshadaccessto age-appropriatetoysduringtheirsessions.Theparticipantswerefamiliarwiththetoys,hadthenecessaryskillstointeractwith them,butrarelydidsoindependently.

5.2. Procedures

Thedesign,procedures,andinterventionsremainedthesameasinExperiments1and2withtheinclusionofafewminor changes.BecauseJacobwasonlyavailableonceperweekandwehadtoconductmanysessionsinthesameday,hissessions lastedonly5minandwedidnotmeasurepost-interventioneffects.HisDRAconsistedofa continuousreinforcement schedule(i.e.,fixedratio1)eachtimehetransferredanitemfromonecontainertoanother.Wealsomadesomechangesto theDRAinterventionforGreg.Heonlyreceivedhisreinforcerifhewasengaginginfunctionalplaywhentheintervalended (andnotforthefirstoccurrencefollowingtheendoftheintervalasinatypicalintervalschedule)anditsdurationwasfixed. Thischangewastofacilitatethesubsequentimplementationbyhisparent.ForZoe,weonlyassessedDRAwithpromptingas DRAwastheinterventionthathadproducedthemostdesirableoutcomesonstereotypyinExperiment1.SimilarlyforEric and Fred, we focused exclusively on noncontingent music with prompting as previous studies had shown that the interventionfailedtoproducedesirablecollateraleffectsfortheseparticipants(Lanovaz,Rapp,etal.,2012;Rappetal.,2013). Finally,wecombinednoncontingentmusicwithDRAforGreginordertoexaminetheuniquecontributionofthelatter. Forallparticipants,weaddedapromptingprocedureacrossallconditions(i.e.,baseline,noncontingentmusic,andDRA). ForJacob,Eric,Fred,andGreg,thepromptingprocedureinvolvedprovidingaphysicalpromptevery15sifthechildwasnot engagingintheappropriatebehavior.ForMorgan,weimplementedaleast-to-mostpromptingproceduretoengagein on-taskbehaviorcontingentontheoccurrenceoftargeteddisruptivebehavior(i.e.,playingwithmaterials,standingup,and rockingthechair)becauseourobservationssuggestedthatthesebehaviorsinterferedwithtaskengagement.Duringthe least-to-mostpromptingsequence,theresearchassistantbeganwithaverbalprompt.Iftheparticipantdidnotcomplywith theverbalpromptwithin5s,theresearchassistantaddedagesturalprompt.IfMorganstilldidnotcomplywiththeverbal plusgesturalprompt,theresearchassistantsubsequentlyaddedaphysicalprompt.Finally,theresearchassistantsnoted thatZoeengagedinhigherlevelsofengagementwhenhereducatorplacedaniteminherhands.Thus,theprompting procedureinvolvedgivingheranitemthatprovidedsensorystimulationassoonasshehadnotmanipulatedanitemfor2s. 5.3. Resultsanddiscussion

Table5displaysthemeansandrangesforeachparticipantacrossconditions.Fig.5showstheimmediate(upperfivepanels) andsubsequenteffects(lowerfivepanels)ofDRAforZoe.ResultssuggestthatcombiningDRAwithpromptingcontinuedto produceimmediatereductionsin vocalstereotypy,bodyrocking,and fingermoving,but alsomarginallyincreasedface touching.LevelsofobjectmanipulationremainedsimilaracrossthepromptingandDRAwithpromptingconditions,butwere considerablyhigher thanlevels observed in Experiment 1. Moreover, DRAwith prompting respectively decreasedand increasedsubsequentengagementinfingermovingandfacetouching.Fig.6showstheimmediateandsubsequentresultsof implementingpromptingwithnoncontingentmusicandDRAforMorgan(upperfourpanels)andJacob(lowerfourpanels). Comparedwithpromptingalone,DRAwithpromptingreducedimmediateandsubsequentengagementinvocalstereotypyfor Morgan,butalsoreducedimmediateengagementinon-taskbehavior.Theonlyconsistenteffectofnoncontingentmusicwith promptingwastoreduceimmediateengagementinvocalstereotypy,albeittoalesserextentthanDRA.ForJacob,combining DRAwith prompting reduced engagement in vocal stereotypy and mouthingas well as the rate of task completion. Noncontingentmusicalsoreducedvocalstereotypy,butmeanlevelsremainedhigherthanforDRA.

Fig.7showsthepercentageoftimeEric,Fred,andGregengagedinstereotypyandappropriatebehaviorduringthe interventionsthatinvolvedprompting.ForEric,noncontingentmusicwithpromptingreducedvocalstereotypy,produced noconsistentchangesinmouthing,andincreasedfunctionalplay. ForFred,noncontingentmusicwithpromptingonly producedreductionsinvocalstereotypy.ForGreg,DRAwithpromptingreducedengagementinvocalstereotypy,butthe additionofnoncontingentmusicproducedevenlargerreductions.Nevertheless,thesereductionsdidnotappeartoproduce

(11)

reallocationtowardfunctionalplay.Wedidnotobservesystematicpost-interventionchangesacrossconditionsforanyof thethreeparticipants(datanotdepicted).Theresultssuggestthatpromptingdidnotinterferewiththeeffectivenessofthe interventionsinreducingstereotypy.Furthermore,noncontingentmusicincreasedfunctionalplayinoneparticipantand didnotappeartointerferewithengagementinappropriatebehaviorfortheothers.

6. Experiment4:effectsofextendedexposure

Ourpreviousexperimentsindicatethatusingnoncontingentmusicaloneorincombinationwithpromptingmaybea suitablefirstinterventionforasequentialinterventionmodel.Inadditiontoreducingengagementinvocalstereotypy,the interventionneverincreasedmotorformsofstereotypy,norinterferedwithengagementinappropriatebehavior.Moreover, noncontingentmusicsometimesincreasedengagementinappropriatebehavior.Giventhatweassessedtheeffectsofmusic

Table5

MeansandrangesforeachparticipantacrossconditionsforExperiment3.

Intervention Post-intervention Participants Baseline Musica

DRA Baseline Musica

DRA Zoe Vocalstereotypy 97% 63%# 98% 88% (92–100%) (28–93%) (96–100%) (62–98%) Rocking 90% 34%# 90% 71% (77–98%) (1–80%) (84–96%) (23–97%) Fingerwiggling 65% 20%# 66% 43%# (51–81%) (1–59%) (39–80%) (2–72%) Facetouching 7% 12%" 7% 15%" (5–12%) (7–25%) (3–11%) (5–39%) Objectmanipulation 25% 25% 23% 29% (17–35%) (17–34%) (12–35%) (18–41%) Morgan Vocalstereotypy 87% 28%# 9%# 92% 80% 80%# (69–98%) (3–59%) (1–26%) (85–97%) (48–95%) (71–91%) On-taskbehavior 69% 63% 37%# 67% 68% 56%# (62–77%) (58–72%) (29–47%) (59–84%) (48–84%) (21–66%) Jacob Vocalstereotypy 45% 31%# 23%# (35–57%) (13–49%) (5–46%) – – – Mouthing 35% 49% 0%# (16–59%) (27–73%) (0–2%) Objecttapping 15% 13% 10% (6–38%) (5–28%) (3–16%) Taskrate(permin) 8 10 5#

(5–12) (5–18) (4–8) Eric Vocalstereotypy 10% 1%# – 17% 8% (0–17%) (0–4%) (4–40%) (3–17%) Mouthing 13% 7% 20% 12% – (0–72%) (0–28%) (0–40%) (0–50%) Functionalplay 21% 39%" 19% 22% (9–38%) (13–73%) (6–29%) (9–51%) Fred Vocalstereotypy 55% 21%# 41% 51% (9–84%) (5–62%) (22–54%) (15–76%) Objecttapping 15% 12% – 11% 12% – (4–21%) (11–17%) (2–17%) (6–25%) Functionalplay 23% 28% 26% 32% (16–36%) (14–51%) (16–40%) (16–44%) Greg Vocalstereotypy 47% 5%# 28%# 47% 49% 42% (28–65%) (1–11%) (14–46%) (31–59%) (40–65%) (25–53%) Functionalplay 33% 38% 49% 9% 13% 6% (10–66%) (30–52%) (30–77%) (0–26%) (2–61%) (1–11%) Notes:DRA:differentialreinforcementofalternativebehavior,":increasecomparedtobaseline(basedonvisualinspectionofmultielementgraph),#: reductioncomparedtobaseline.

(12)

onlyduringbrief10-minsessions,weexaminedwhetherthepositiveoutcomewouldpersistforsessionsoflongerduration toextendthepotentialutilityoftheresultsofthecurrentstudy.

6.1. Participantsandprocedures

WeinvitedEricandFredtoparticipateinthefourthexperiment.Ericparticipatedinthree90-minsessionsandFredin four50-to60-minsessions.SessiondurationswereshorterforFredbecauseheengagedinelopementwhenhestayedinthe sameroomforextendedperiodsoftime.Bothparticipantshadaccesstothesameage-appropriatetoysasduringExperiment 3andweprovidednosocialconsequencesforengaginginvocalstereotypyorfunctionalplay.Duringtheseperiods,the participantswerefreetoplaywiththetoysandmovearoundtheroom.Ericinitiallyparticipatedintwo90-minsessions

Fig.5.PercentageoftimeZoeengagedinvocalstereotypy,motorstereotypy,andobjectmanipulationduring(fiveupperpanels)andfollowing(fivelower panels)promptingaloneanddifferentialreinforcementofalternativebehavior(DRA)withpromptingsessions.

(13)

duringwhichhispreferredsongplayedcontinuouslyinaloopandtheninone90-minsessionduringwhichthechildren’s songsvaried.Fredparticipatedinabriefreversal:thefirsttwosessionsinvolvedthesamepreferredsongplayinginaloop, thethirdsessiondidnotincludemusic,andthelastsessionreturnedtothesamepreferredsonginaloop.

6.2. Resultsanddiscussion

Fig.8presentsthepercentageoftimeEricandFredengagedinvocalstereotypyduring50-to90-minsessions.Thedata weredividedin10-minintervalstofacilitatecomparisonswiththeotherexperimentsandalsotoexaminetrendsoverthe extendedsessions.Eric(upperpanel)maintainedlowandstablelevelsofvocalstereotypy(M=10%)duringthefirst90-min sessionwiththesamesongplayinginaloop(i.e.,constantmusic),butlevelsincreasedfollowing30minintothesecond 90-minsession(M=33%).Whenweintroducedvariedmusic,engagementinvocalstereotypyreturnedtolowlevelsduringthe entire90-minsession(M=7%).ThefirstsessionforFred(lowerpanel)onlylasted50minbecausehemadeseveralattempts toleavetheroom.Levelsofvocalstereotypywhenhispreferredsongplayedcontinuouslyinaloopweregenerallylow,but showedaslightincreasingtrendacrossthelast30minofthesession(M=8%).Duringthesecondsession,Fredalsodisplayed generallylowlevelsofvocalstereotypy(M=12%).Thewithdrawalofmusicforanentire60-minsessionproducedincreases

Fig.6.PercentageoftimeandrateMorgan(fourupperpanels)andJacob(fourlowerpanels)engagedinvocalstereotypy,motorstereotypy,andappropriate behaviorduringandfollowingpromptingalone,noncontingentmusicwithprompting,anddifferentialreinforcementofalternativebehavior(DRA)with promptingsessions.

(14)

inengagementinvocalstereotypy(M=63%)whencomparedtotheconstantmusicsessions.Thereintroductionofthe preferredsongforafinal60-minsessionclearlyreplicatedthereductionsinvocalstereotypy(M=8%)observedinthetwo initialsessions.Theresultssuggestthattheeffectsofmusicmaycontinueacrosslongsessiondurations,butthatitmaybe importanttovarythesongforsomeindividuals.

7. Generaldiscussion

Insum,thefirstexperimentindicatedthatnoncontingentmusicgenerallyproducedbetteroutcomesthanDRA.The subsequentimplementationofDROinthesecondexperimentreducedstereotypyintwoofthreeparticipantsforwhom otherinterventionsdidnot.Inthethirdexperiment,theadditionofpromptsdidnotinterferewiththereductionsinvocal stereotypy produced by theinterventions while maintainingmotor forms of stereotypy and appropriate behavior at desirablelevels.Finally,thefourthexperimentshowedthattheeffectsofnoncontingentmusicmaypersistduringsessions withextendeddurations.

Overall, at least one intervention reduced engagement in vocal stereotypy for 11 of 12 participants. Specifically, noncontingentmusicaloneorwithpromptsreducedimmediateengagementinvocalstereotypyin8of11participantswith whomtheinterventionwasimplemented.Whennoncontingentmusicreducedimmediateengagementinvocalstereotypy, we alsoobserved immediate increases in appropriate behavior in three participants, immediate reductions in motor stereotypyinoneparticipant,andsubsequentdecreasesinvocalstereotypyinoneparticipant.Incontrast,DRAaloneorin combination with promptsreduced vocal stereotypy in fourof nine participants.Furthermore, theintervention was associatedwithreductionsinmotorstereotypyintwoparticipants.Theinterventionalsoreducedappropriatebehaviorin two participantsfor whomthe intervention had reduced immediateengagement in vocalstereotypy and marginally increasedappropriatebehaviorinonlyoneparticipant.

Fig.7.PercentageoftimeEric(threeupperpanels)andFred(threemiddlepanels)engagedinstereotypyandfunctionalplayduringpromptingaloneand noncontingentmusicwithpromptingsessions.PercentageoftimeGreg(twolowerpanels)engagedinvocalstereotypyandfunctionalplayduring promptingalone,differentialreinforcementofalternativebehavior(DRA)withprompting,andDRAplusnoncontingentmusicwithpromptingsessions.

(15)

The mainimplication of theresultsis that noncontingentmusicmay bemoreeffective thanDRAtoreduce vocal stereotypy. Notably,noncontingentmusic reduced vocalstereotypyfor 73% of participantswhereasDRAreduced the behaviorfor45%ofparticipants,suggestingthatitmaybepreferabletobeginthesequentialinterventionmodelusing noncontingentmusic.Thecollateraleffectsofnoncontingentmusicwerealsoclinicallydesirable:theinterventionincreased appropriatebehaviorinthreeparticipants,neverinterferedwithappropriatebehaviorintheremainingparticipants,and never increasedimmediate or subsequent motor stereotypy. Noncontingent music alsoreduced vocal stereotypyfor sessionswithextendeddurations,butitwasnecessarytovarythesongforoneparticipant.Ourresultsalsosuggestthat practitioners shouldconsidersupplementing noncontingentmusicwithprompts,which mayhinderengagementwith motorformsofstereotypywhilestrengtheningorincreasingengagementinappropriatebehavior.

Whennoncontingentmusicfailstoreducestereotypy,DROappearstobeamoreadequatealternativethanDRAasit reducedstereotypyinalargerproportionofparticipants.Thatsaid,addingpromptsmayalsobeimportantforDROasthe interventiondidnotincreaseappropriatebehaviorifitwasnotalreadyoccurringathighlevelsduringbaselinesessions.For oneparticipant(Yasmine),DROincreasedengagementinvocalstereotypy.Herresultssuggestthatanotherintervention shouldbeplannedaspartofthesequentialinterventionmodelwhenbothpreviousinterventionsdonotproducethedesired outcomes.TheresearchliteraturesuggeststhatresponsecostmaybeasuitablecandidatetofollowDRO(Falcomataetal., 2004;Watkins&Rapp,2014),butstudiesshouldbeconductedtoconfirmthishypothesis.Ouranalysesthussuggestthata potentiallyefficientsequentialinterventionmodelwouldinvolvetheimplementationofnoncontingentmusicfirst,DRO second,andresponsecostlastifbothpreviousinterventionsfailedtoproducedesirableeffects.

Unexpectedly,DRAincreasedthetargetappropriatebehaviorforonlyoneofnineparticipants.Onepotentialexplanation isthattheschedulesofreinforcementwerenotdenseenoughtoincreasetheappropriatebehavior.Thatsaid,wedidnot observe more desirable outcomesin the participantswith denser schedules and any added benefitsof using denser scheduleswouldprobablyhavebeenoffsetbythechallengesassociatedwiththeirimplementationinappliedsettings. AnotherexplanationisthattheselectionoftheappropriatebehaviormayhavecontributedtohoweffectivetheDRAwasat strengtheningcollateralbehavior.Thatis,usingdifferentalternativebehaviormayhaveproduceddifferentresultsand shouldbeinvestigatedinthefuture.ThereductionsobservedinappropriatebehaviorduringDRAprovidefurthersupportfor researchsuggestingthatdenseschedulesusingediblesmaydisruptengagementinotherbehavior(Frank-Crawfordetal., 2012). Namely,thetimespentconsumingediblesmayconsiderablyreducetheamountoftimeavailable toengagein appropriatebehavior,whichwouldexplaintheresults.

Altogether,thefourexperimentsextendtheresearchliteratureonthetreatmentofvocalstereotypyinseveralways.First, ourstudyisthefirsttosystematicallycomparetheeffectsofnoncontingentmusicandDRAonvocalstereotypy,motor stereotypy,andappropriatebehavior.Comparingtreatmentstogetherisimportantaspractitionersrelyontheseresults whenselectingbehavioralinterventionstoimplementinappliedsettings(Shabani&Lam,2013).Second,ourresultsextend previous research by showingthat noncontingentmusic never interfered withongoing appropriate behavior. On the contrary,noncontingentmusicwasevenassociatedwithincreasesinappropriatebehaviorforsomeparticipants,afinding thatisconsistentwithatleastonepriorstudy(Burlesonetal.,1989).Hence,ourresultsminimizeaclinicalconcernthat

Fig.8.PercentageoftimeEric(upperpanel)andFred(lowerpanel)engagedinvocalstereotypyacross10-minintervalsduring50-to90-minsessionswith nomusic,constantmusic,andvariedmusic.Thearrowsidentifythefirst10-minintervalforeachextendedsession.

(16)

noncontingentmusicmayinterferewithotherimportantbehavior(Lanovaz&Sladeczek,2012).Theobservedchangesin vocalstereotypyarealsoconsistentwiththose ofpriorstudieswhich showedthat musicreducedengagementinthe behavior(e.g.,Lanovaz&Sladeczek,2011;Sayloretal.,2012).

Third,ourresultsreplicatethefindingsofotherstudiesthathaveusedDROandhaveshownthattheschedulemaybe thinnedovertimetomakeiteasiertoimplementinappliedsettings(Rozenblatetal.,2009;Tayloretal.,2005).Fourth,the thirdexperimentindicatesthataddingpromptsdoesnotinterferewiththeeffectivenessofotherbehavioralinterventions andmayevenenhancetheireffects.Forexample,wehadshowninapreviousstudythatnoncontingentmusicincreased motorstereotypyforEricandGreg(Rappetal.,2013).Bycontrast,suchincreasesinmotorstereotypyinthepresenceof promptswerenotobservedinthisstudy.Finally,thefourthexperimentreplicatedandextendedthestudywithlonger sessiondurationsconductedbyLindbergetal.(2003)usingnoncontingentreinforcementwithtangibleitems.Weshowed thattheeffectsofnoncontingentmusicmaycontinueduringextendedapplicationandthatvaryingmusicmaybeeffective whenapreferredsongnolongerreducesengagementinvocalstereotypy.

Somelimitationsshouldbeconsideredwheninterpretingtheresultsofthecurrentstudy.Theresultsofthecomparison betweennoncontingentmusicandDRAislimitedinsofaraswechosetouseediblesratherthanmusicasreinforcersduring DRAforallbutoneparticipantbecausetheformeraremorepracticaltodeliverinappliedsettings.Thedifferentialeffects mayhavethusbeentheresultsofthedifferentstimuli.TheclinicalrelevanceofusingDRAwithmusicwouldbelimitedgiven the complexity of its implementation. We thus preferred comparing two interventions which could be realistically implementedbyeducatorsandparentsinappliedsettings.Similarly,weselectedthedensestschedulesofreinforcement thatcouldbepracticallyappliedintheparticipants’environments.Selectingdenserschedulesmayhaveproducedmore desirableeffects,butwouldhavebeenchallengingtoimplementforcaregivers.Inthethirdexperiment,wedidnotconducta no-promptingbaseline,whichlimited theanalysisoftheuniquecontributionofprompting.Althougha comparisonof resultsacrossexperimentsandstudiessuggestthataddingpromptshadbeneficialeffects,thelackofanexperimentaldesign precludesdefiniteconclusions.Tominimizeconfoundingeffectsassociatedwithwearingheadphones,weplayedthemusic throughexternalspeakers,whichmaybedisruptivetoothers.Inclinicalpractice,wewouldrecommendthattheindividual wearsheadphonesinstead(Sayloretal.,2012).

Futureresearchshouldreplicateourstudybyevaluatingtheeffectsoftheproposedsequentialinterventionmodelwitha groupofparticipants.Examiningtheuniquecontributionofprompting onstereotypy andappropriate behaviorwhen noncontingentmusicorDROproducesundesirablecollateraleffectsmayalsoextendresearchwhilepotentiallyimproving treatment.Researchersshouldalsoconsiderconductingstudiesinwhicheducatorsandcaregiversapplytheproceduresand measuringsocialvalidity. Largerscale studiescomparing thecosteffectiveness aswellastheeffects ofinterventions designedtoreducevocalstereotypywhenappliedbyindividualswhoarenottrainedinbehavioranalysismayalsobecrucial inthelongterm.Intheend,programsthatfacilitatetheselectionandimplementationofinterventionsbypractitionerswith differenttrainingbackgroundsmayproducethelargestimpactonthetreatmentstereotypyinindividualswithautismand otherdevelopmentaldisabilities.

Acknowledgments

ThestudywassupportedinpartbyanexperimentationgrantfromtheOfficedespersonneshandicape´esduQue´bec (#2361-09-51).WethanktheCentredere´adaptationdel’OuestdeMontre´alfortheircollaborationaswellasJulieDuquette, FannyJuneauandCyrielL’Hommefortheirassistancewithconductingthestudy.

References

Ahearn,W.H.,Clark,K.M.,MacDonald,R.P.,&Chung,B.I.(2007).Assessingandtreatingvocalstereotypyinchildrenwithautism.JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysis,40,263–275.

Britton,L.N.,Carr,J.E.,Landaburu,H.J.,&Romick,K.S.(2002).Theefficacyofnoncontingentreinforcementastreatmentforautomaticallyreinforcedstereotypy. BehavioralInterventions,17,93–103.

Burleson,S.J.,Center,D.B.,&Reeves,H.(1989).Theeffectofbackgroundmusicontaskperformanceinpsychoticchildren.JournalofMusicTherapy,26,198–205.

Carroll,R.A.,&Kodak,T.K.(inpress).Anevaluationofinterruptedanduninterruptedmeasurementofvocalstereotypyonperceivedtreatmentoutcomes.Journal ofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis.(inpress).

Cassella,M.D.,Sidener,T.M.,Sidener,D.W.,&Progar,P.R.(2011).Responseinterruptionandredirectionforvocalstereotypyinchildrenwithautism:A systematicreplication.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,44,169–173.

DiGennaroReed,F.D.,Hirst,J.M.,&Hyman,S.R.(2012).Assessmentandtreatmentofstereotypicbehaviorinchildrenwithautismandotherdevelopmental disabilities:Athirtyyearreview.ResearchinAutismSpectrumDisorders,6,422–430.

Falcomata,T.S.,Roane,H.S.,Hovanetz,A.N.,Kettering,T.L.,&Keeney,K.M.(2004).Anevaluationofresponsecostinthetreatmentofinappropriatevocalizations maintainedbyautomaticreinforcement.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,37,83–87.

Fisher,W.,Piazza,C.C.,Bowman,L.G.,Hagopian,L.P.,Owens,J.C.,&Slevin,I.(1992).Acomparisonoftwoapproachesforidentifyingreinforcersforpersonswith severeandprofounddisabilities.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,25,491–498.

Frank-Crawford,M.A.,Borrero,J.C.,Nguyen,L.,Leon-Enriquez,Y.,Carreau-Webster,A.B.,&DeLeon,I.G.(2012).Disruptiveeffectsofcontingentfoodon high-probabilitybehavior.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,45,143–148.

Horrocks,E.,&Higbee,T.S.(2008).Anevaluationofastimuluspreferenceassessmentofauditorystimuliforadolescentswithdevelopmentaldisabilities.Research inDevelopmentalDisabilities,29,11–20.

Lanovaz,M.J.,Rapp,J.T.,&Ferguson,S.(2012a).Theutilityofassessingmusicalpreferencebeforeimplementationofnoncontingentmusictoreducevocal stereotypy.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,45,845–851.

Lanovaz,M.J.,Rapp,J.T.,&Ferguson,S.(2013a).Assessmentandtreatmentofvocalstereotypyassociatedwithtelevision:Apilotstudy.JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysis,42,544–548.

(17)

Lanovaz,M.J.,Robertson,K.,Soerono,K.,&Watkins,N.(2013).Effectsofreducingstereotypyonotherbehaviors:Asystematicreview.ResearchinAutismSpectrum Disorders,7,1234–1243.

Lanovaz,M.J.,&Sladeczek,I.E.(2011).Vocalstereotypyinchildrenwithautism:Structuralcharacteristics,variability,andeffectsofauditorystimulation. ResearchinAutismSpectrumDisorders,5,1159–1168.

Lanovaz,M.J.,&Sladeczek,I.E.(2012).Vocalstereotypyinchildrenwithautismspectrumdisorders:Areviewofbehavioralinterventions.BehaviorModification, 36,146–164.

Lanovaz,M.J.,Sladeczek,I.E.,&Rapp,J.T.(2012).Effectsofnoncontingentmusiconvocalstereotypyandtoymanipulationinchildrenwithautismspectrum disorders.BehavioralInterventions,27,207–223.

Lindberg,J.S.,Iwata,B.A.,Roscoe,E.M.,Worsdell,A.S.,&Hanley,G.P.(2003).Treatmentefficacyofnoncontingentreinforcementduringbriefandextended application.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,36,1–19.

Love,J.J.,Miguel,C.F.,Fernand,J.K.,&LaBrie,J.K.(2012).Theeffectsofmatchedstimulationandresponseinterruptionandredirectiononvocalstereotypy. JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,45,549–564.

MacDonald,R.,Green,G.,Mansfield,R.,Geckeler,A.,Gardenier,N.,Anderson,J.,etal.(2007).Stereotypyinyoungchildrenwithautismandtypicallydeveloping children.ResearchinDevelopmentalDisabilities,28,266–277.

Matson,J.L.,Dempsey,T.,&Fodstad,J.C.(2009).Stereotypiesandrepetitive/restrictivebehavioursininfantswithautismandpervasivedevelopmentaldisorder. DevelopmentalNeurorehabilitation,12,122–127.

Mayes,S.D.,&Calhoun,S.L.(2011).ImpactofIQ,age,SES,gender,andraceonautisticsymptoms.ResearchinAutismSpectrumDisorders,5,749–757.

Querim,A.C.,Iwata,B.A.,Roscoe,E.M.,Schlichenmeyer,K.J.,Ortega,J.V.,&Hurl,K.E.(2013).Functionalanalysisscreeningforproblembehaviormaintainedby automaticreinforcement.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,46,47–60.

Rapp,J.T.(2005).Someeffectsofaudioandvisualstimulationonmultipleformsofstereotypy.BehavioralInterventions,20,255–272.

Rapp,J.T.,Swanson,G.,Sheridan,S.,Enloe,K.,Maltese,D., Sennott,L.,etal.(2013).Immediateandsubsequenteffectsofmatchedandunmatchedstimulion targetedvocalstereotypyanduntargetedmotorstereotypy.BehaviorModification,37,543–567.

Rapp,J.T.,&Vollmer,T.R.(2005).StereotypyI:Areviewofbehavioralassessmentandtreatment.ResearchinDevelopmentalDisabilities,26,527–547.

Rozenblat,E.,Brown,J.L.,Brown,A.K.,Reeve,S.A.,&Reeve,K.F.(2009).EffectsofadjustingDROschedulesonthereductionofstereotypicvocalizationsin childrenwithautism.BehavioralInterventions,24,1–15.

Saylor,S.,Sidener,T.M.,Reeve,S.A.,Fetherston,A.,&Progar,P.R.(2012).Effectsofthreetypesofnoncontingentauditorystimulationonvocalstereotypyin childrenwithautism.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,45,185–190.

Schumacher,B.I.,&Rapp,J.T.(2011).Evaluationoftheimmediateandsubsequenteffectsofresponseinterruptionandredirectiononvocalstereotypy.Journalof AppliedBehaviorAnalysis,44,681–685.

Shabani,D.B.,&Lam,W.Y.(2013).Areviewofcomparisonstudiesinappliedbehavioranalysis.BehavioralInterventions,28,158–183.

Singh,N.N.,&Millichamp,C.J.(1987).Independentandsocialplayamongprofoundlymentallyretardedadults:Training,maintenance,generalization,and long-termfollow-up.JournalofAppliedBehaviorAnalysis,20,23–34.

Symons,F.,&Davis,M.(1994).Instructionalconditionsandstereotypedbehavior:Thefunctionofprompts.JournalofBehaviorTherapyandExperimental Psychiatry,25,317–324.

Taylor,B.A.,Hoch,H.,&Weissman,M.(2005).Theanalysisandtreatmentofvocalstereotypyinachildwithautism.BehavioralInterventions,20,239–253.

Watkins,N.,&Rapp,J.T.(2014).Environmentalenrichmentandresponsecost:Immediateandsubsequenteffectsonstereotypy.JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysishttp://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jaba.97.(advancedonlinepublication)

Figure

Fig. 2 shows the results of the analyses for Kyle (four upper panels), Morgan (two lower middle panels), and Lucas (two lower panels)
Table 4 presents the means and ranges of the response forms for the three participants
Fig. 8 presents the percentage of time Eric and Fred engaged in vocal stereotypy during 50- to 90-min sessions
Fig. 7. Percentage of time Eric (three upper panels) and Fred (three middle panels) engaged in stereotypy and functional play during prompting alone and noncontingent music with prompting sessions

Références

Documents relatifs

Aeru‑ ginosa chez le rat par SsoPox‑W263I : L’utilisation de SsoPox‑W263I permettait une réduction significative de la mortalité à 48 heures dans le groupe traitement

CANADIAN COUNSELLOR JOURNAL OF THE CANADIAN GUIDANCE AND COUNSELLING ASSOCIATION REVUE DE LA SOCIETE CANADIENNE D' ORIENTATION ET DE CONSULTATION..

Applied researchers who wish to estimate interactions in binary outcomes under multiplicity can, notably, specify a flexible selection mechanism dependent on estimated parameters

A multi- rate unscented Kalman filter is proposed for the first time in needle steering to fuse asynchronous data coming from 3D B-mode ultrasound images, robot sensors

Cette brève analyse du discours politique à travers le rap montre alors comment peut se former, à partir de la prise de parole, une posture contestataire. C’est lorsque l’on

We concluded that (i) the trajectory classes were of same nature on cells and glass, however the trajectory distribution was strikingly different between surface types, (ii) on

Indeed, registry data acquisition is based largely on voluntary reporting by physicians and/or clinical centers Table III Estimated number of new PID cases by age group in 2011,

“We take pretty seriously that idea of taking people on,” he says, “because more often than not, we’re making some kind of, you know, not forever marriage, but a