• Aucun résultat trouvé

Active Neural Field model of goal directed eye-movements

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Active Neural Field model of goal directed eye-movements"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-01839369

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01839369

Submitted on 14 Jul 2018

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access

archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci-entific research documents, whether they are pub-lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

Active Neural Field model of goal directed eye-movements

Jean-Charles Quinton, Laurent Goffart

To cite this version:

Jean-Charles Quinton, Laurent Goffart. Active Neural Field model of goal directed eye-movements. Grenoble Workshop on Models and Analysis of Eye Movements, Jun 2018, Grenoble, France. �hal-01839369�

(2)

www.posterse ssion.co m www.posterse ssi on.co m www.postersession.com

A

ctive

N

eural

F

ield model of goal directed eye-movements

Jean-Charles Quinton

1

, Laurent Goffart

2,3

1 Univ. Grenoble Alpes, CNRS, INPG, LJK, Grenoble, France 2 CNRS, Univ. Aix-Marseille, INT, Marseille, France

3 CNRS, Univ. Aix-Marseille, CGGG, Aix-en-Provence, France

from

s

ensory…

…to

m

otor ?

Moving target

A

ctive

N

eural

F

ield model

I

ntroduction

For primates (including humans), interacting with objects of interest in the environment often involves their foveation, many of them not being static (e.g. other animals, relative motion due to self-induced movement). Eye movements allow the active and continuous sampling of local information, exploiting the graded precision of visual signals (e.g., types and distributions of photoreceptors). Foveating and tracking targets thus requires adapting to their motion.

[1] Quinton, J-C. and Goffart, L. (2018) A unified neural field model of the dynamics of goal-directed eye movements. Connection Science, 30(1):20-52.

[2] Goffart L, Bourrelly C & Quinet J. (2017) Synchronizing the tracking eye movements with the motion of a visual target: basic neural processes. Prog Brain Res 236:243-268.

[3] Bourrelly, C., Quinet, J., Cavanagh, P., & Goffart, L. (2016). Learning the trajectory of a moving visual target and evolution of its tracking in the monkey. Journal of neurophysiology, 116(6), 2739-2751.

R

eferences

S

imulation

R

esults

(vs. experimental)

We here relied on a simplistic learning rule (the center of mass of the neural activity during the trial drives the adaptation of the projection eccentricity) and oculomotor commands (neural field to motor map). The computational model made it possible to replicate neurophysiological

findings in presence of rapidly moving targets (Bourrelly et al., 2016).

This research has received funding from the French program "investissement d'avenir" managed by the National Research Agency (ANR), from the European Union (Auvergne European Regional Development Funds) and from the "Region Auvergne" in the framework of the IMobS3 LabEx (ANR-10-LABX-16-01), as well as from the LabEx PERSYVAL-Lab (ANR-11-LABX-0025-01).

Considering the delays involved in the transmission of retinal signals to the eye muscles, a purely reactive schema could not account for the smooth pursuit movements which maintain the target within the central visual field. Internal models have been posited to represent the future position of the target (for instance extrapolating from past observations), in order to compensate for these delays. Yet, adaptation of the sensorimotor and neural activity may be sufficient to synchronize with the movement of the target, converging to encoding its location here-and-now, without explicitly resorting to any frame of reference

(Goffart et al., 2017).

Oculomotor muscles

Retinal

projection

Committing to a distributed dynamical systems approach, we relied on a computational implementation of neural fields to model an adaptation mechanism sufficient to select, focus and track rapidly moving targets. By

coupling the generation of eye-movements with dynamic neural field

models and a simple learning rule, we demonstrated how different eye-movements and the synchronization with rapidly moving targets could be generated from a single dynamical system without explicit encoding of the

target location (Quinton & Goffart, 2018). Qualitatively different behaviors corresponded to attractors (e.g., smooth pursuit as a fixed point attractor).

Focus ut ut+dt p2 p1 Projections p0 Competition c Stimulation s at+dt Eye movement Eye Action project. pa Divergent pathways Varying delays u(t)

catch-up saccades smooth pursuit saccades

fixations target

lost

Projection center of mass post-learning (along X axis)

Pro je ction (al ong Y ax is)

Trial index in learning sequence

Pro je ction ce nter of mass

Time in trial (in seconds)

Ey

e

/ target

X

-coordinate

• Distributed neuro-inspired model • Catch-up saccades reduced and

smooth pursuit velocity increased • No encoding of future locations of

the target (here-and-now at best) • Neural projections not matching

Références

Documents relatifs

Moreover, the appearance of this difference during the third block of adaptation trials (B block) cannot be easily related to the design of our protocol because (1) the two

In conclusion, we propose a neural model of early visual processing areas for motion detection, segmentation, and integration that suggests interactions between the

A possible arhiteture of how the major brain areas involved in the ontrol of. voluntary saadi eye movements might ooperate.(Refer to the text

The results show that the new controller can reach a target posed at a starting distance of 1.2 meters in less than 100 control steps (1 second) and it can follow a moving target at

Participants with a higher visual attention span showed better performance in orthographic decision and processing times.. The overall findings suggest that orthographic learning

Pairwise comparisons showed that correct saccades were larger when the target stimulus was a face than a vehicle (face: 6.75 ± 0.65°, Vehicle: 6.26 ± 0.89°, p < 0.005) but

Principal component analysis of the gene expression changes revealed that Wnt inhibition in FLCN KO cells rescued the defect in exit from human naïve pluripotency (Fig. 4 m),

For example, the spatial distance to the next fixation (saccade amplitude) is a key variable in rauding (words that are spatially close are much more likely to be selected