• Aucun résultat trouvé

What proportion of decodable words in a text is most beneficial for early reading instruction?

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "What proportion of decodable words in a text is most beneficial for early reading instruction?"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-03245270

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-03245270

Submitted on 1 Jun 2021

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

What proportion of decodable words in a text is most beneficial for early reading instruction?

Cynthia Boggio, Marie-Line Bosse, Céline Pobel-Burtin, Valérie Perthué, Maryse Bianco

To cite this version:

Cynthia Boggio, Marie-Line Bosse, Céline Pobel-Burtin, Valérie Perthué, Maryse Bianco. What pro-

portion of decodable words in a text is most beneficial for early reading instruction?. British Dyslexia

Association’s International Conference 2021, May 2021, Oxford, United Kingdom. �hal-03245270�

(2)

WHAT PROPORTION OF DECODABLE WORDS IN A TEXT IS MOST BENEFICIAL FOR EARLY READING INSTRUCTION?

Cynthia Boggio 1,3 , Marie-Line Bosse 1 , Céline Pobel-Burtin 2 , Valérie Perthué 3 & Maryse Bianco 2

1

Univ. Grenoble Alpes, CNRS, LPNC, 38000 Grenoble, France;

2

Univ. Grenoble Alpes, LaRAC, 38000 Grenoble, France;

3

Editions Hatier, 75006 Paris, France.

Acknowledgment:

Funding for this project was provided by Les Editions Hatier and we thank them for their support. We sincerely thank F. Pittion, S. Wang, E.

Montaut, A. Perinet and A. Leclercq-Samson for data analysis. Finally, we are very grateful to all the teachers and students who agreed to participate in this study. We would also like to thank the Academy of Grenoble for agreeing to support us in this project.

Introduction

Method

Data analysis Results

Conclusion

15 first grade teachers

Phonics method

246 French students Mean age: 6.5

Test 2

• Reading

• Phonological awareness

• Comprehension

• + Spelling Test 1

• Reading

• Phonological awareness

• Comprehension Septembre 2019

Novembre 2019 Every day

for 10 weeks

• Texts read by children

• Graphemes taught

 Calculation of the decodability rate with Anagraph (Rioux, 2018) Collection of decodability rate

Decodable text

Texts containing only graphemes already taught

Juel & Roper (1985) ; Mesmer (2005)

More

Allor et al. (2013); Price-Mohr & Price (2019); Solity & Vousden (2009)

Less

Cheatham & Allor (2012);

Jenkins et al. (2004); Slavin et al. (2009)

?

What is the best practice ?

Figure 1. Decodability rate by weeks and by classes

Note. Each line represents the average decodability rate per week. The dispersion between the minimum and maximum decodability rate of the week is represented by the shaded area.

• Decodability rates increase over the weeks for each class

• In the first weeks, the rates are very diverse across the classes

• After 9 weeks, the gap reduces and the rate concentrates between 70 and 90%

• To facilitate the analyses, a grouping of similar classes is necessary

Figure 2. Grouping of classes by clustering (by Kmeans and hierarchical classification methods)

Cluster A:

decodability rate around 50%

- Medium and stable

Cluster B:

decodability rate aroud 70%

- Medium to hight

Cluster C:

decodability rate aroud 20%

- Low to medium

Cluster D:

decodability rate around 90%

- Hight and stable

Mixt model:

glmer(ScoreT2~cluster+decodageT1+comprehensionT1+

phonologieT1+(1|Class) + (1|Item), family = binomial, df_T2)

Figure 3: Reading measure

Figure 4: Phonological awareness measure

Figure 5: Spelling measure

Figure 6: Comprehension measure

Decodabilityrate (%)

Weeks

A

C

B

D

 Measures of reading, phonological awareness and spelling : No significant effect of the cluster variable.

 Measure of comprehension : Students in cluster A performed significantly better than students in cluster B.

- High variability of mean percentage of decodable text across classes

- Three patterns of decodability emerged:

- Patterns do not significantly affect

• Reading

• Phonological awareness

• Spelling

- Patterns might affect the comprehension performance in line with Price-Mohr & Price (2019): less decodable texts would promote comprehension.

 Further studies are needed to confirm these results.

Ankrum (2021)

Both

References:

Allor, J. H., et al. (2013). Teaching students with intellectual disability to integrate reading skills: Effects of text and text-based lessons. RASE, 34(6), 346-356.

Ankrum, J. W. (2021). Complex Texts or Leveled Readers for the Primary Grades? Yes and Yes!. Early Child Educ J, 1-7.

Cheatham, P., & Allor, H. (2012). The influence of decodability in early reading text on reading achievement. Read Writ, 25, 2223–2246.

Jenkins, J. R., et al. (2004). Effects of reading decodable texts in supplemental first-grade tutoring. Sci Stud of Read, 8(1), 53–85 Juel, C., & Roper, D. (1985). The influence of basal readers on first grade reading. Read Res Q, 134-152.

Mesmer, H. A. E. (2005). Text decodability and the first-grade reader. Read Writ Q, 21(1), 61-86.

Price-Mohr, R., & Price, C. (2020). A comparison of children aged 4–5 years learning to read through instructional texts containing either a high or a low proportion of phonically-decodable words. Early Child Educ J, 48(1), 39-47.

Riou, J. (2018). La plateforme Anagraph, éléments de contexte et description. La Lettre de l'AIRDF, 64(1), 70-71.

Slavin, R. E., et al. (2009). Effective reading programs for the elementary grades: A best evidence synthesis. Rev Educ Res, 79(4),1391–1466.

Solity, J., & Vousden, J. (2009). Real books vs reading schemes: A new perspective from instructional psychology. EducPsychol, 29(4), 469–511.

0 5 10 15

Reading score /15

A B C D

0 2 4 6 8

Phonologicalscore /8

A B C D

0 4 8 12

Spellingscore /12

A B C D

0 2 4 6

Comprehensionscore /7

A B C D

*

After controlling the variability of initial student level, classes and items.

Weeks

Decodabilityrate (%)

1 2 3

4 5 6

7 8 9

10 11 12

13 14 15

Références

Documents relatifs

The results on vocabulary indicate that in this population of Portuguese- speaking children growing up in Luxembourg, Luxembourgish (L2) and German (L3) lexical knowledge was

versions of vector spaces, in which you can not only add and scalar multiply vectors, but can also compute lengths, angles, inner products, etc. One particularly common such

The analog versus digital distinction appears to involve two interrelated capabilities: lower-level perceptual capabilities to process geometric and topological properties

We retrieve synthetic word images corresponding to lexicon words and use dynamic k-NN to assign text label to a given scene text image.. We compared our method with the most

(C) Spiking responses of cortical neurons in vitro to somatic current injection (black) are well predicted by integrate- and-fire models with sharp spike initiation (blue, first

a) Henry started to work as a window-cleaner. c) He made his money only in selling and buying houses. d) He didn’t marry his girlfriend because he didn’t love her.. 4-Answer

Spelling words at the end of first grade: Is it easier to spell after hearing the word than after seeing its picture?. Cynthia Boggio, Maryse Bianco, Céline Pobel-Burtin,

1 The problem of identifying logics is not only of interest for proof theory, but for the whole area of logic, including mathematical logic as well as philosophical logic.. This