• Aucun résultat trouvé

*[ɦ-] > [ŋ-]? An unusual case of diachronic strengthening in Tikuna (isolate, Western Amazonia)

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "*[ɦ-] > [ŋ-]? An unusual case of diachronic strengthening in Tikuna (isolate, Western Amazonia)"

Copied!
1
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

*[ɦ-]  >  [ŋ-]? An unusual case of diachronic strengthening in Tikuna (isolate, Western Amazonia)

Denis Bertet

March 21, 2019—DDL (Lyon, France)

San Martín de Amacayacu Tikuna (SMAT;Colombia) features lexical word-initial velar nasals [ŋ]   /ŋ/, as in e.g. [ˈŋa11] /ŋa11/ ‘to scold’, [ˈŋo̞ː11wa5]   /ŋo11wa5/ ‘wild dog’, or   [ˈŋã33]   /~ŋa33/

‘lowland  paca’. The  velar  nasal  phoneme  /ŋ/ exhibits an anomalous distribution, in particular with respect to nasality, a syllable-level phonological feature. While  the  language’s  phonology  virtually   only generates either entirely oral or entirely nasal syllables on   the  surface,  /ŋ/  gives  rise  to  the   only phonetically half oral-half nasal syllables, as illustrated by the first two examples above.

In fact, in regard to nasality, syllables with a velar nasal onset can be shown to behave as though they had no onset synchronically. Interestingly, it so happens that various converging lines of evidence independently point at the   velar   nasal   realization   of   /ŋ/   in   today’s  SMAT being the result of a fairly recent development from an earlier word-initial laryngeal fricative or velar approximant (*≈[ɦ] or *≈[ɣ̞]), or possibly from a complete absence of onset (*[Ø]).

Language-internal synchronic evidence includes, among other things, a handful of functional words that display two alternative realizations—with  or  without  an  initial  [ŋ]—conditioned by the realization or omission of stress, as e.g. [ˈŋẽ̞ː33ɾɯ4]~[(ɦ)ẽ̞3ɾɯ4] for /(ˈ)~ŋe334/ ‘because’.  There are reasons to believe that in such pairs of surface realizations, the unstressed, [ŋ]-less alternative might reflect the former phonological shape of the word more faithfully than the other alternative, in which a secondary initial [ŋ]  could have arisen through strengthening (or even epenthesis) under the effect of stress. An unrelated piece of language-internal evidence is the phonologically exceptional   word   [ˈŋo̞ː31ho̞1ho̞3] ‘nocturnal rat sp.’,  most likely an onomatopoeic creation from

*≈[ho̞ho̞ho̞], an imitation of that rodent’s whisper.

Diachronic evidence includes wordlists by 19th century   European   explorers   in   which   today’s  

SMAT [ŋ]-initial words are systematically transcribed with an initial <h> or <Ø>, as in e.g. <ho-o>

or <å.å> for today’s SMAT [ˈŋo̞̰͡o̞ʔo̞3] ‘devil’.   Only by the middle of the 19th century do transcriptions with initial nasal graphemes (<n>, <mh>, <ñ>) begin to occasionally appear in these wordlists where   today’s  SMAT exhibits initial  [ŋ]’s. Furthermore, a few old loanwords from Old Omagua   and   Spanish   in   today’s  SMAT have developped a word-initial   [ŋ]   where   the   original   language had an absence of onset, as in e.g. [ˈŋɯː3ɾɯ3ka3ɾe1]   ‘mosquito   net’   <   Old   Omagua  

*[ɨɾɨˈkari]  ‘id.’.

One last line of evidence—one of dialectological nature—comes from the observation that the reflex of SMAT [ŋ]  in  certain  Tikuna  varieties  is  a  phone  I  tentatively  identify  as  [ɣ̞]. I hypothesize that this non-nasal, non-stop realization is phonetically closer to the ancestor of SMAT [ŋ] than  [ŋ]  

itself.

I finally show that there exist a few known cases of developments of velar nasals from former laryngeal fricatives—or even from a complete absence of syllable onset—across the languages of the world.

Taken as a whole, these individually weak pieces of evidence build a reasonably strong case for today’s  SMAT [ŋ]   being   an   innovation   that   arose   over   the   course   of   the   19th century. Its most plausible immediate ancestor is */ɦ/ [ɦ]. A laryngeal nature of the ancestor of today’s  SMAT /ŋ/  [ŋ]  

would make it typologically less surprising for it not to behave quite as a segment in the language.

The data this talk is based on come primarily from personal work with Tikuna consultants and first-hand recordings that were kindly shared to me by colleague researchers.

Références

Documents relatifs

Software design and allocation: The hardware designer may use a high level hardware description model of the targeted platform architecture, with only key properties of the

In actual speech, an NP may alternatively trigger agreement according to the lexical gender of its head (in which case it is underspecified for social deixis) or according to the

I believe that our College has already begun forging this trail through Canadian health care culture—we’ve called it the patient’s medical home, and we

The number of generations entirely visited for recurrent random walks on random environment.. Pierre Andreoletti,

Deaf signers (native and non-native) of Catalan Signed Language (LSC) were asked in a picture-sign interference task to sign picture names while ignoring distractor-signs with

Official  bilingualism  stipulates  that  French  and  English  are  the  two  official  languages  in  Canada.  The  two  groups  –  Anglophones  and 

ments restés debout en rive droite et la crevasse au pied amont qui atteste leur déplacement ; le dièdre ouvert au sein du versant rive gauche à l'aval immédiat des parties de

3D Engine, Avatars, VRML, X3D, Sign Language, French Sign Language, Automatic Translation, Natural Language .Automatic Processing,