• Aucun résultat trouvé

DEVELOPMENT OF HEAD-TURN PREFERENCE FROM 2 TO 12 WEEKS OF AGE: THE INFLUENCE OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONTEXTS

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "DEVELOPMENT OF HEAD-TURN PREFERENCE FROM 2 TO 12 WEEKS OF AGE: THE INFLUENCE OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONTEXTS"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-02479401

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02479401

Submitted on 14 Feb 2020

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

DEVELOPMENT OF HEAD-TURN PREFERENCE FROM 2 TO 12 WEEKS OF AGE: THE INFLUENCE

OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONTEXTS

Marianne Jover, M Clark, Daniela Corbetta

To cite this version:

Marianne Jover, M Clark, Daniela Corbetta. DEVELOPMENT OF HEAD-TURN PREFERENCE FROM 2 TO 12 WEEKS OF AGE: THE INFLUENCE OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONTEXTS.

Healthy and Active Children (I-MDRC CIAPSE), Sep 2019, Verona, Italy. �hal-02479401�

(2)

DEVELOPMENT OF HEAD-TURN PREFERENCE FROM 2 TO 12

WEEKS OF AGE: THE INFLUENCE OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONTEXTS

Jover, M. 1 , Clark, M. 2 , Corbetta, D. 2

1 Aix Marseille Univ, PSYCLE, Aix-en-Provence, France

2 Department of Psychology, the University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, USA

CONTEXT

The development of head control in early infancy has been studied for quite a long time (e.g. Turkewitz, 1965 ; Rönnqvist, Hopkins, 1998 ; Lima-Alvarez, et al., 2014). Two overarching conclusions from these studies are that prior to 2 months of age, infants’ head control is weak, especially when in the supine position, and that the majority of infants prefer turning their head to the right side. In most of these studies, an experimenter momentarily held the infants head at midline before releasing it, or presented a visual stimulus to induce head turn. Very little is known, however, about the development of infants’ spontaneous head movements during this early period.

METHOD

Spontaneous head movements were explored in 3 infants (GM, LN and MA) followed weekly from the age of 3 to 12 weeks, until they acquired head control. Infants were observed in 5 conditions: baseline, toys-in-view (preferred head turn side), musical toys out- of-view (non-preferred head turn side), mobile overhead, and mother reading to infant. The toys-in-view condition was not considered for this poster since the head would more likely remain turned in that condition. Each condition lasted 5 min while infants laid in supine on a white padded surface surrounded by two vertical white panels to block distractions from the surroundings. Two synched cameras, placed overhead and to the side, recorded the whole session.

Head movements were coded with respect to the frontal plane at a frequency of 15 Hz with a custom software which provided 2D kinematic data of selected points on the video (VideoAnalyser, Mare, 2010, figure 1). Data were filtered. The infants’ nose was the body point of interest selected to describe head turns (figure 2). The rate of change, direction, and duration of head turns were extracted and compared across conditions within each session.

Non-parametric Friedman and Durbin-Conover tests were used to compare the head movements between the conditions.

Figure 1: Video Analyser interface.

The small circular marker on the nose represents the 2D position (in pixels) that was coded frame by frame for each

infant.

180 190 200 210 220 230 240

200 220 240 260 280 300 320

pixel

pixel

Figure 2: Graphic representation of the infant’s nose displacement during 5mn used to describe head turns.

REFERENCES

Mare, K. (2010). Video Analyser: A program for the analysis of movements. Available from mailto:marianne.jover@univ-amu.fr

Turkewitz, G., Gordon, E. W., & Birch, H. G. (1965). Head turning in the human neonate: Spontaneous patterns. The Journal of genetic psychology, 107(1), 143-158.

Rönnqvist, L., & Hopkins, B. (1998). Head position preference in the human newborn: A new look. Child Development, 69(1), 13-23.

Lima-Alvarez, C. D. D., Tudella, E., van der Kamp, J., & Savelsbergh, G. J. (2014). Early development of head movements between birth and 4 months of age: a longitudinal study. Journal of motor behavior, 46(6), 415-422.

Robertson, S. S., & Johnson, S. L. (2009). Embodied infantattention.Developmental Science 12(2), 297304

0 20 40 60 80 100

% of motionless

CG MA LN Mean

0 5 10 15 20

cm

CG MA LN Mean

RESULTS

Infants acquired head control at 11 (CG), 13 (LN) and 14 (MA) weeks.

Descriptively, the mean amplitude of spontaneous head displacement was already large at 3 weeks and did not vary before head control was acquired (figure 3). However, the percentage of time the infant was motionless tended to decrease during that time (figure 4).

Figure 3: Amplitude of the infant’s nose displacement on the x and y axis according to the time to head control

Figure 4: Percentage of the time spent motionless (i.e., velocity <1cm/s)

according to the time to head control.

DISCUSSION

These preliminary results show that infant spontaneous head turns were present well before they acquired head control, however we do not see any developmental trend. Infants were more active in the Baseline and Mobile conditions and more still in the mother condition, possibly due to a motor inhibition mechanism triggered when attention (to the music and mother voice) is engaged (Robertson & Johnston, 2009). This indicates that young babies can move their head differentially depending on the context.

0 5 10 15 20

baseline mobile toysNP mother

cm

LN CG MA

x2=5.4, df=3, p<.15

0 20 40 60 80 100

baseline mobile toysNP mother

% of moti onles s

LN CG MA

x2=9, df=3, p<.03

Figure 5: Percentage of the time spent motionless (green), mean velocity (red) and amplitude of the infant’s nose displacement according to context.

Infants did not move their head the same depending of the context (figure 5). The difference between the condition was significant, showing that infants were longer motionless in the musical toy (toyNP) and mother conditions as compared to the other conditions. Moreover, their head movements were slower in those conditions. All pairwise comparison showed that each condition differed from each other. However, the amplitude of the infant’s nose displacement did not differ significantly according to the condition.

0 1 2 3 4

baseline mobile toysNP mother

Cm/ s

LN CG MA

x2=9, df=3, p<.03

Références

Documents relatifs

O livro estrutura-se em prefácio, prólogo e nove capítulos, ao longo dos quais são desenvolvidos os três eixos temáticos que Bernstein se pro- põe a criticamente analisar:

Neurosciences are a heterogeneous ensemble in which we can distinguish three major directions--neurological analysis of cognitive processes, experimental psychology and

Take into account the cultural settings in patterns of urban development and all territorial and urban planning in establishing the laws, standards and

Savonius Rotor; power coefficient; tip speed ratio; water current energy

Specifically, économie sociale et solidaire and associations are similar but not synonymous with voluntary sector and voluntary organizations, respectively;

In summary, project-based learning involving technology and toys is likely to give rise to important learning for students when it (a) allows for non-traditional and

Using this bench, a numerous tests conducted in order to evaluate different types of winding wire insulation: conventional, corona-resistant and high thermal resistance wires, as

A quantitative turn in the historiography of economics?1 José Edwards2, Yann Giraud3 & Christophe Schinckus4 Note: This short essay is an introduction to Not Everything that can