• Aucun résultat trouvé

Heuristics and mental flexibility in the problem solving processes of regular and gifted fifth and sixth graders

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Heuristics and mental flexibility in the problem solving processes of regular and gifted fifth and sixth graders"

Copied!
8
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-01287312

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01287312

Submitted on 12 Mar 2016

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

Heuristics and mental flexibility in the problem solving processes of regular and gifted fifth and sixth graders

Benjamin Rott

To cite this version:

Benjamin Rott. Heuristics and mental flexibility in the problem solving processes of regular and

gifted fifth and sixth graders. CERME 9 - Ninth Congress of the European Society for Research

in Mathematics Education, Charles University in Prague, Faculty of Education; ERME, Feb 2015,

Prague, Czech Republic. pp.1059-1065. �hal-01287312�

(2)

Heuristics and mental flexibility in the problem solving processes of regular and gifted fifth and sixth graders

Benjamin Rott

University of Duisburg-Essen, Faculty of Mathematics, Essen, Germany, benjamin.rott@uni-due.de

This study reports on a mixed-methods design analyzing the problem solving processes of gifted and regular fifth and sixth graders (age 10–12). The analysis focuses on heuristic techniques, indicating that the regular pupils need more heuristics to be equally successful as the gifted ones. This finding is explained by the theory that heuris- tics can be used by the regular students to compensate a lack of mental flexibility.

Keywords: Mathematical problem solving, heuristics, intellectual flexibility.

INTRODUCTION

Within problem solving, which is a very important part of mathematics, heuristics play an integral role.

It is generally assumed that the use of heuristics is related to problem solving with successful problem solvers using a greater number and variety of heu- ristics than less successful ones. But there are still some issues unresolved as these results have mostly been obtained with regular students. The number and appearance of heuristics in the processes of gifted or creative problem solvers might differ from those of regular problem solvers. In this article, the problem solving processes of two groups of fifth and sixth grad- ers are analyzed and compared: one group consists of pupils from regular lower secondary schools while the other group consists of pupils that have been very successful at mathematical competitions.

THEORETICAL BACKGROUND

The theoretical framework of this article focuses on an integral aspect of mathematics that is important for gifted as well as for regular students: on mathe- matical problem solving and particularly on the use

of heuristics in problem solving. After justifying the classification of gifted students by their participation in competitions, different aspects of problem solving are discussed. This implies the use of heuristics, their function of compensating a lack of intellectual flexi- bility, as well as reports on their trainability.

Mathematical competitions provide the opportuni- ty for students to actively engage in mathematical problem solving and to compete with other problem solvers. Successful participants of such competitions are mostly mathematically gifted and talented stu- dents (Bicknell & Riley, 2012); a fact which should be true especially for younger students as they have less opportunity (only regarding their age) to compensate missing talent by training. Classifying the successful participants as gifted also fits Renzulli’s (1978) clas- sical definition of giftedness as these students show above-average abilities as well as task commitment and creativity by solving the problems posed at math- ematical competitions.

The term “problem solving” has different meanings ranging from solving routine tasks to working in per- plexing or difficult situations (e.g., Schoenfeld, 1992).

This article refers to the latter interpretation, prob- lem solving as working on non-routine tasks. This implies that the attribute “problem” depends on the solver, not on the task. A task is a problem for a per- son that does not know any procedures or algorithms to solve it. Instead of algorithms that just have to be followed step by step, heuristics can help solving prob- lems by ordering or reducing the search space and by helping to generate new ideas (Rott, 2014). In this arti- cle, heuristics are considered as methods, mental tools, or mental operations such as “working backwards” or

“drawing a figure”. The use of heuristics is thoroughly

(3)

Heuristics and mental flexibility in the problem solving processes of regular and gifted fifth and sixth graders (Benjamin Rott)

1060 discussed by Schoenfeld (1992) in general and by Engel

(1998) who focuses on mathematical competitions.

Bruder (2003) and Bruder and Collet (2011) – draw- ing on the work of the psychologists Lompscher and Hasdorf – describe the qualities of creative and in- tuitive problem solvers: One main characteristic of these problem solvers is their intellectual flexibility that allows them (amongst others) to easily consid- er different aspects and to focus on important parts of problems. Bruder then divides intellectual flexi- bility into five groups of actions, namely Reduction, Reversibility, Consideration of Aspects, Change of Aspects, and Transferring. For the actions of each group of intellectual flexibility, she presents heuristic actions that are able to help less flexible problem solv- ers overcome their lack of intuitive problem solving skills. For example, the intuitive skill of structuring facts can be compensated by creating a table; the intui- tive skill of reversing relationships can be substituted by working backwards.

Research results regarding the trainability and use of heuristics can be summarized as follows: In the 1960ies and 70ies there have been several studies in America showing (often weak) positive correlations between the use of heuristic strategies and perfor- mance on ability tests as well as on specially construct- ed problem solving tests (Schoenfeld, 1992). Newer studies support this supposed relationship between the use of heuristics and success in problem solving (e.g., Komorek et al., 2007; Rott, 2012). Most of these studies did not only measure the number of heuristics used and the participants’ success in problem solving, but also conducted some sort of training. The results show consensually that usage of heuristics can be accomplished by training. However, these trainings have often been limited to small groups of problems with unknown transferability of strategies to other problems (cf. Schoenfeld, 1992); additionally, these trainings have been limited to regular (school and university) students without specifically addressing gifted students. The findings of these studies seem to support the claim of Bruder that learning heuristics can (at least partly) compensate the abilities of intui- tive problem solvers in regular students. But we do not know enough about the abilities of creative or gifted problem solvers to really draw such conclusions.

The research intention of this article is to further ex- plore the problem solving abilities of gifted students

and to compare them to those of regular students. Can these two groups be distinguished by the number and appearance of the heuristics in their processes? In the short run, such a comparison can help us to better un- derstand the way in which heuristics work. In the long run, this research can help us to better teach problem solving in schools (for gifted and regular students).

DESIGN OF THE STUDY

The aim of the research presented in this paper is to explore the problem solving behavior of fifth and sixth graders (aged 10 to 12) by analyzing and comparing the processes of two groups of pupils: novices and experts

1

.

“Novices”: The so-called novices were regular pupils from secondary schools in Hanover, Germany that took part in the first four terms of the support and research program MALU

2

for fifth graders, which lasted from November 2008 to June 2010 (with 10 – 15 pupils each term). These pupils came to the University of Hanover once a week for 1.5 hours and worked on problems for about half of this time. Ability tests and a consideration of school grades as selection criteria ensured a mixture of pupils that can be classified as

“non-gifted”.

“Experts”: The so-called pupil experts were successful participants of mathematical competitions, namely of the final round of the German Mathematical Olympiad

3

in 2009/10 (8 pupils of grade 5 and 6) and price winners of the Mathematical Kangaroo

4

in 2009 (2 pupils of grade 6). As very successful problem solv- ers at a young age, these pupils are considered to be

1 The use of the terms „non-gifted” and “gifted” is mostly avoided in this context because of possible negative connotations of

“non-gifted” and because there was no official test to ensure the

“gifted” status of the pupil experts – however, the second group meets the criterion of “reproducible superior performance” for expertise.

2 Mathematik AG an der Leibniz Universität which means Mathematics Working Group at Leibniz University

3 The German Mathematical Olympiad consists of four rounds:

(1) tasks to be solved at home to qualify for (2) a written tests at schools (180 minutes for grades 5 and 6). The best 200 stu- dents of all grades qualify for (3) the final round of the federal state which takes place at a central place. And the 12 winners of those state finals are invited to the final round of all German states – but this round is only for students of grade 8 and higher.

4 An international competition which consists of 30 tasks (75

min); 5 to 6 % of the German participants receive prices.

(4)

“gifted”. They were asked to take part in this study at the venue of the Olympiad and the school of the Kangaroo winners; the pupils did so voluntarily and have not worked with their video-partners beforehand.

To explore the pupils’ problem solving behavior, their processes were videotaped. To ensure uninfluenced problem solving attempts, the pupils worked with- out interruptions or hints from the researchers.

For the same reason the pupils were not trained to think-aloud or interrupted by interview questions;

instead, they were asked to work in pairs to enable an insight into their thoughts through their natural communication. Three problems were selected for the comparison (see Figure 1).

METHODOLOGY

To analyze the problem solving processes as well as the products (everything that had been written down or sketched during those processes) of both groups of pupils, a mixed methods design has been chosen. This allows for detailed analyses of singular processes as well as an overview of all the data.

Product Coding: The pupils’ products were graded in four categories of success: (1) No access, when they showed no signs of understanding the task properly or did not work on it meaningfully. (2) Basic access, when the pupils mainly understood the problem and showed basic approach. (3) Advanced access, when they understood the problem properly and solved it for the most part. And (4) full access, when the pupils

solved the task properly and presented appropriate reasons, if necessary.

This grading system was customized for each task with examples for each category. Then, all the prod- ucts were rated independently by the author and research assistants. After calculating Cohen’s kappa (k > 0.85 for each task), the few products with differing ratings were discussed and recoded, reaching consen- sus every time.

Process Coding – Heuristics: Occurrences of heu- ristic techniques (like drawing a figure or examining special cases) were coded using a manual that was de- veloped for analyzing videotaped problem solving processes (see Table 1); that development was based on empirical processes as well as on related research literature (Koichu et al., 2007, being the most notewor- thy influence; see Rott, 2012, for details). The coding procedure is a sort of qualitative content analysis (cf.

Mayring, 2000) which helps ensuring its reliability and objectivity and makes it suitable for qualitative as well as quantitative analyses.

All videos were coded independently by several re- searchers, who first identified points of time in the processes where heuristics were used and then char- acterized the heuristics afterwards. In accordance with the TIMSS 1999 video study (cf. Jacobs et al., 2003, p. 103 f.), the “percentage of agreement” approach

Figure 1: The three problems selected for the study

(5)

Heuristics and mental flexibility in the problem solving processes of regular and gifted fifth and sixth graders (Benjamin Rott)

1062 (Bakeman & Gottman, 1997, p. 59)

5

was used to com-

pute the interrater-reliability of randomly chosen videos (40 % of the processes). More than P

A

= 0.7 for identifying points of time in the videos with heuristics and more than P

A

= 0.85 for characterizing the heuris- tics was achieved. After calculating the reliability, all differing codes were analyzed and recoding consen- sually (100 % of the processes).

Pupils’ products were coded individually with the result that in 6 of the 55 processes discussed here the two members of a pair obtained results with differing product ratings. To manage the data, for each pair the better result was chosen to further work with. The heuristics in the pupils’ processes have also been cod- ed individually. The numbers given here represent the number of different heuristics noticed for each pair; for example, when one member of a pair drew a figure while the other one didn’t do so, this heuristic was counted for the pair.

6

RESULTS

Quantitative results

The evaluation presented here starts quantitatively by comparing some statistical results regarding the product and process codings of both groups. Looking at the novice group (i.e., the regular, non-gifted pu-

5 Chance-corrected measures like Cohen’s kappa are not suit- able for this calculation, as there is no model to calculate the agreement by chance for a random number of heuristics dis- tributed randomly over the course of the process.

6 Please note that in previous publications from the MALU data pool (e.g., Rott, 2012), the individually coded results have been reported. This time, the pair data is reported, therefore the numbers do not match those from previous articles.

pils), the results are distributed widely among the four product categories in all three problems indicating no ground or ceiling effects. As expected, the pupil experts are significantly more successful (c² = 14.54;

p < 0.001), scoring exclusively in categories 3 or 4.

For the novices, the number of coded heuristics is related to the success in solving the problems with mean scores of 1–3 heuristics in less and 3–5 heu- ristics in more successful processes (see Table 2 for details). There are significant Spearman rank-order correlations

7

for the Coasters (r

s

 = 0.69; p < 0.01), the Number Series (r

s

 = 0.78; p < 0.001), and the Chessboard (r

s

 = 0.99; p < 0.05) problems as well as for all three problems (r

s

 = 0.72; p < 0.001) combined. This finding is in accordance with research on the topic and the values mostly match the correlations reported by Komorek and colleagues (2007); this result meets the expectations as the use of heuristics should be helpful in solving problems.

However, there are successful processes with only one or two heuristics as there are unsuccessful ones with three or more. Some pairs picked a heuristic and used it to solve the problem outright; on the other hand, heuristics did not help every time. As expected, there is no straight “the-more-the-better” rule for the use of heuristics.

For the pupil experts, there is no such correlation between the number of heuristic techniques and suc-

7 As the product categories yield only ordinally scaled data, no Pearson correlation coefficient was calculated. The web-tool by R. Lowry (http://vassarstats.net/corr_rank.html) also pro- vides a way to calculate the significance level for n < 10.

Code Description Examples

Drawing a figure

Drawing a figure, a graph, or a diagram. Coasters: a drawing of possible positions of the two squares.

Number Series: drawing a diagram of numbers with possi- ble neighbors.

Auxili ary elements

Introducing auxiliary elements like aux- iliary lines or additional variables.

Coasters: drawing auxiliary lines to indicate area.

Chessboard: drawing borders of squares to illustrate their size or count them.

Special cases

Assigning special values (like 0 or 1) to algebraic problems or examining special positions in geometric problems.

Coasters: positions of the two squares which make it evident that the marked area amounts to one fourth of a square.

Mental flexi bility

“Thinking outside the box”; special ideas and activities that are not captured by other heuristic categories

Coasters: (mental) rotation of the squares. Number Series:

flexible way of adding numbers to both sides of the series.

Chessboard: easily identifying the possible overlap of squares bigger than 1x1.

Table 1: An extract of the heuristics coding manual

(6)

cess (r

s

 = -0.06; p = 0.84), but this can be explained by ceiling effects. Surprisingly, opposed to the vague

“the-more-the-better” rule, the experts use significant- ly less heuristics compared to equally successful nov- ices (i.e., pairs that reached product categories 3 or 4) (t = 2.73; p = 0.01).

Table 2 summarizes some statistical data of both groups. Column “count (%)” shows the number of pairs for each of the four product categories. Column

“heu” shows the mean numbers of heuristics coded in the processes for each product category. The experts use less heuristics for each problem in the

product categories 3 and 4.

Qualitative results

To further explore this surprising result – better problem solvers use less heuristics – the processes are analyzed qualitatively in the following paragraphs. Coded heuristic actions are indicated with italics.

The first (abbreviated and smoothed) exam-

ple deals with the Coasters problem. After reading the problem formulation, the novices Hannelore and Lucy start to measure the length of the sides of the squares to somehow calculate the requested area; Hannelore also introduces notations to points in the given figure.

They soon notice that their first approach does not work and start to question whether the squares could be arranged in another way. Lucy then draws figures, among them a special case in which the size of the area is easily identified (see Figure 2). They return to the given figure and start adding auxiliary lines to indi- cate a decomposition (see Figure 2). Lucy notices that

she could “cut off” a triangle and add it at the other side to regain the special case. She concludes that the area is always as big as in the special case. They then do not write down “a fourth of a square” but calculate the size of the requested area. Overall, they worked nearly 12 minutes on this problem.

Bernd and Tobi, two of the pupil experts, worked on this problem for 2 minutes. After reading, Bernd says: “Wait, this is exactly one fourth. Because you can push it over there, so that you get exactly four parts.” (mental flexibility) Tobi quickly agrees and they

request the next problem without further justifying their solution. Bernd’s recognition can be interpreted as Consideration of Aspects within the framework of intellectual flexibility, because he “recognize[s] the correlation of facts and [is] easily able to vary them.”

(Bruder, 2003, p. 17)

The second comparison of novice and expert process- es deals with Marco’s Number Series. It takes the nov- ices Birk and Janus more than 30 minutes to solve this problem. They start with “1, 3, 6, 10, 15” and are stuck, because there is no number left to add to 15 (they try

Table 2: Results of the heuristic coding for the pupil novices and experts

Figure 2: Lucy’s figures working on the Coasters problem

(7)

Heuristics and mental flexibility in the problem solving processes of regular and gifted fifth and sixth graders (Benjamin Rott)

1064 21 but reject this idea quickly). They start new rows

with different numbers, always adding numbers to the right end until they get stuck and start anew. To keep track of the numbers they had already used, they keep a list of all numbers from 1 to 15 to cross out (tool of systematization). In this period, they occasionally use backtracking, i.e. deleting the last number(s) to continue with a different number instead of starting a new row. After more than 20 minutes, Birk intro- duces a new idea; he creates a table with all possible combinations of two numbers smaller than 16 adding up to a square number. This way, he realizes that the numbers 8 and 9 only have one possible neighbor each and have to start and end the row (looking for patterns).

Shortly after this realization, they solve the problem.

It took the experts Robert and Lasse about 5 minutes to solve this problem. They immediately start with a list of numbers to cross out (tool of systematization) and begin their first row with the given example, “3, 6, 10”. Instead of only adding numbers to the right, they work on both sides of the row (mental flexibility). To the right, they add “15, 1, 8”. Finding no neighbor for the “8”, they do not restart, but complete the row on the left side: “13”, “12”, “4”, “5”, “11”, “14”, “2”, “7” and “9”.

Within the framework of intellectual flexibility, this idea can be interpreted as Change of Aspects, because

“[b]y intuition they consider different aspects of the problem which avoids or overcomes getting stuck.”

(Bruder, 2003, p. 17)

Of course, these two examples are more obvious than the majority of the processes; they have been selected to illustrate an argument. However, the appearance of mental flexibility seems to be a distinguishing fac- tor between novice and expert processes as this code appears significantly more often in the experts’ pro- cesses: overall, mental flexibility appears in 15 of 43 novice processes compared to 10 of 14 expert process- es (c² = 5.73; p = 0.017); this trend continues at the level of individual problems (Coasters: 8/16 compared to 3/5; Number Series: 6/16 compared to 4/5; Chessboard:

1/9 compared to 3/4).

The experts need less heuristics because of their men- tal flexibility. In other words, the novices that are not that flexible need more heuristics (and more time) to get similar ideas. This finding supports the claim of Bruder and Collet (2011) that a lack of intellectual flexibility can be compensated by the use of heuristics.

For example, not being able to imaginary rotate the

squares in the Coasters problem, most of the novices draw figures of squares in different positions.

DISCUSSION

The quantitative analysis of the regular pupils’ prob- lem solving processes showed a significant moderate to high correlation between the number of heuristics in those processes and the pupils’ success. This result is in accordance with the literature as a vague “the- more-the-better” rule according to the number of heu- ristics has been reported many times. Surprisingly, this result does not apply to the group of gifted pupils (successful participants of mathematical competi- tions, i.e., “experts”) whose processes have also been analyzed. These pupils have been more successful than the regular ones (as expected) but used signifi- cantly less heuristics than equally successful pupils of the first group. A qualitative comparison of the processes of both groups revealed an explanation for this finding: the experts’ processes contained a signif- icantly higher number of actions coded as “mental flexibility” than those of the novices (regular pupils).

This finding can be explained by the theory of Bruder and Collet (2011). The lack of intellectual flexibility of the regular students (compared to the experts) can at least partly be compensated by the use of heuristics.

This has implications for practicing and teaching problem solving. Of course, intuitive problem solvers have an advantage working on problems as “the active intuition might be the most important device for dis- coveries, e.g. the sudden realization of analogies can only be thought of as an intuitive event.” (Winter, 1989, p. 177, translated by the author) But not-so-intuitive problem solvers might overcome their disadvantage by learning and practicing heuristic techniques.

Of course, there are limitations to this study: Firstly,

the group sizes are quite small; especially the group of

the pupil experts consists of only ten pupils. A bigger

number of both regular and gifted pupils would be

desirable to see if the observed patterns can be con-

firmed. Secondly, the pupils have worked in pairs to

enable access to their thoughts through their natu-

ral communication. This might have influenced their

problem solving behavior and it might be problematic

to generalize the findings of this study to individual

problem solving behavior of regular and gifted pupils.

(8)

Further studies need to evaluate whether the actions of “mental flexibility” are a genuine part of gifted pupils’ problem solving behavior or whether being

“mentally flexible is a learned (and thus learnable) ability these pupils picked up when participating in and training for mathematical competitions. It might be that the actions coded as “mental flexibility” are a combination of heuristics which are performed most- ly cognitive and so elaborate that they are unidenti- fiable for the raters observing the problem solving processes of the pupil experts.

It would be interesting to see whether a follow up study with older students might provide a clearer distinction between regular and gifted students and similar results.

REFERENCES

Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Observing interaction:

An introduction to sequential analysis (2nd ed.). New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Bicknell, B., & Riley, T. (2012). The role of competitions in a math- ematics programme. APEX: The New Zealand Journal of Gifted Education, 17(1). Retrieved from www.giftedchildren.

org.nz/apex/pdfs17/bicknell.pdf.

Bruder, R. (2003). Learning Methods and Techniques on How to Solve Problems. Retrieved from http://www.math-learning.

com/files/learnmethod.pdf

Bruder, R., & Collet, Ch. (2011). Problemlösen lernen im Mathematikunterricht. Berlin: Cornelsen Verlag Scriptor.

Engel, A. (1998). Problem-Solving Strategies. New York:

Springer.

Jacobs, J., Garnier, H., Gallimore, R., Hollingsworth, H., Givvin, K.

B., Rust, K. et al. (2003). Third International Mathematics and Science Study 1999 Video Study Technical Report.

Volume 1: Mathematics. Washington: National Center for Education Statistics. Institute of Education Statistics, U. S.

Department of Education.

Koichu, B., Berman, A., & Moore, M. (2007). Heuristic literacy development and its relation to mathematical achieve- ments of middle school students. Instructional Science, 35, 99–139.

Komorek, E., Bruder, R., Collet, Ch., & Schmitz, B. (2007). Contents and results of an intervention study in mathematics lessons at secondary level I with a teaching concept to support mathematical problem-solving and self-regulative com- petencies. In M. Prenzel (Ed.), Studies on the educational quality of schools: the final report on the DFG priority pro- gramme (pp. 175–197). Münster: Waxmann.

Mayring, P. (2000). Qualitative Content Analysis [28 paragraphs].

Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung / Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 1(2), Art. 20. Retrieved from http://www.

qualitativeresearch.net/index.php/fqs/article/view/1089 Renzulli, J. S. (1978). What Makes Giftedness? Reexamining a

Definition. Phi Delta Kappan 60(3), 180–184, 261.

Rott, B. (2012). Heuristics in the Problem Solving Processes of Fifth Graders. In Tai-Yih Tso (Ed.), Proceedings 36

th

Conference of the Int. Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 4, pp. 35–42). Taipei, Taiwan:

PME.

Rott, B. (2014). Rethinking Heuristics – Characterizations and Examples. In A. Ambrus & E. Vasarhelyi (Eds.), Problem Solving in Mathematics Education – Proceedings of the 15

th

ProMath Conference (pp. 176–192). Haxelnyomda,

Hungary.

Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically:

Problem solving, metacognition, and sense-making in mathematics. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook for Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning (pp. 334–370). New York: MacMillan.

Winter, H. (1989). Entdeckendes Lernen im Mathematik-

unterricht – Einblicke in die Ideengeschichte und ihre

Bedeutung für die Pädagogik. Braunschweig: Vieweg.

Références

Documents relatifs

However, if we remove the objective function (i.e., we simply search for a schedule which satisfies the cumulative constraint without having to minimize the tardiness sum), then we

Bringing Together Cognitive Research and Educational Assessment Since PISA 2003, problem solving has come a long way from a pen-and-paper-based as- sessment of Analytical

Elle impliquait la construction d’une banque de fragments d’anticorps recombinants de type scFv à partir de la séquence d’un scFv correctement replié et fonctionnel

De plus, ce point aurait peut-être pu être abordé plus précisément en y ajoutant par exemple l’idée de dépistages, ou d’accompagnement des patients, hors patients éligibles

Il y a deux ans, j’ai effectué un stage d’initiation à la recherche au sein du Centre de Recherche Cardio-Thoracique de Bordeaux (INSERM U1045) Université

Uma ampla visão geral da bioeconomia na América Latina e no Caribe e experiências concretas em áreas como química verde e biocombustíveis na Argentina, Uruguai, Brasil, Colômbia

Il est structuré avec des liens sémantiques pensés par les concepteurs et proposés à l'apprenant à travers un logiciel de présentation du monde qui met en “menus”,

El usuario decide cuántas unidades de análisis desea evaluar. Puede escoger aplicar la metodología a sólo un espacio territorial o integrar en el análisis varias unidades