• Aucun résultat trouvé

Social influence and discourse similarity networks in workgroups

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Social influence and discourse similarity networks in workgroups"

Copied!
10
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Social

Networks

j ou rn a l h o m e p a g e :w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / s o c n e t

Social

influence

and

discourse

similarity

networks

in

workgroups

Johanne

Saint-Charles

a,∗

,

Pierre

Mongeau

b

aDépartementdecommunicationsocialeetpublique,Centrederechercheinterdisciplinairesurlebien-être,lasanté,lasociétéetl’environnement (CINBIOSE),UniversitéduQuébecàMontréal,CasePostale8888,succ.Centre-Ville,Montréal,Québec,H3C3P8,Canada

bDépartementdecommunicationsocialeetpublique,UniversitéduQuébecàMontréal,CasePostale8888,succ.Centre-Ville,Montréal,Québec,H3C3P8, Canada

a

r

t

i

c

l

e

i

n

f

o

Articlehistory: Availableonlinexxx Keywords: Discoursesimilarity Network Similaritythreshold Socialinfluence Workgroupstages Socio-semantic

a

b

s

t

r

a

c

t

Adoptingasocio-semanticperspective,thisstudyaimstoverifytherelationbetweensocialinfluence anddiscoursesimilaritynetworksinworkgroupsandexploreitsmodificationovertime.Dataconsistof videotranscriptsof453-hgroupmeetingsandweeklysociometricquestionnaires.Relationbetweentie strength,actorcentralitywithintheinfluencenetwork,andsharedelementsofdiscoursebetweengroup membersareexaminedovertime.Observedcorrelationssupportthehypothesisofarelationbetween socialinfluenceanddiscoursesimilarity.Changesovertimesuggestasimilaritythresholdabovewhich therelationbetweensimilarityandinfluenceisreversed.

©2017ElsevierB.V.Allrightsreserved.

1. Introduction

Socialinfluenceandrelatedphenomena,suchasleadershipand socialpower,areintrinsictoanyhumanorganisingprocess.Social influencehasbeenconceivedasarelationalprocessforwhich inter-personalinfluencehappeningatthelevelofthedyadconstitutes abuildingblock(Carteretal.,2015;FriedkinandJohnsen,2011; Simpsonetal.,2015).Inworkgroups,socialinfluenceaffectshow groupsmakedecision,conducttheirwork,orshapetheir under-standingofthetaskandtheenvironment,andmore(Johnsonetal., 2015;Melamedand Savage,2016; Pavitt,2014;Westaby etal., 2016).Interactionsandcommunicationarecentraltosocial influ-ence(Guastello,2007; Moscovici, 1988), and discourse isa key componentofboth. Here,discourse isunderstoodas“ageneral termthatappliestoeitherwrittenorspokenlanguagethatisused forsomecommunicativepurpose”(Ellis,1999,p.81).

Thestudypresentedinthispaperoffersanovelperspectiveon theinterplaybetweensocialinfluenceanddiscourseinworkgroups byexploringtherelationbetweensocialinfluencenetworksand discoursesimilaritynetworks,a typeof networkin which rela-tionshipsbetweenpeopleareestablishedonsharedelementsof discourse(words,expressions,concepts,topics,etc.).Basedon ver-balinteractionsbetweenworkgroupmembersovertime,thestudy offersempiricalsupportfortherelationbetweensocial relation-shipsanddiscoursesimilarityatthedyadicandgrouplevelsand

∗ Correspondingauthor.

E-mailaddress:saint-charles.johanne@uqam.ca(J.Saint-Charles).

confirmstherelevanceofperformingthattype ofjointanalysis tobetterunderstandcommunication phenomenasuchas inter-personal influence. Our resultsalso highlightthepertinence to consider strength and reciprocity of social ties in thestudy of discoursesimilarity.Finally,thelongitudinalaspectofthestudy suggestsnon-linearco-evolutionbetweenthetwonetworksand thepresenceofa“similaritythreshold”abovewhichgreater sim-ilarityisassociatedwithlessinfluence,aresultthatcouldexplain someaspectsoftheevolutionofworkgroupsthroughtime.

2. Structuralanddiscursiveapproachestosocialinfluence

Thestudyofsocialinfluencecanbegroupedintotwobroadand generallydistinctapproaches:(a)astructuralapproachthat under-standssocialinfluencethroughrelationshipsbetweenindividuals and(b)adiscursiveapproachthatidentifiesinfluencewithinthe discourse of social actors. In what follows, we present a brief overviewofbothandarguethateachapproachmustbetakeninto accounttobetterunderstandsocialinfluenceinworkgroups.

2.1. Astructuralapproach

Thestructuralapproachfocusesontherelationalaspectofsocial influenceandrelatedconcepts,suchasleadershipandsocialpower. Atthedyadiclevel,therelationalproximityperspectivetheorises thatthemerepresenceofatieallowsforinformationexchange and sharedinfluence (Rice,1993).However, numerous studies, startingwithColemanetal.(1957),havefoundevidencethattie strengthmustbeconsideredinthestudyofinfluence,particularly http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2017.09.001

(2)

giventherelationalintensityandreciprocityitimplies(Araland Walker,2014;Bondetal.,2012).Atthegroupororganisationlevel, socialinfluenceisseenasintertwinedwithmembers’relationship networksand,mostimportantly,thestructuresandpatternsthey create(Carteretal.,2015;Friedkin,1993;FriedkinandJohnsen, 2011;Saint-CharlesandMongeau,2009).AccordingtoBrassand Krackhardt(2012,p.355),“thestructureofsocialnetworksstrongly affects the extent to which personal attributes, cognition, and behaviour result in power in organisations”. Methodologically speaking,moststudiesusing this approach employsociometric questionnairestogatherdataoninfluencerelationshipsorother typesofrelationships,includingfriendship,advice,andsupport.

Networkcentralityseekstomeasure“theprominenceor impor-tanceoftheactorsina socialnetwork”(Wassermanand Faust, 1994,p.170).Degreecentralitybasedonthenumberofan indi-vidual’sincomingand outgoingties is thoughttoberelated to influencebecausecentralpeoplehaveeasieraccesstoresources (BrassandLabianca,1999).Manystudieshaveshownthatactors’ degreecentralitywithinnetworksislinkedtotheiroverall influ-encewithinthegrouportheorganisation,suchinfluencebeing measured through actors’ perceptions of the most influential individualsintheirgroupororganisationorusingavarietyof per-formancemeasures.Beginningwiththeearliestlaboratorystudies oncommunicationpatternsintask-orientedgroups(Bavelas,1950; Leavitt,1951),thislinkhasbeendemonstratedinavarietyof con-texts,includingissueresolutioninsmallorganisations(Friedkin, 1993); multiculturalteams (Salk and Brannen, 2000); the role of CEOs in mergers and acquisitions (El-Khatib et al., 2015); decision-making in Initial Public Offerings (Owen-Smith et al., 2015);innovation(Ibarra,1993a);organisationalstrategies(Boje andWhetten,1981);onlinediscussiongroups (Huffaker,2010); andmore.Nonetheless,theprocessthroughwhichanindividual assumesacentralpositioninanetworkisnotyetfullyunderstood. Researchershavealsoexploredhowcentralitycombineswith otherfactorsalsocorrelatedwithsocialinfluence,includingformal status(AstleyandSachdeva,1984);genderandethnicity(Ibarra, 1993b);politicalskills(Treadwayetal.,2013);trust(Sparroweand Liden,2005);anduseofstrategies,cognitiveandcommunicative abilities,emotionalabilities,andperformance(Brass,1985;Brass andBurkhardt,1993;Emery,2012;Krackhardt,1990;Zuchowski, 1987).

Thesekindsofstudiesarefrequentlycriticisedforfocusingon themetaphor of “ties as conduits”(Borgatti and Foster, 2003), whichneglectswhatcirculatesthroughtheconduits−notably, dis-course(LabiancaandBrass,2006;MongeandContractor,2003).In lightofthiscritique,thenextsectionexaminesstudiesthathave exploredtherelationbetweendiscourseandinfluence.

2.2. Adiscursiveapproach

Beginningwiththerhetoricaltradition(Craig,1999)and con-tinuingthroughagrowingcontemporaryinterestindiscoursein organisationalstudies(BarberioandMonti, 2014),discoursehas alwaysbeenseenasanimportantmeansofinfluenceandpower. Whathasbeencoined “thelinguisticturnof the20thcentury” (Rorty,1967)hasspurredtheemergenceofmanydistinctive,but complementary,fieldsofstudyfocusedon“discourse”(Barberio andMonti,2014;Oswicketal.,2010).Thevarioususagesofthe term“discourse” resultingfromthe field’sexpansion(Alvesson andKarreman,2000;Fairclough,2003;Jian,2008)haveproduced abroadconceptwhosecoremeaningisthatoflanguageusedby humansforcommunication.

Theinterplaybetweendiscourse andinfluencehasbeenthe subjectofasignificantnumberofstudies,manyofwhichfocused onhowdiscourse affectsorsupportstheinfluenceofdominant groupsorindividuals.Discourseisseenbothasinextricablylinked

withpowerandanessentialtoolofmanipulation(Aman,2009; Fairclough,1989;Van Dijk,1989; vanDijk, 2006;Wang,2006). Thesestudiesfavourpredominantlyqualitativemethods, includ-ingvariantsofdiscourseanalysis(critical,historical,Foucauldian); narrativeandmetaphoranalysis;linguisticanalysis,rhetoric,and interaction;andconversationanalysis.Qmethodology,a combina-tionofdiscourseandcontentanalysis,andothermixedmethods havealsobeenused(Balogunetal.,2014;Clareetal.,2013;Dijk, 2012;Holmes,2009;KotwalandPower,2015;McKenna,2016).

Thediversityof topicsand settingsin thisliteratureis itself testimonytothescientificandsocialimportanceoftherelation betweendiscourseandinfluence.Forexample,HardyandMaguire (2016)havequestionedthewaydiscourseonriskisintertwined withpowerissueswithinorganisations.Duvaletal.(2015)have shedlight ontheframing influenceofforms imposed ongrant applicants (primarily NGOs) by a granting agency for interna-tionaldevelopment.Othershaveexplored(dominant)publicpolicy discourseanddebatesoverissuessuchaswetlandmanagement (Clareetal.,2013).Anotherresearchtrendisconcernedwiththe “discourseofstrategy”,whichexaminesdiscoursefromthe per-spectiveof “strategyas practice”(Balogun et al.,2014,p. 176). Otherscholars haveexplored howdiscourse contributes tothe creationorreproductionofstatus,gender,andothersocial inequal-ities,aswellashowresistancetodominantdiscoursesisexpressed intheworkplace,education,publicpolicy,medicine,themedia, andsports(BergvallandRemlinger,1996;Codo,2011;Edleyand Wetherell,1997;EzeifekaandOsakwe,2013;KilbyandHorowitz, 2013;Mayes,2010;MenzandAl-Roubaie,2008;Prego-Vazquez, 2007;Toft,2014;ZanoniandJanssens,2015).

Organisational studies addressing managers’ influence and leadershipabound(DayandAntonakis,2012),andmorerecent the-orisationshavealsoconsideredleaders’discourses(Fairhurstand Connaughton,2014;FairhurstandCooren,2009).Transformational and neocharismatictheories(Jordan, 2005;Meda,2005), which arguethataleader’sinfluencedependsontheabilitytoframethe situationsoastoinspiremembers,arerepresentativeofthis litera-ture(Antonakisetal.,2004).Thisresearchtrendismoreconcerned with“whatmakesagoodleader”thanwithexposingpowerissues hiddeninthediscourse.

Insmallgroupsettings,the“dominantdiscourse”focusisalso lessprevalent.Forexample,researchershaveexaminedhow dis-courseinfluencesinteractionaldynamics duringmeetings; how chairpersonsguidemeetingstowardspecifictopics,andhow deci-sionsaremade(AsmußandSvennevig,2009;Barske,2009;Clifton, 2009;Holmes,2009;Lazzaro-Salazaretal.,2015).Micro-analytical approachesbasedondiscourse,linguistic,orconversationanalysis havefrequentlybeenusedtoexploretheseissues.

Theexplorationofconflicts,disagreements,andclique forma-tion as loci of influence hasalso revealed sequential discourse phenomenaintheconstructionofalliances(Kangasharju,2002). Nielsen (2009, p. 23) investigated “how interpretational work supportsorganisationalgoalsand values”inbusinessmeetings. Specifically,she shows how this interpretative work is accom-plishedthroughemployees’acquisitionoforganisationallanguage. Inequalityandpowerissuesbetweenstakeholdersinthecontext ofdeliberativesmallgroupsor“democracymeetings”havealso attractedattention(Lazzaro-Salazaretal.,2015;Vargasetal.,2016). With regard toleadership, Choiand Schnurr (2014) have used discourseanalysistoexploredifferentmembers’performanceof leadershipinaleaderlessteam.

Thisoverviewleaveslittledoubtastotheimportanceofthe interplaybetweensocialinfluenceanddiscoursebut,asKrinsky (2010,p.627)observes,thisresearchtrendhastendedtoneglect relationalcontexts,including “therelations betweenthethings speakerssay”,andtreatdiscourseasstaticratherthandynamic. In thesamevein, Brummans etal. (2008)notethat studies on

(3)

conflictframingare“unabletoexplaintheintricatecommunication processesthroughwhichpeoplemakesenseofasituation”.

Approachesfocusedontheco-constructionof meaninghave developedamoredialogicalinterpretationoftheinfluence pro-cess(ChoiandSchnurr,2014;FairhurstandConnaughton,2014) thatshedslightontherelationalandconstructedaspectsof mean-ing through discourse in cases, for instance, where influential individualsareseenasactingasintermediariesbetweenvarious representations of the situation (Barge, 1989, 1996; Barge and Hirokawa,1989;Thayer,1988).FairhurstandConnaughton(2014) haveshownintheirliteraturereviewanumberofstudieslookingat relationshipsandinteractionswithregardsmostlytoleadershipas aninfluence-orientedlanguage.Theirreviewhighlightsthe impor-tantrolediscourseandsocialinteractionsplayininfluenceandalso showthatthestructuralaspectremainsneglectedinthosestudies.

3. Linkingsocialnetworksanddiscoursesimilarity

A socio-semantic perspective for the joint study of social networksand discourse similarity appearsas a promising path tobringtogetherthestructuralandthediscursive.Scholarshave travelledthispath,althoughnotwithregardtoinfluence.

Homophily,thetendencyforpeopletoformrelationshipswith similarothers,isa well-establishedsocialphenomenon (Carley, 1991;CurryandDunbar,2013;McPhersonandSmith-Lovin,1987; McPhersonet al.,2001).Asstated byCurry and Dunbar(2013, p.337):“Individualswhoaresimilarwithregardtorace, ethnic-ity,sex,age,religion,education,occupation,socialclass,attitudes, opinions,andbeliefsaremorelikelytoassociatewithoneanother than would be expected by chance”. In view of this, semantic homophilyistobeexpected(RothandCointet,2010).Aswell,using similaritytohighlightcommonelementsof differentdiscourses hasbeensuggestedbyresearchersinterestedinidentifyinggroups ofindividualssharingsemanticuniverses(RoussiauandBonardi, 2001).Thereby,researchershavedevelopedmodelsandmeasures ofsimilarityofdiscoursebetweenindividualinterconnectedina socialnetwork toexplainvarious individual,socialand cultural phenomena.

Forinstance,Carley (1986a,1991) hasmodelledtherelation betweensocialinteractionsandcognitivestructureswithinsmall organisations.Hermodelfocusesonhowinformationexchanged throughthestructureofsocialinteractionsaffectsindividuals’ cog-nitivestructureandhowbothstructurescombinetocreategroup cohesion,consensus,and stability. In short,themore members sharethesameinformationthemorethegroupbecomes cohe-sive,capableofconsensusandstablethroughtime.Intheirstudyof riskperception,SchererandCho(2003)confirmedtheimportance social networks in building “groups or communities of like-minded”individuals.CointetandRothhavestudiedthecoevolution ofthesocialandsemanticnetworksofscientists(Roth,2008)and bloggers(CointetandRoth,2009)andhighlightedtheirsimilarities anddifferences(RothandCointet,2010).Forexample,although degreecentralityinthesocialnetworkiscorrelatedwith central-ityintheactors-conceptsnetworkbothnetworksexhibitdifferent patternsinthewaybothmeasuresarelinked.Intheirstudyofa community-basedprotest,TeoandLoosemore(2011)notedthat socialnetworksplay acritical rolein thetransmissionof ideas, information,beliefs,andperceptions.Theyobservedthatshared experiencesthroughrelationshipnetworkshelpedcreateshared culturalmeaningsandinterpretations.Danowski(2013)hasshown thatbetween2007and2011,“semantic-baseddivisionnetworks arebecomingmoredecoupledfromtheformalmembership-based networks”inhisstudyofthedivisionsoftheInternational Commu-nicationAssociation.Basovetal.(2017)andBasovandBrennecke (inpress)haveexploredinstrumental,expressiveandinteraction

networksofsmallself-runcreativeorganisationsandtheirlinksto sharedmeaningasawaytobringtogetherthesocialandthe cul-tural.Thecomplementaritybetweensocialnetworksanddiscourse similarityhavealsogivenrisetoseveralstudiesofsocialmediaand networksofco-citationandco-publication−trendsthatboth ben-efitfromtheincreasingavailabilityoflargedatasets(Roth,2013).

4. Workgroupsovertime

AworkgroupisunderstoodhereinasimilarwayasCohenand Bailey(1997):itisanensembleofindividualsmutuallyinteracting intheirtasksandsharingresponsibilityfortheoutcomes.Group membersconsiderthemselvesandarebeingconsideredbyothers asasocialentityembeddedinlargersocialsystems.Workgroups likesocialnetworksaredynamicandchangeovertime(Arrowetal., 2004;Faust,2011;MongeauandSaint-Charles,2011).

Manyofthenumerousgroupdevelopmentmodelsproposed thatgroupsevolvethroughvariousstages(Braaten,1974;Dunphy, 1968;Mills,1967;Schutz,1958;St-Arnaud,1978;Tuckman,1965; TuckmanandJensen,1977;Wheelan,1994).Althoughlinear mod-elshavebeencriticisedforneglectingtherolesoforganisational time, task types, and environmental uncertainty (Chang,2006; Hare, 2003; McGrath, 1991; Poole and DeSanctis, 1990), their validityissupportedbyresearch,particularlyforzero-history, lead-erless,andfixed-durationgroups(Braaten,1974;Dunphy,1968; Mills,1967;Wheelan,2009).

Linearprogressivemodelshaveidentifiedfromthree tonine stages leading to group maturity, at which point group mem-bersbecomemorefocusedontaskaccomplishment.Threemain stages are commonto all models(Chidambaram and Bostrom, 1996;Wheelan,2009).Borrowing fromtheterminologyinitially proposedbyTuckman(1965),wecallthesecommonstages form-ing,storming,andnorming.Theformingstage,asitsnameimplies, referstotheperiodduringwhichthegroupisformedandmembers areinobservationandtesting mode.Thisisfollowed by storm-ing,duringwhichdisagreementsarevoicedandconflictsappear. Resolvingtheseconflictsleadstotheestablishmentofnormsand groupstabilisation,ornorming.Thesestageshavefuzzyboundaries (McGrathetal.,2000);groupsmaylingerinonestagewithout nec-essarilymovingontothenext.Forexample,theformingstagemay takelongerinzero-historygroupsbecausemembersdonotalready knowoneanotherandmayneedmoretimeunderstandthegroup’s goals(ChidambaramandBostrom,1996).

With regard to the content of exchanges between group members,studieshaveshownasignificantincreaseinexchange prototypicalityandhomogeneityovertime(Postmesetal.,2005); thisisconsistentwithalinearprogressivemodel.Johnsonetal. (2015)observedthattheemergenceofalternativeopinionsearly ingroupdevelopmentmayleadtotaskconflicts,whichsuggests an explanation for the group moving into the storming stage. Theseresultsmayimply co-evolutionbetweengroupmembers’ exchangesandtherelationshipstheydevelop,thusaddingmore incentivetostudybothrelationshipsanddiscoursebetween mem-bers.

5. Hypothesis

Thevariousresearchtrendsidentifiedabovehaveestablished the importance of discourse and relationships for social influ-encein groups andorganisations, aswellas theneedto study themtogether.Wehavealsopresentedtherelevanceofdiscourse similaritynetworkstorevealrelationshipsbetweenindividuals. Therefore, our general hypothesis posits a positive association between influence networks and discourse similarity networks inworkgroups.Totestthisassociation,weproposefourspecific

(4)

hypotheses,oneatthedyadiclevel(interpersonalinfluence)and theothersatthegrouplevel.

Aswehaveseen,interpersonal(dyadic)influenceisconsidered oneofthebuildingblocksofsocialinfluenceandtheintensityand reciprocityofthedyadictieaffectthisinfluence.Thus,we hypothe-sisethatthestrongerthedyadictie,themoresimilarthediscourse.

Hypothesis1. Discourse similarity and influence tie strength

betweentwoindividualsarepositivelyrelated.

Atthegrouplevel,ourhypothesesarebased ondegree cen-tralityintheinfluencenetworkandareconsistentwithwhatis presentedintheliteraturecitedabove.Inordertotakeintoaccount thestrength ofties, weusedboth valueddegreecentrality and dichotomisedmatrices.Itisexpectedthatthemostcentral indi-vidualsintheinfluencenetworkwillbethosesharingthelargest numberofelementsofdiscoursewiththelargestnumberof mem-bers;theseindividualswillthereforebecentralinthediscourse similaritynetworks.

Hypothesis2. Thesimilarityofagroupmember’sdiscourseto

thatofallothergroupmembersispositivelyrelatedtohisorher valueddegreecentralityintheinfluencenetwork.

Becauseitaffectsdyadicinterpersonalinfluence,we hypothe-sisethattiestrengthwillalsobeofimportanceatthegrouplevel.

Hypothesis3. Thepositiverelationbetweendegreecentralityand

discoursesimilarityforstronginfluencetiesisstrongerthanthe relationbetweenweakinfluencetiesanddiscoursesimilarity.

Ourfourth hypothesisis basedonthethree stages ofgroup developmentpresentedabove.Weexpectthatthesestageswill bereflectedintheinterplaybetweentheinfluencenetworkand discoursesimilarity networks.Althoughwe expecta consistent positivecorrelationbetweendiscoursesimilarityandvalued cen-tralityintheinfluencenetwork,weassumethatthiscorrelation willbeweakerduringthesecondstage(storming)giventhatthis stageischaracterisedbydisagreementsandconflicts.The assump-tionhereisthatthewordsusedbymembersadvocatingdifferent opinionsshouldbedistinct.

Hypothesis4. Thepositiverelationbetweenvalueddegree

cen-tralityanddiscoursesimilaritywillbeweakerduringthesecond stageofgroupdevelopment.

6. Method

Theoverallmethodconsistsofcorrelatingdatafromsociometric questionnairesoninfluencetieswithinworkgroupswithdiscourse similaritynetworkscreatedfromgroupmembers’discussion dur-ingmeetings.

6.1. Sample

Dataforthisstudywerecollectedfromasampleof34 French-speakingstudents(29womenand5menintheirearlytwenties) enrolledin an undergraduate communication programme. Stu-dents were randomly divided into five groups of six to eight relatively homogeneous individuals in terms of ethnic origin, gender,educationalbackground,andage.Thesegroupscanbe con-sideredzero-history, leaderless,and of fixedduration. Students wereatthebeginningoftheirundergraduateprogramme,hadnot metoneanotherbefore,andnoformalstructureexistedwithin theirgroup.Thesegroupswereaskedtoworktogetheronatask worth40%oftheirtotalcoursemark;themarkwascollective.The taskwastoproduceanacademicpaperandpresentittotheir class-matesattheendofthesession.Thetaskwasthereforerealand impliedsignificantconsequencesforgroupmembers(thecourse

Table1

Categorisationofreciprocalties.

Tiestrength Reciprocity Strengthvaluecombination 1 Weakreciprocal(co-presence

influenceties)

1-1 2 Non-reciprocal(allinfluence

ties)

1-2,2-1,1-31-3,3−1 3 Reciprocal(stronginfluence

ties)

2-3,2-2,3-2,3-3

ismandatoryintheprogramme).Groupsmetforthreehoursper

weekninetimesovertenconsecutiveweeks(withtheexception

ofoneweek,whentherewasnomeeting).Allstudentswere

vol-unteers.Forethicalreasons,aprocedurewassetupallowingany

studenttoanonymouslyrefusetoparticipate.Thiswaspossible

becauseonlyhalfofthegroupsformedforthecoursewereretained

forthestudy.ThestudyreceivedResearchEthicsBoardapproval

fromtheauthors’institution.

6.2. Datacollection

Data oninfluence ties werecollected througha sociometric

questionnairefilledoutbygroupmembersthedayaftereach

meet-ing(n=306 withoutmissing values, 34 members×9 meetings;

consideringtheinevitableabsenceofmembersatmeetingsover

aperiodofnineweeks,theobservedminimumisn=280).

Datarelatedtodiscoursesimilaritywerecollectedfromvideo

transcriptsof453-hgroupmeetings(9meetings×5groups×3h)

fora totalof135hofvideo.Duetofinancialconstraints,

trans-cripts were limited to 30min per meeting. In order toensure

thattranscriptsbestreflectedmembers’contributions,theywere

dividedintothree10-minuteperiodspermeeting:beginning,

mid-meeting, and end-of-meeting.As a result, 22.5hof video were

transcribedforatotalof33,262“texts”,eachofwhichcorresponds

toaspeakingturn.

6.3. Influencetiestrength

Self-declaredtiestrengthwascapturedaftereachmeetingby

thefollowingquestion:“Inthelastmeeting,youwereinfluenced

by....”Acompletelistofgroupmemberswasprovidedand

fol-lowedbya3-pointLikert-typescale.Eachmembercouldusethis

scaletoevaluatehowmuchorhowoftenhe/shehadbeen

influ-encedbyeveryothermemberduringthemeeting:1=verylittle

orrarelyinfluenced;2=somewhatorsometimesinfluenced,and

3=greatlyorofteninfluenced.

We calculated the mean declared tie strength over the 9

meetingsforeachdyadwithineachgroup.Thecalculationwas

per-formedonorientedties(n=1454valid),i.e.theAline/Alexiadyad

wasconsidereddistinctfromtheAlexia/Alinedyad,forexample.

Strengthratingswerecategorisedaccordingtowhetherthey

werereciprocalornot.Giventhatthelowestrating“1”(weak

influ-enceties)canbeconsideredanexpressionofmereco-presence,

reciprocalratingsof“1”havebeenseparatedfromtheothers.When

membersofadyadusedtheratings2or3(influenceandstrong

influenceties),thetiewasconsideredreciprocal(seeTable1).

6.4. Centralityininfluencenetwork

Aswehaveseeninthepresentationofthestructuralapproach, degreecentralityhasbeenconsistentlyshownascorrelatedwith variousmeasuresof influence.As such,we canconfidently use centralityintheinfluencenetworkasaproxyforinfluence. Val-ueddegreecentralitiesininfluencenetworkswithineachgroup werecalculated.Centralitywasalsocalculatedfordichotomised matricesforeachstrengthratingandforcombinationsof2and

(5)

3(influence2).Becauseofthevariationingroupsize,meansof individualinfluencecentralitieswerenormalised.

6.5. Discoursesimilarity

Discoursesimilarityisoftenbasedontheuseofsimilarwords orexpressions(Carley,1993;MongeauandSaint-Charles,2014; Roth and Cointet, 2010) although it has been variously mea-sured.Forexample,similaritycanbederivedfromatopicanalysis (Blei and Lafferty, 2009; Danowski, 2011; Maier et al., 2015), automaticandmanualthematiccoding(Cohenetal.,2005; Saint-Charlesetal.,2014),semanticmapping(HolmbergandHellsten, 2016;LeydesdorffandHellsten,2006),cognitivemapping(Carley, 1997),QAPcorrelationofindividualsemanticnetworks(Basovand Brennecke,inpress;Danowski,2013)ordistributionalsemantics (FabreandLenci2015;Harris,1954).

Inthisstudy,discoursesimilarityisbasedonanalysisofvideo transcriptsofeachgroupmeeting.Textanalysisandcalculations usedtocreateadiscoursesimilarityindexwereperformedusing Sématosemanticanalysissoftware(Planteetal.,2005).

Adiscoursesimilarityindexwascalculatedbasedonan exami-nationofthesimilarityofwordsandnounphrasespresentwithin the transcribed text.Similarity was established using a French semanticthesaurusofsynonymsandsemanticproximities(Plante etal.,2005)togroupwordsandnounphrasesinthedatasetinto semanticfields.Asemanticfieldisacollectionofwordsornoun phrases with semantic affinities within a given dataset. Carley (1993)andRothandCointet(2010)suggestasimilartechnique togroupwordsandnounphrasesrepresentingthesameideato form“concept”.Forexample,inagivensetoftexts,thesemantic field“fight”mayincludethewords“confrontation”,“antagonism”, “battle”,“combat”,“conflict”,“opposition”,and“rivalry”ornoun phrasessuchas“siblingrivalry”.

A two-modematrix wascreatedby theties that linkgroup memberswiththesemanticfieldstheyused.Thesetiesarevalued accordingtothenumberoftimes anindividualemploysa par-ticularsemanticfield.Usingtheminimumvalueofallsemantic fieldssharedbyadyad,thismatrixisthenconvertedtoanactorx actormatrix.Forexample,ifAlineusesthesemanticfield“fight” 10timesduringthefirstmeetingwhileAlexiausesit20times,the dyadisattributed10“points”forthissemanticfield.This opera-tionisrepeatedforeachsharedsemanticfieldandthetotalisthen dividedbythetotal“points”foreachdyad.Giventhatsomegroup memberstalkmoreormoreoftenthanothers,frequency(number ofutterancesforeachmember)wasnormalisedandtotallengthof speechweightedusingthenumberofbitsinthetextfile.

Valuedcentralitywasthencalculatedforeachmeetingto pro-duceanindividualdiscoursesimilarityindex.Wealsocalculated themeansimilarityindexforeachindividualoverthe9meetings, aswellastheaveragediscoursesimilarityfordyads.

6.6. Groupstages

In order toexplore group development stages, we grouped observationsintothreeperiods.Becausetimingandmovingfrom onestagetoanothermayvaryfromgrouptogroup,wecreated threeequalperiodscontainingthreemeetingseach(thefirstthree, themiddlethree,andthelastthree).

7. Results

Hypothesis1whichstatesthatdiscoursesimilarityand influ-encetiestrengtharerelatedatthedyadiclevel,issupportedbythe observedcorrelationbetweenmeantiestrengthandaverage dis-coursesimilarityfordyadsoverthe9meetings.Thecorrelation,

Fig.1.Meandiscoursesimilarityaccordingtoreciprocityofinfluencetiestrength.

Table2

Correlationcoefficientsbetweeninfluencecentralityandsimilarityindex.

Influencecentrality Correlationwith individualmean similarityindex(n=34)

Correlationwith similarityindex (n=306) Allties(valued

centrality)

0.49** 0.30**

Stronginfluence(value 3)

0.57*** 0.35**

Influence(values2and 3)

0.40** 0.28**

Influence(value2) 0.33 0.11 Weakinfluence(value

1;co-presence)

−0.58*** −0.34***

**p<0.01;***p<0.001(two-tailedtest).

which is significant but moderated(r=0.42; p=0.000; n=200),

explains17.8%oftheobservedvariance.

Thishypothesisisalsosupportedbyasignificantdifferencein

averagediscoursesimilaritydependingonwhethertheinfluence

isreciprocal,non-reciprocal,orexpressesco-presence(dl=1451;

f=58.677;p<0.000).Fig.1showsthemeanlevelofsimilarityfor

eachcategory.

Hypothesis2whichstatesthatagroupmember’sdiscourse sim-ilaritywiththatofallothergroupmembersispositivelycorrelated with his or her valued degree centrality in the influence net-work,issupportedbyapositivebutmoderatecorrelationbetween meanvalueddegreecentralityandmeansimilarityindex(r=0.49; p<0.003;n=34)(Table2).

Totestwhetherthepositivecorrelationbetweenvalueddegree centrality anddiscourse similarity wasgreaterfor strong influ-encetiesthanforweakinfluenceties(Hypothesis3),wecalculated meandegreecentralityforthedichotomisedinfluencematricesand correlatedtheseresultswithmeandiscoursesimilarity(Table2). Resultsshowthatmeaninfluencecentralityismorestrongly corre-latedwithsimilarityindexforstrongerties(r=0.57;p<0.001)than fortiesofstrength2(r=0.33;p=n.s.).Moreover,moderate nega-tivecorrelationswereobservedbetweenmeandiscoursesimilarity andmeaninfluencecentrality(r=−0.58;p<0.001)forco-presence (weakinfluence)ties.Table2alsoshowscorrelationscalculatedfor all306observationsofall34peopleduring9meetings(ratherthan forthemean).Duetoweeklyvariability,correlationsarelowerbut similartowhatisobservedforthemeans.Strictlyspeaking,these resultsdonotsupportHypothesis3,whichhypothesisesaweaker butstillpositivecorrelation.Nonetheless,althoughthisresultis notentirelyconsistentwiththehypothesis,itisalignedwithitand providesanunexpectednuance.

(6)

Table3

Regressioncoefficientsforsimilarityasdependentvariable.

Influencevalue Beta(stderror) Model1 Stronginfluence(value3) 0.077(0.012)*** Model2 Stronginfluence(value3) 0.053(0.014)***

Weakinfluence(value1;co-presence)−0.0413(0.012)*** Unstandardisedcoefficients(stderror).**p<0.01;***p<0.001;(two-tailedtest).

Regression analysis shows that the best predictors of

dis-coursesimilarityresideattheextremesofmeasuredtiestrengths

(Table3).Itmeansthatthemoreonehasstrong(3)tieswithother members(model1)orthemoreheorsheonehasstrongtiesand thelessheorshehasco-presenceties(model2),themoreheor sheshareselementsofdiscoursewithothergroupmembers(or vice-versa).

We explored the existence of differences between low and highsimilarityindexes for valued influence centrality (without dichotomising).Todoso,wedividedvalidobservationsintotwo equal groups of low and high similarity indexes. Correlations betweendiscoursesimilarityand valued centralityinthe influ-encenetworkwithineachofthesegroupsarerelativelyweak,but significant.Inthesubgroupofthosewhosediscourseshowslittle similaritytothatofallgroupmembers,thereisapositive corre-lationwithvaluedcentrality(r=0.27;p<0.001;n=139),whilea negativecorrelation(r=−0.20;p<0.01;n=139)isobservedinthe subgroupofthosewhosediscourseishighlysimilartothatofall members.Inotherwords,withinthesubgroupoflowsimilarity, themoresimilaramemberistoallgroupmembers,thehigher hisorherinfluencecentrality,whiletheoppositeistrueforthe high-similaritysubgroup.Thisresultsuggeststhattheremaybea thresholdatwhichtherelationbetweendiscoursesimilarityand influencecentralityisreversed.

Totest whether thecorrelation betweencentrality and dis-course similarity weakened during the second stage of group development,asstatedby Hypothesis4, wecalculated correla-tionsbetweensimilarityindexesandcentralitiesforeachofthe threeperiods:P1–firstthreemeetings(n=92);P2–middlethree meetings(n=98);andP3–lastthreemeetings(n=88).Resultsshow partialsupportforHypothesis 4:forP1, similarityand central-itygrowsimultaneously(r=0.49;p<0.001);forP2, there isno significantcorrelationand,forP3,thecorrelationispositiveand significant,butlow(r=0.29;p<0.005).

Inordertobetterunderstandthis resultand consideringthe possibilityofathresholdatwhichtherelationbetween similar-ityandinfluencereversesdirection,wecreatedfiveequalgroups ofpercentile-basedobservationsforeachperiod.While maintain-ingasufficient number ofobservationsbygroups, thisdivision makesitpossibletoexaminethepotentialemergenceofasimilarity threshold.

Asexpected,therelationislinearduringP1(Fig.2a).ForP2, thenon-linearrelationexplainstheabsenceofasignificant corre-lation.Observationsabovethe80thsimilaritypercentileindicate lessinfluence(Fig.2b).Thecorrelationislinearbelowthis per-centile(r=0.41;p<0.001;n=60)andsimilartothatofP1(r=0.49; p<0.001; n=92), but negative above this threshold (r=−0.45; p<0.005;n=38).Thedifferencebetweenthemeansimilarityof thesetwogroupsissignificant(p<0.001;f=4.338;dl=37).ForP3, thislowcorrelationcanbeattributedtothepresenceoftwomain groups,onewithlowsimilarityandinfluence,theotherwithhigher similarityandinfluence.Themeandifferencebetweenthesegroups issignificant(p<0.001;dl=87;f=108.09)(Fig.2c).

8. Discussion

Oneofourstudy’sprimarycontributionsistobringempirical supporttotherelationbetweensocialnetworksanddiscourse sim-ilarity.Inthatwecomfortotherstudiesthathaveobtainedsimilar resultsinothercontextsandwithdifferentwaysofmeasuring dis-coursesimilarity(BasovandBrennecke,in press;Carley,1986a, 1991;CointetandRoth,2009;Danowski,2013;Roth,2008;Roth and Cointet,2010; Scherer andCho, 2003;Teoand Loosemore, 2011).Suchencouragingresultsareaninvitationtopursuethis lineofresearch.

Toourknowledge,thisisthefirsttimethatthistypeof longitudi-nalanalysishasbeenrealisedinworkgroupstobetterunderstanda communicationphenomenonsuchasinterpersonalinfluence.Also, fromamethodologicalpointofview,weareawareofnootherstudy usinga socio-semanticapproach toanalysediscoursesimilarity networksbasedonverbalinteractionsbetweengroupmembers1

acommondatasourcefordiscourseanalysisinqualitativemethods (Holmes,2009).Ourstudydemonstratesthepertinenceof quan-titativeanalysisofverbalinteractionsasawaytouncoverother aspectsofthelinksbetweendiscourseandrelationships.

Asecondcontributionofthisstudyliesintheimportancegiven totiestrength(AralandWalker,2014;Bondetal.,2012).Under a moderatecorrelationbetweenvaluedcentralities ininfluence networksandsimilarityindexes,distinctphenomenaarehidden: thedirectionofthecorrelation isreversed betweenthose with numerous strong influence ties (r=0.57) and those with many co-presence(weakinfluence)ties(r=−0.58).Combinedwithour resultatthedyadiclevel,whichshowstheimportanceofstrength andreciprocityfordiscoursesimilarity,thissupportstheneedto considertiestrengthinthestudyofsocialinfluence.More impor-tantly,itopensnewavenuestodeepenourunderstandingofthe factorsinplaywhenonereachesacentralpositioninaworkgroup influencenetwork.Becauseourstudywasconductedwith zero-history,leaderless,highlyhomogeneousgroupswithregardtoage, genderandethnicity,weknowthatneitherpre-existingstructure norstatusdifferenceswerepresent.Thismayimplythat seman-tichomophily(RothandCointet,2010)playedaroleearlyinthe group’slifeandcontributedtothedevelopmentofstrong influ-encerelationshipsbetweensemanticallysimilargroupmembers, thereby“excluding”othersintheprocess.Usingthefrequencyof contactsasaproxyfortiestrength,Basovetal.(2017)haveobtained slightlydifferentresults.Indeed,theyfoundthatindividualswith thehighestfrequency ofcontactshave a relativelylowlevel of conceptssharingwhilethosewithlessfrequencyofcontactshave atendencytobridgecontainbetweensubgroups.Withregardto socialinfluence,thismaysuggesttheonesweinteractwith regu-larlyarenotnecessarilytheoneshavingthebiggestinfluenceon theevolutionofourdiscourse.

Alternativelytothestrengthoftieexplanation,individual char-acteristicsassociatedwithinfluenceintheliterature,suchhasa largerepertoire of communication strategies (Bass,1990), may havefavouredthecentralityofgroupmemberspossessingsuch arepertoireandrenderedtheirdiscoursemore“adoptable”. Con-versely, given that “perceived listening effectiveness” hasbeen showntoaffectemergingleadershipinworkgroups(Johnsonand Bechler,1998),itmaybethatgroupmembersbecamemore cen-tralintheinfluencenetworkbecausetheywereusingthewordsof otherswho,asaresult,feltlistenedto.

1Basovetal.(2017)dataincludesverbalexpressionsofgroupmembersextracted fromamixofinterviewsandethnographicobservationsofdyadicconversations combinedwithvarious“publications”madebymembers(posts,journalarticles, proseandpoetry),adifferentdatasetthanverbatimtranscriptofexchangesbetween membersduringworksessions.

(7)

Fig.2. a,b,c.Influencevaluedcentralityandfivepercentilegroupsofsimilarityindexesforthreeperiods.

Ourthirdandfourthcontributionsarebasedonalongitudinal exploratoryanalysis.Firstly,theresultsofthis studyshowthat therelationbetweeninfluencenetwork centralitiesand similar-ityindexesinworkgroupschangesthroughtimeinanon-linear mannerthat appears tofollow thethree main stages of group development.Duringthefirststage,thepositivecorrelationmay beexplainedbythefactthatthosewiththehighestsimilaritymay beperceivedasmore“prototypical”(Brownetal.,2004;Hoggetal., 2004)ortransformational(Jordan,2005;Meda,2005)becausethe wordsandexpressionsusedaresimilartothoseemployedbyother groupmembers.Atthesecondstage,theanalysissuggestsa “sim-ilaritythreshold”aroundthe80thpercentile,abovewhichgreater similarityis associated withlessinfluence. Thisconstitutes our fourthcontribution.ThisthresholdcouldbeparalleledtoCointet andRoth(2009)studyofbloggersinwhichthemost“influential” blogsseemed tobethosewiththelargerreadership − uptoa pointas“aboveacertainthreshold,theincreaseininfluenceis flat-ter,althoughstillrelativelyincreasing”(p.6).Withregardtofixed durationworkgroups,thesupposedthresholdappearsduringthe midtermmeetings,hereunderstoodasthesecondstageofgroup developmentmarked by debates, confrontations, and decision-makingregardingthetaskathand.Influenceand similarityare positivelycorrelatedbelow thisthresholdand negatively corre-latedaboveit.Weproposethatsimilaritymustbehighenoughfor peopletobe“in”thegroup,butlowenoughfortheirdifferenceto standout;originalelementsintheirdiscoursedistinguishthem. Ina debate,differencemaybeattractive(Robertand Mongeau, 2014).Thisassumptionisalsoconsistentwiththeexpectancy vio-lationtheoryproposedbyBurgoonandBacue(2009)inthestudyof non-verbalcommunication.Thistheorystatesthatbehavioursthat deviateslightlyfromthenormattractattention.Ourresultssuggest thepotentialgeneralisationofthistheorytoverbalcommunication. Alternatively,inhertheoryofstabilityinsocialgroup(Carley,1991) postulatesthataperfectlystablegroupwouldbecome unproduc-tiveforlackofnewinformationtoexchange.Inviewofthis,the similaritythresholdcouldbeinterpretedasamanifestationofa groupautoregulationmechanism:abovethesimilaritythreshold members’attentionshouldshiftfromsimilarelementstomore dissimilaronesastoomuchsimilarityreducesthemotivationto exchangeinformation.

Finally,thedivisionintotwogroupsatthelaststagemaybe interpretedasaphenomenonofexclusionofthelesssimilarand lesscentralmembersintheinfluencenetwork.Indeed,onecould postulatethat duringstage2,themostcentralmembersinthe influencenetworkwerethosemoreactivelyparticipatinginthe debates−continuingtheconversationuntiltheyreachacertain levelofconvergencethatexcludesothersfromthenew,negotiated, discourse.

Letus reiteratethat therelationbetweeninfluenceand dis-courseisnot seen hereinterms oflinear causality.Adifferent discourseintheearlystagesofagroup’slifecouldleadtohavingless influence,whichcouldinturnhelpmaintainadistinctdiscourse.

Conversely,holdingasimilardiscoursecouldsupportthecreation ofstronginfluenceties,whichmaythenhelpmaintaindiscourse similarity.

9. Conclusion

Thegoalofourstudywastoverifythehypothesisofa rela-tionbetweenworkgroupmembers’influenceandtheirdiscourse similaritywiththatofothergroupmembers.Todoso,we exam-inedtiestrength,actorcentralityininfluencenetwork,andshared discourseelementsinvideotranscriptsof453-hgroupmeetings andweeklysociometricquestionnaires.Wealsoexploredthe evo-lutionoftherelationbetweeninfluencecentralityanddiscourse similarityovertime.

Adoptingasocio-semanticperspective,ourstudycontributes inseveralwaystothejointapplicationofstructuralanddiscursive approaches.Amongstthestudiesbringingtogethersocialnetworks anddiscoursesimilarityitis,toourknowledge,theonlystudyto dothisforverbalexchangesinworkgroups,andwithafocuson socialinfluencefromalongitudinalperspective.

Ourresultsshowthatinfluencenetworkcentralityiscorrelated withdiscoursesimilaritynetworkcentrality(i.e.havingadiscourse thatmostresemblesthatofallgroupmembers).Atboththedyadic andgrouplevels,discoursesimilarityishigherforstrong(and recip-rocal)ties.Theseresultsalsosuggestthatinfluenceanddiscourse sharingco-evolvethroughtimeandgroupmembers’interactions. Atthebeginningofthegroup’slife, therelationbetween influ-enceandsimilarityislinearandpositive,butatthemidtermthis relationbecomesnegativeforthosewiththehighestcentralityin thediscoursesimilaritynetwork.Inthefinalmeetings,wenoted thepresenceoftwosubgroups(onewithhighsimilarityandhigh centralitymembersandtheotherwithlowsimilarityandlow influ-ence);althoughdifferencebetweenthesesubgroupsissignificant, differencewithinthemisnot.Thesechangesthroughtimehaveled ustopostulatetheexistenceofasimilaritythresholdbelowwhich similaritybringsgreaterinfluenceandabovewhichoriginalityand differencearerequired.

These findingsare theresult ofcapturing and analysingthe verbal interactions of members in homogeneous, zero-history, leaderless,fixed-duration,bonafidegroupsoverasufficientlylong period.Thestrengthofthisdatasetisalsoa limitation:because thesearestudentgroups,therearenotmanygroupsinother orga-nisationswhosharetheircharacteristics2orwhowouldbewilling

tohave allof theirmeetingsrecorded.Because observationsin ourdatavaryconsiderablyfromoneweektothenextandfrom onegrouptoanother,longerperiodsandmoregroupswouldbe required, butcapturing and transcribingverbalinteractions are timeconsumingandcostly.Othermeansarethereforecalledfor.

2Groupsof“founders”maywellhavesuchcharacteristics,atleastintermsof homogeneityand“leaderless”.

(8)

Althoughoneadvantageofthesocio-demographic homogene-ityofourgroupsisthatitlimitstheimpactofvariablessuchas statusandgender,thesearenon-negligibleintheprocessofsocial influence.Forinstance,studiestakinggenderintoaccountshow differencesbetweenmenandwomeninterms ofinfluenceand accordingtocommunicationstyle(Carli,2001;Eaglyetal.,2003). Gendermayalsoinfluencetheuseofcertainsemanticfields.Carley (1986b)hasshownthat,overtime,menadoptconceptsfirst pro-posedbywomen.Thegroupsstudiedherewerepredominantly composedofwomen;acomparativeanalysisbasedongenderwas thereforenotpossible.Aswehaveseen,studieslinkingcentrality toinfluencehavealsodemonstratedtheimpactofothervariables, includingcommunicationstrategiesandpoliticalskills, personal-ity,emotionalabilities,andethnicity.Thevarianceexplainedbyour results,whichfluctuatesbetween16%and34%,isnotable consid-eringthemultitudeoffactorspotentiallyatplay.Thisconfirmsthe relevanceofstudyingtheinterplaybetweendiscourseand influ-encenetworksandalsoraisesquestionsaboutthecombinedeffect ofmultiplefactors.

Regardingsemanticdata,ourstudyisbasedonsemanticfields automaticallyextractedbyatextanalysissoftwareusinga seman-ticthesaurus.Differentwaystomeasuresimilaritycouldleadto differentresults.Forinstance,analysesmorefocusedonmeaning, suchasthematicortopiccategorisation,maynarrowtheresults andmakeitpossibletodistinguishbetween,forexample,usingthe samewordstoexpressagreementordisagreement.Suchan anal-ysiswouldalsopermittheexplorationofsharedcognitiveframes. Anotherpotentiallineofinquiryistheanalysisofverbalstrategies usedbygroupmembers,giventhatleadershipemergencehasbeen linkedtostrategieslikesummarising,orientinggroupprocesses, andseekingevaluation(Barge,1989).

Toconclude,ourresultsreveala newavenue forclosingthe gapbetweentiesasconduitsandtheflowofcontent(Borgattiand Foster,2003)throughthestudyofgroupsimilarityandopennew perspectivesfor studyingtheemergenceofsocial influenceand leadershipinworkgroups.

Acknowledgement

ThisworkwassupportedbytheSocialSciencesandHumanities ResearchCouncilofCanada[grantnumbers410-2007-1438& 820-2008-1054].

References

Alvesson,M.,Karreman,D.,2000.Varietiesofdiscourse:onthestudyof organizationsthroughdiscourseanalysis.Hum.Relat.53,1125–1149. Aman,I.,2009.Discourseandstrivingforpower:ananalysisofBarisanNasional’s

2004Malaysiangeneralelectionmanifesto.DiscourseSoc.20,659–684. Antonakis,J.,Cianciolo,A.T.,Sternberg,R.J.,2004.Leadership:past,present,and

future.In:Antonakis,J.,Cianciolo,A.T.,Sternberg,R.J.(Eds.),TheNatureof Leadership.ThousandOaks:SagePublications,Californie,pp.125–170. Aral,S.,Walker,D.,2014.Tiestrength,embeddedness,andsocialinfluence:a

large-Scalenetworkedexperiment.Manage.Sci.60,1352–1370.

Arrow,H.,Poole,M.S.,Henry,K.B.,Whellan,S.,Moreland,R.,2004.Time,change, anddevelopment:thetemporalperspectiveongroups.SmallGroupRes.35, 73–105.

Asmuß,B.,Svennevig,J.,2009.Meetingtalk.J.Bus.Commun.46,3–22.

Astley,W.G.,Sachdeva,P.S.,1984.Structuralsourcesofintraorganizationalpower: atheoreticalsynthesis.Acad.Manage.Rev.9,104–113.

Balogun,J.,Jacobs,C.,Jarzabkowski,P.,Mantere,S.,Vaara,E.,2014.Placingstrategy discourseincontext:sociomateriality,sensemaking,andpower:placing strategydiscourseincontext.J.Manage.Stud.51,175–201.

Barberio,V.,Monti,A.,2014.Réseauxsémantiquesetlégitimédudiscours organisationnel:uneillustrationempirique.Communiquer.Revue internationaledecommunicationsocialeetpublique,7–25.

Barge,J.K.,Hirokawa,R.Y.,1989.Towardacommunicationcompetencymodelof groupleadership.SmallGroupBehav.20,167–189.

Barge,J.K.,1989.Leadershipasmedium:aleaderlessgroupdiscussionmodel. Commun.Q.37,237–247.

Barge,J.K.,1996.Leadershipskillsanddialecticsofleadershipingroupdecision making.In:Hirokawa,R.Y.,Poole,M.S.(Eds.),CommunicationandGroup DecisionMaking.Sage,London,pp.301–342.

Barske,T.,2009.Sametoken,differentactions:aconversationanalyticstudyof socialroles,embodiedactions,andokingermanbusinessmeetings.J.Bus. Commun.46,120–149.

Basov,N.Brennecke,J.DualitybeyondDyads:multiplexpatterningofsocialties andculturalmeaningsRes.Sociol.Organ.53:(inpress).

Basov,N.,Lee,J.-S.,Antoniuk,A.,2017.Socialnetworksandconstructionofculture: asocio-Semanticanalysisofartgroups.In:Cherifi,H.,Gaito,S.,Quattrociocchi, W.,Sala,A.(Eds.),ComplexNetworks&TheirApplicationsV:Proceedingsof the5thInternationalWorkshoponComplexNetworksandTheirApplications (COMPLEXNETWORKS2016).SpringerInternationalPublishing,Cham,pp. 785–796.

Bass,B.M.,1990.Bass&Stogdill’sHandbookofLeadership:Theory,Research,and ManagerialApplications.FreePress,NewYork.

Bavelas,A.,1950.Communicationpatternsintask-orientedgroups.J.Acoust.Soc. Am.22,725–730.

Bergvall,V.L.,Remlinger,K.A.,1996.Reproduction,resistanceandgenderin educationaldiscourse:theroleofcriticaldiscourseanalysis.DiscourseSoc.7, 453–479.

Blei,D.,Lafferty,J.D.,2009.Topicmodels.Textmining:classification,clustering, andapplications10,34.

Boje,D.M.,Whetten,D.A.,1981.Effectsoforganizationalstrategiesandcontextual constraintsoncentralityandattributionsofinfluenceininterorganizational networks.Adm.Sci.Q.26,378.

Bond,R.M.,Fariss,C.J.,Jones,J.J.,Kramer,A.D.I.,Marlow,C.,Settle,J.E.,Fowler,J.H., 2012.A61-million-personexperimentinsocialinfluenceandpolitical mobilization.Nature489,295–298.

Borgatti,S.P.,Foster,P.C.,2003.Thenetworkparadigminorganizationalresearch: areviewandtypology.J.Manage.29,991–1013.

Braaten,L.J.,1974.Developmentalphasesofencountergroupsandrelated intensivegroups.InterpersonalDev.,112–129.

Brass,D.J.,Burkhardt,M.E.,1993.Potentialpowerandpoweruse:aninvestigation ofstructureandbehavior.Acad.Manage.J.36,441–470.

Brass,D.j.,Krackhardt,D.M.,2012.Politicsinorganizations:theoryandresearch considerations.In:Ferris,G.R.,Treadway,D.C.(Eds.),SIOPOrganizational FrontiersSeries.Routledge,Taylor&FrancisGroup,NewYork,pp.355–375. Brass,D.J.,Labianca,G.,1999.Socialcapital,thesocialledger,andsocialressources

management.In:Leenders,R.,Gabbay,S.(Eds.),CorporateSocialCapitaland Liability.KluwerAcademicPublishers,Norwell(MA),pp.323–338.

Brass,D.J.,1985.Men’sandwomen’snetworks:astudyoninteractionpatternsand influenceinanorganization.Acad.Manage.J.28,327–343.

Brown,D.J.,Scott,K.A.,Lewis,H.,2004.Informationprocessingandleadership.In: Antonakis,J.,Cianciolo,A.T.,Sternberg,R.J.(Eds.),TheNatureofLeadership. ThousandOaks:SagePublications,Californie,pp.125–170.

Brummans,B.H.J.M.,Putnam,L.L.,Gray,B.,Hanke,R.,Lewicki,R.J.,Wiethoff,C., 2008.Makingsenseofintractablemultipartyconflict:astudyofframingin fourenvironmentaldisputes.Commun.Monogr.75,25–51.

Burgoon,J.K.,Bacue,A.E.,2009.Nonverbalcommunicationskills.In:Greene,J.O., Burleson,B.R.(Eds.),HandbookofCommunicationandSocialInteractionSkills. Routledge,NewYork.

Carley,K.M.,1986a.Anapproachforrelatingsocialstructuretocognitive structure.J.Math.Sociol.12,137–189.

Carley,K.M.,1986b.Knowledgeacquisitionasasocialphenomenon.Inst.Sci.14, 381–438.

Carley,K.M.,1991.Atheoryofgroupstability.Am.Sociol.Rev.56,331–354. Carley,K.,1993.Codingchoicesfortextualanalysis:acomparisonofcontent

analysisandmapanalysis.Sociol.Methodol.23,75.

Carley,K.M.,1997.Anapproachforrelatingsocialstructuretocognitivestructure. J.OrganizationalBehav.18,533–558.

Carli,L.L.,2001.Genderandsocialinfluence.J.Soc.Issues57,725–741. Carter,D.R.,DeChurch,L.A.,Braun,M.T.,Contractor,N.S.,2015.Socialnetwork

approachestoleadership:anintegrativeconceptualreview.J.Appl.Psychol. 100,597–622.

Chang,A.,2006.Understandingthemultidimensionalityofgroupdevelopment. SmallGroupRes.37,327–350.

Chidambaram,L.,Bostrom,R.,1996.Groupdevelopment(I):areviewand synthesisofdevelopmentmodels.GroupDecis.Negotiation6,159–187. Choi,S.,Schnurr,S.,2014.Exploringdistributedleadership:solvingdisagreements

andnegotiatingconsensusina‘leaderless’team.DiscourseStud.16,3–24. Clare,S.,Krogman,N.,Caine,K.J.,2013.Thebalancediscourse:acasestudyof

powerandwetlandmanagement.Geoforum49,40–49.

Clifton,J.,2009.Beyondtaxonomiesofinfluence:doinginfluenceandmaking decisionsinmanagementteammeetings.J.Bus.Commun.46,57–79. Codo,E.,2011.Regimentingdiscourse,controllingbodies:disinformation,

evaluationandmoralcategorizationinastatebureaucraticagency.Discourse Soc.22,723–742.

Cohen,S.G.,Bailey,D.E.,1997.Whatmakesteamswork:groupeffectiveness researchfromtheshopFIoortotheexecutivesuite.J.Manage.23,239–290. Cohen,H.,Lefebvre,C.,Meunier,J.-G.,Forest,D.,Biskri,I.,2005.Classificationand

categorizationincomputerassistedreadingandanalysisoftexts.In: HandbookofCategorizationinCognitiveScience.Elsevier,Amsterdam(pp. 955–978%@0080446124).

Cointet,J.-P.,Roth,C.,2009.Socio-semanticDynamicsinaBlogNetwork.,pp. 114–121.

(9)

Coleman,J.,Katz,E.,Menzel,H.,1957.Thediffusionofaninnovationamong physicians.Sociometry20,253–270.

Craig,R.T.,1999.CommunicationTheoryasaField.CommunicationTheory9, 119–161.2009(versionfranc¸aise)Lacommunicationentantquechamp d’études,Revueinternationaledecommunicationsocialeetpublique.2001 (2001),pp.2001–2042.

Curry,O.,Dunbar,R.I.M.,2013.Dobirdsofafeatherflocktogether?:the relationshipbetweensimilarityandaltruisminsocialnetworks.Hum.Nat.24, 336–347.

Danowski,J.A.,2011.SocialNetworksBasedonSemanticNetworkSimilarity: AssessmentofLatentDirichletApproximation(LDA)TopicModelingtoIndex Persons’Similarity.INSNA—SunbeltXXXI,St-Pete,Florida.

Danowski,J.A.,2013.ChangeOverTimeinICADivisionNetworksBasedon SemanticSimilarityofPapersPresentedVersusCo-Memberships.In:ICA(Ed.). InternationalCommunicationAssociation,London,England.

Day,D.,Antonakis,J.,2012.Leadership:past,present,andfuture.In:TheNatureof Leadership.SagePublications,pp.3–25.

Dijk,T.A.V.,2012.Knowledge,discourseanddomination.In:Meeuwis,M., ¨Ostman, J.-O.(Eds.),PragmaticizingUnderstanding.StudiesforJefVerschueren.John BenjaminsPublishingCompany,pp.151–196.

Dunphy,D.C.,1968.Phases,roles,andmythsinself-analyticgroups.J.Appl.Behav. Sci.4,195–225.

Duval,A.-M.,Gendron,Y.,Roux-Dufort,C.,2015.Exhibitingnongovernmental organizations:reifyingtheperformancediscoursethroughframingpower. Crit.Perspect.Acounting29,31–53.

Eagly,A.H.,Johannesen-Schmidt,M.C.,vanEngen,M.L.,2003.Transformational, transactional,andlaissez-Faireleadershipstyles:ameta-Analysiscomparing womenandmen.Psychol.Bull.129,569–591.

Edley,N.,Wetherell,M.,1997.Jockeyingforposition:theconstructionof masculineidentities.DiscourseSoc.8,203–217.

El-Khatib,R.,Fogel,K.,Jandik,T.,2015.CEOnetworkcentralityandmerger performance.J.FinancialEcon.116,349–382.

Ellis,D.G.,1999.FromLanguagetoCommunication,2nded.LawrenceErlbaum Associates,Mahwah,N.J.

Emery,C.,2012.Uncoveringtheroleofemotionalabilitiesinleadership emergence:alongitudinalanalysisofleadershipnetworks.Soc.Networks34, 429–437.

Ezeifeka,C.R.,Osakwe,N.N.,2013.Genderrepresentationinthe1999Nigerian constitution:acriticaldiscourseanalysisforsocio-politicalequity.Discourse Soc.24,687–700.

Fabre,C.,Lenci,A.,2015.DistributionalSemanticsToday,vol.56.TAL,pp.7–20. Fairclough,N.,1989.LanguageandPower.Longman,London;NewYork. Fairclough,N.,2003.AnalysingDiscourse:TextualAnalysisforSocialResearch.

Routledge,London;NewYork.

Fairhurst,G.T.,Connaughton,S.L.,2014.Leadership:acommunicativeperspective. Leadership10,7–35.

Fairhurst,G.T.,Cooren,F.,2009.Leadershipasthehybridproductionof presence(s).Leadership5,496–490.

Faust,K.,2011.Animalsocialnetworks.In:Scott,J.,Carrington,P.J.(Eds.),TheSAGE HandbookofSocialNetworkAnalysis.SAGE,London;ThousandOaks,Calif(p. 622).

Friedkin,N.E.,Johnsen,E.C.,2011.SocialInfluenceNetworkTheory:ASociological ExaminationofSmallGroupDynamics.CambridgeUniversity,PressNewYork. Friedkin,N.E.,1993.Structuralbasesofinterpersonalinfluenceingroups:a

longitudinalstudy.Am.Sociol.Rev.58,861–872.

Guastello,S.J.,2007.Non-lineardynamicsandleadershipemergence.Leadership Quarterly18,357–369.

Hardy,C.,Maguire,S.,2016.Organizingrisk:discourse,power,andriskification. Acad.Manage.Rev.41,80–108.

Hare,A.P.,2003.Roles,relationships,andgroupsinorganizations:some conclusionsandrecommendations.SmallGroupRes.34,123–154. Harris,Z.S.,1954.DistributionalStruct.10,146–162.

Hogg,M.A.,Abrams,D.,Otten,S.,Hinkle,S.,2004.Thesocialidentityperspective: intergrouprelations,self-Conception,andsmallgroups.SmallGroupRes.35, 246–276.

Holmberg,K.,Hellsten,I.,2016.Integratinganddifferentiatingmeaningsin tweetingaboutthefifthIntergovernmentalPanelonClimateChange(IPCC) report.FirstMonday21.

Holmes,J.,2009.Languageintheworkplace.In:LiteratureReview.Victoria UniversityofWellington,VictoriaNewZealand.

Huffaker,D.,2010.Dimensionsofleadershipandsocialinfluenceinonline communities.Hum.Commun.Res.36,593–617.

Ibarra,H.,1993a.Networkcentrality,powerandinnovationinvolvement: determinantsoftechnicalandadministrativeroles.Acad.Manage.J.38, 471–501.

Ibarra,H.,1993b.Personalnetworksofwomenandminoritiesinmanagement:a conceptualframework.Acad.Manage.Rev.18,56–87.

Jian,G.,2008.Fixingthemeaningofchange:analysisofchangemanagement meetingexcerpts.In:The58thAnnualInternationalCommunication AssociationConference,Montréal.

Johnson,S.D.,Bechler,C.,1998.Examiningtherelationshipbetweenlistening effectivenessandleadershipemergence:perceptions,behaviors,andrecall. SmallGroupRes.29,452–471.

Johnson,A.R.,vandeSchoot,R.,Delmar,F.,Crano,W.D.,2015.Socialinfluence interpretationofinterpersonalprocessesandteamperformanceovertime usingbayesianmodelselection.J.Manage.41,574–606.

Jordan,D.A.,2005.APhenomenologicalStudyofTranscendentLeadersin Healthcare.MedicalUniversityofSouthCarolina−CollegeofHealth Professions,SouthCarolina(p.395).

Kangasharju,H.,2002.Alignmentindisagreement:formingoppositionalalliances incommitteemeetings.J.Pragmatics34,1447–1471.

Kilby,L.,Horowitz,A.D.,2013.Openingupterrorismtalk:thesequentialand categoricalproductionofdiscursivepowerwithinthecallopeningsofatalk radiobroadcast.DiscourseSoc.24,725–742.

Kotwal,A.,Power,K.,2015.Eatingwords:adiscoursehistoricalanalysisofthe publicdebateoverIndia’s2013NationalFoodSecurityAct.OnHorizon23, 174–189.

Krackhardt,D.,1990.Assessingthepoliticallandscape:structure,cognitionand powerinorganizations.Adm.Sci.Q.,342–369.

Krinsky,J.,2010.Dynamicsofhegemony:mappingmechanismsofculturaland politicalpowerinthedebatesoverworkfareinNewYorkCity,1993–1999. Poetics38,625–648.

Labianca,G.,Brass,D.J.,2006.Exploringthesocialledger:negativerelationships andnegativeasymmetryinsocialnetworksinorganizations.Acad.Manage. Rev.31,596–614.

Lazzaro-Salazar,M.V.,Marra,M.,Holmes,J.,Vine,B.,2015.Doingpowerand negotiatingthroughdisagreementinpublicmeetings.PragmaticsSoc.6, 444–464.

Leavitt,H.J.,1951.Someeffectsofcertaincommunicationpatternsongroup performance.J.AbnormalSoc.Psychol.,38–50.

Leydesdorff,L.,Hellsten,I.,2006.Measuringthemeaningofwordsincontexts:an automatedanalysisofcontroversiesabout‘Monarchbutterflies,’

‘Frankenfoods,’and‘stemcells’.Scientometrics67,231–258.

Maier,D.,Waldherr,A.,Miltner,P.,Jaehnichen,P.,Pfetsch,B.,2015.Exploringissues inanetworkedpublicsphere.Combininghyperlinknetworkanalysisandtopic modeling.In:Atteveldt,W.V.(Ed.),BringingTogetherSocialandSemantic NetworksinCommunicationResearch.CACommunication&Technology, PoliticalCommunication&MassCommunicationDivisions,PuertoRico(p.36). Mayes,P.,2010.Thediscursiveconstructionofidentityandpowerinthecritical

classroom:implicationsforappliedcriticaltheories.DiscourseSoc.21, 189–210.

McGrath,J.E.,Arrow,H.,Berdahl,J.L.,2000.Thestudyofgroups:past,present,and future.Pers.Soc.Psychol.Rev.4,95–105.

McGrath,J.E.,1991.Time,interaction,andperformance(TIP):Atheoryofgroups. SmallGroupRes.22,147–174.

McKenna,E.F.,2016.Thediscourseofdeferenceanditsimpactontourist-Host powerrelations.J.TravelRes.55,555–565.

McPherson,J.M.,Smith-Lovin,L.,1987.Homophilyinvoluntaryorganizations: statusdistanceandthecompositionofface-to-Facegroups.Am.Sociol.Rev. 52,370–379.

McPherson,M.,Smith-Lovin,L.,Cook,J.M.,2001.Birdsofafeather.Homophilyin SocialNetworks27,415–444.

Meda,A.K.,2005.TheSocialConstructionofEthicalLeadership.Benedictine University%Uinternal-pdf://Meda,2005–1142137344/Meda,2005.pdf,Illinois. Melamed,D.,Savage,S.V.,2016.Status,factionsizes,andsocialinfluence:testing

thetheoreticalmechanism.A.J.S122(July).

Menz,F.,Al-Roubaie,A.,2008.Interruptions,statusandgenderinmedical interviews:theharderyoubrake:thelongerittakes.DiscourseSoc.19, 645–666.

Mills,T.M.,1967.TheSociologyofSmallGroups.Prentice-Hall,EnglewoodCliffs, NJ.

Monge,P.R.,Contractor,N.S.,2003.TheoriesofCommunicationNetworks.Oxford UniversityPress,NewYork.

Mongeau,P.,Saint-Charles,J.,2011.Approchescommunicationnellesdesgroupes danslesorganisations.In:Grosjean,S.,Bonneville,L.(Eds.),Communication Organisationnelle:Approches,ProcessusEtEnjeux.Chenelière,Montréal(p. 346).

Mongeau,P.,Saint-Charles,J.,2014.Centralitéderéseauxetsimilitudedediscours: uneapprochesociosémantiqueduleadershipémergentdanslesgroupesde travail.Communiquer.Revuedecommunicationsocialeetpublique,121–139. Moscovici,S.,1988.Lamachineàfairedesdieux:sociologieetpsychologie.Fayard,

Paris.

Nielsen,M.F.,2009.Interpretativemanagementinbusinessmeetings: understandingmanagers’interactionalstrategiesthroughconversation analysis.J.Bus.Commun.46,23–56.

Oswick,C.,Grant,D.,Marshak,R.J.,WolframCox,J.,2010.Organizationaldiscourse andchange:positions,perspectives,progressandprospects.J.Appl.Behav.Sci. 46,8–15.

Owen-Smith,J.,Cotton-Nessler,N.C.,Buhr,H.,2015.Networkeffectson organizationaldecision-making:blendedsocialmechanismsandIPO withdrawal.Soc.Networks41,1–17.

Pavitt,C.,2014.Aninteractiveinput-process-outputmodelofsocialinfluencein decision-makinggroups.SmallGroupRes.45,704–730.

Plante,P.,Dumas,L.,Plante,A.2005.Sémato.Logicield’analysesémantiquedes documentstextuels.UQAM,p.Logicield’analysesémantiquedesdocuments textuels.

Poole,M.S.,DeSanctis,G.,1990.Understandingtheuseofdecisionsupport systems:thetheoryofadaptivestructuration.In:Ful,J.,Steinfield,C.(Eds.), OrganizationsandCommunicationTechnology.Sage,NewburyParkCA,pp. 175–195.

Postmes,T.,Haslam,S.A.,Swaab,R.I.,2005.Socialinfluenceinsmallgroups:an interactivemodelofsocialidentityformation.Eur.Rev.Soc.Psychol.16,1–42.

(10)

Prego-Vazquez,G.,2007.Frameconflictandsocialinequalityintheworkplace: professionalandlocaldiscoursestrugglesinemployee/customerinteractions. DiscourseSoc.18,295–335.

Rice,R.E.,1993.Usingnetworkconceptstoclarifysourcesandmechanismsof socialinfluence,in:W.Richards,J.,&G.Barnett(Ed.),Progressin

communicationsciences:Advancesincommunicationnetworkanalysis.Ablex Norwood,NJ,pp.43–52.

Robert,P.F.,Mongeau,P.,2014.Caractéristiquessociosémantiquesdesméthodes conventionnellesetalternativesdedélibération:communiquer.Revuede communicationsocialeetpublique,101–120.

Rorty,R.,1967.TheLinguisticTurn:RecentEssaysinPhilosophicalMethod. UniversityofChicagoPress,Chicago.

Roth,C.,Cointet,J.-P.,2010.Socialandsemanticcoevolutioninknowledge networks.Soc.Networks32,16–29.

Roth,C.,2008.Coévolutiondesauteursetdesconceptsdanslesréseaux épistémiques:lecasdelacommunauté«zebrafish».Revuefranc¸aisede sociologie49,523.

Roth,C.,2013.Socio-Semanticframeworks.Adv.ComplexSyst.WorldSci.,16. Roussiau,N.,Bonardi,C.,2001.LesReprésentationsSociales—ÉtatDesLieuxEt

Perspectives.Mardaga,Sprimont.

Saint-Charles,J.,Mongeau,P.,2009.Differentrelationshipsforcopingwith ambiguityanduncertaintyinorganizations.SocialNetworks,33–39. Saint-Charles,J.,Surette,C.,Parkes,M.W.,Morrison,K.E.,2014.Connectionsfor

health,ecosystemsandsocietyleadingtoactionandchange.Ecohealth11, 279–280.

Salk,J.E.,Brannen,M.Y.,2000.Researchnotes.nationalculture,networks,and individualinfluenceinamultinationalmanagementteam.Acad.Manage.J.43, 191–202.

Scherer,C.W.,Cho,H.,2003.Asocialnetworkcontagiontheoryofriskperception. RiskAnal.23,261–267.

Schutz,W.C.,1958.FIRO:AThree-DimensionalTheoryofInterpersonalBehavior. HoltRinehart,NewYork.

Simpson,J.A.,Farrell,A.K.,Ori ˜na,M.M.,Rothman,A.J.,2015.Powerandsocia.In: luencein,relationships.,in:Mikulincer,M.,Shaver,P.R.,Simpson,J.A.,Dovidio, J.F.(Eds.),APAHandbookofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,Volume3: InterpersonalRelations.AmericanPsychologicalAssociation,Washington,pp. 393–420.

Sparrowe,R.T.,Liden,R.C.,2005.Tworoutestoinfluence:integrating leader-Memberexchangeandsocialnetworkperspectives.Adm.Sci.Q.50, 505–535.

St-Arnaud,Y.,1978.In:Morin,G.(Ed.),Lespetitsgroupes:participationet communication,2002.

Teo,M.M.,Loosemore,M.,2011.Community-basedprotestagainstconstruction projects:acasestudyofmovementcontinuity.Constr.Manage.Econ.29, 131–144.

Thayer,L.,1988.Leadership/communication:acriticalreviewandamodest proposal.In:Goldhaber,G.M.,Barnett,G.A.(Eds.),HandbookofOrganizational Communication.AblexNorwood,NJ,pp.231–263.

Toft,A.,2014.Contestingthedeviantother:discursivestrategiesforthe productionofhomelesssubjectivities.DiscourseSoc.25,783–809. Treadway,D.C.,Breland,J.W.,Williams,L.M.,Cho,J.,Yang,J.,Ferris,G.R.,2013.

Socialinfluenceandinterpersonalpowerinorganizations:rolesof performanceandpoliticalskillintwostudies.J.Manage.39,1529–1553. Tuckman,B.W.,Jensen,M.A.C.,1977.Stagesofsmall-groupdevelopmentrevisited.

GroupOrganizationalStud.,419–427.

Tuckman,B.W.,1965.Developmentalsequencesinsmallgroups.Psychol.Bull., 384–399.

VanDijk,T.A.,1989.Structuresofdiscourseandstructuresofpower.In:Anderson, J.A.(Ed.),CommunicationYearbook12.Sage,NewburyParkCA,pp.18–59. Vargas,A.,Lo,A.Y.,Rohde,N.,Howes,M.,2016.Backgroundinequalityand

differentialparticipationindeliberativevaluation:lessonsfromsmall-group discussionsonforestconservationinColombia.Ecol.Econ.129,104–111. Wang,J.,2006.Questionsandtheexerciseofpower.DiscourseSoc.17,529–548. Wasserman,S.,Faust,K.,1994.SocialNetworkAnalysisMethodsandApplications.

CambridgeUniversityPress.

Westaby,J.D.,Woods,N.,Pfaff,D.L.,2016.Extendingdynamicnetworktheoryto groupandsocialinteractionanalysis:uncoveringkeybehavioralelements, cycles,andemergentstates.OrganizationalPsychol.Rev.6,34–62. Wheelan,S.A.,1994.GroupProcesses:ADevelopmentalPerspective.Allyn&

Bacon,Boston.

Wheelan,S.A.,2009.Groupsize,groupdevelopment,andgroupproductivity. SmallGroupRes.40,247–262.

Zanoni,P.,Janssens,M.,2015.Thepowerofdiversitydiscoursesatwork:onthe interlockingnatureofdiversitiesandoccupations.OrganizationStud.36, 1463–1483.

Zuchowski,I.E.,1987.CommunicationCompetenceandtheLiaisonRole.Central MichiganUniversity:DepartmentofSpeechCommunicationandDramatic arts.

Figure

Fig. 1. Mean discourse similarity according to reciprocity of influence tie strength.
Fig. 2. a, b, c. Influence valued centrality and five percentile groups of similarity indexes for three periods.

Références

Documents relatifs

Examining the number of sexual partners in the last 12 months showed a difference in behaviour (cf. table 6.2); men with multiple partners were two times less likely than those

Thanks to the prosopographical method and the networks’ analysis assets, concerning a long period (1800- 1940) we bring the proofs, that the Third French Republic (1871-1940)

Interestingly enough, Plato saw this quite clearly in his ‘‘Statesman,’’ where he noted that people are not sheep, and leaders are not shepherds; Plato regarded the leader as

L’expression de «ville nouvelle» recouvre deux intuitions distinctes. D’une part, la définition même de la ville, identifiée par sa population, ses fonctions, sa morphologie,

However, when relating to sustainability, the concept of environmental sustainability is usually targeted (e.g. [15]), while societal considerations are often

SATHYAMURTHY (T.V.) éd. — Social Change and Political Dis course in India. Structures of Power, Movements of Resistance. Comme l'explique T.V. Sathyamurthy, l'artisan

The above mentioned texts have looked into aided projects, programs and the social activities of the people to understand the impact of development whereas a work that examines

Les colorants sont des composés chimiques colorés, naturels ou synthétiques, en général organiques, qui ont la propriété de colorer durablement le support sur lequel ils sont