• Aucun résultat trouvé

Relations between childcare and language development among four-year-old neglected children: A cross-sectional study.

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Relations between childcare and language development among four-year-old neglected children: A cross-sectional study."

Copied!
1
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

(—THIS SIDEBAR DOES NOT PRINT—)

D E S I G N G U I D E

This PowerPoint 2007 template produces a

91cmx122cm presentation poster. You can use it

to create your research poster and save valuable

time placing titles, subtitles, text, and graphics.

We provide a series of online tutorials that will

guide you through the poster design process and

answer your poster production questions. To view

our template tutorials, go online to

PosterPresentations.com

and click on HELP DESK.

When you are ready to print your poster, go online

to PosterPresentations.com

Need assistance? Call us at

1.510.649.3001

Q U I C K S TA RT

Zoom in and out

As you work on your poster zoom in and out to the level that is more comfortable to

you. Go to VIEW > ZOOM.

Title, Authors, and Affiliations

Start designing your poster by adding the title, the

names of the authors, and the affiliated institutions. You can type or paste text into the provided boxes. The

template will automatically adjust the size of your text to fit the title box. You can manually override this

feature and change the size of your text.

TIP: The font size of your title should be bigger than

your name(s) and institution name(s).

Adding Logos / Seals

Most often, logos are added on each side of the title. You can insert a logo by dragging and dropping it from your desktop, copy and paste or by going to INSERT > PICTURES. Logos taken from web sites are likely to be low quality when printed. Zoom it at 100% to see what the logo will look like on the final poster and make any necessary adjustments.

TIP: See if your school’s logo is available on our free

poster templates page.

Photographs / Graphics

You can add images by dragging and dropping from your desktop, copy and paste, or by going to INSERT >

PICTURES. Resize images proportionally by holding down the SHIFT key and dragging one of the corner handles. For a professional-looking poster, do not distort your images by enlarging them disproportionally.

Image Quality Check

Zoom in and look at your images at 100% magnification. If they look good they will print well.

ORIGINAL DISTORTED Corner handles Good pr in /n g qu al ity Bad pr in /n g qu al ity

Q U I C K S TA RT ( c o n t . )

How to change the template color theme

You can easily change the color theme of your poster by going to the DESIGN menu, click on COLORS, and choose the color theme of your choice. You can also create your own color theme.

You can also manually change the color of your

background by going to VIEW > SLIDE MASTER. After you finish working on the master be sure to go to VIEW >

NORMAL to continue working on your poster.

How to add Text

The template comes with a number of pre-formatted placeholders for

headers and text blocks. You can add more blocks by copying and pasting the existing ones or by adding a text box from the HOME menu.

Text size

Adjust the size of your text based on how much content you have to present.

The default template text offers a good starting point. Follow the conference requirements.

How to add Tables

To add a table from scratch go to the INSERT menu and click on TABLE. A drop-down box will help you select rows and columns.

You can also copy and a paste a table from Word or

another PowerPoint document. A pasted table may need to be re-formatted by RIGHT-CLICK > FORMAT SHAPE,

TEXT BOX, Margins.

Graphs / Charts

You can simply copy and paste charts and graphs from Excel or Word. Some reformatting may be required

depending on how the original document has been created.

How to change the column configuration

RIGHT-CLICK on the poster background and select LAYOUT to see the column options available for this

template. The poster columns can also be customized on the Master. VIEW > MASTER.

How to remove the info bars

If you are working in PowerPoint for Windows and have finished your poster, save as PDF and the bars will not be included. You can also delete them by going to VIEW >

MASTER. On the Mac adjust the Page-Setup to match the Page-Setup in PowerPoint before you create a PDF. You can also delete them from the Slide Master.

Save your work

Save your template as a PowerPoint document. For printing, save as PowerPoint or “Print-quality” PDF.

Print your poster

When you are ready to have your poster printed go online to PosterPresentations.com and click on the

“Order Your Poster” button. Choose the poster type the best suits your needs and submit your order. If you

submit a PowerPoint document you will be receiving a PDF proof for your approval prior to printing. If your order is placed and paid for before noon, Pacific,

Monday through Friday, your order will ship out that same day. Next day, Second day, Third day, and Free Ground services are offered. Go to

PosterPresentations.com for more information.

Student discounts are available on our Facebook page. Go to PosterPresentations.com and click on the FB icon.

©2015 PosterPresenta/ons.com

2117 Fourth Street , Unit C Berkeley CA 94710

posterpresenter@gmail.com

RESEARCH POSTER PRESENTATION DESIGN © 2015

www.PosterPresentations.com

Neglect : deficit in meeting a child's basic needs, including the failure to provide basical physical/

health care and educational needs (LPJ, 2007)

Language is the most compromised area among neglected children (McDonald et al., 2013), since its

development requires notably warm adult-child interactions and high-quality language input (Evans, 2001; Poll, 2011)

•  41,7 % of neglected children have a significant language delay at age 3 (Sylvestre & Mérette, 2010)

•  More precisely, starting at age 2 ½, neglected children produce shorter utterances then their non neglected peers (Beeghly & Cicchetti, 1994; Eigsti & Cicchetti, 2004), which is very worrying

considering that mean length of utterance [MLU] is a strong predictor of reading skills (Bishop & Adams, 1990; Durand et al., 2013).

High-quality early childhood education [ECE] settings can help enhance language skills of

young children as they are characterized by rich learning opportunities, stimulating material and positive interactions (Weiland et al., 2013).

•  Among components of ECE experience, quality of interactions, as measured by the

Classroom Assessment Scoring System ([CLASS] Pianta et al., 2008) is the strongest predictor of

language skills (Sabol et al., 2013).

•  Very few researchers have investigated the relationship between ECE experience and the language skills of neglected children (Dinehart et al., 2012; Kovan et al., 2014; Merrit & Klein, 2015), and none have specifically considered the quality of interactions. Previous research in this area has

mostly considered ECE attendance in neglected children.

CONTEXT

AIMS OF THE CURRENT STUDY

RESULTS

Portrait of Quality of Interactions in ECE Settings

By domains… By dimensions…

In the weakest domain (Instructional Support), concept development (M = 1.98, SD = 0.63), which

refers to the integration of new concepts to previous activities/connections to real world and the support of high-order cognitive and language skills, such as predicting and reasoning, represents the dimension where the level of quality is the lowest.

Linear regression (n=23*) analysis was used to test if the domains of the quality of interactions predicted neglected children’s MLU. Results of the regression indicate that the three domains are not significant predictors of MLU (R2=.067, F(3,19)=.453, p>.05).

*One participant was excluded from the linear regression analysis because he did not produce the minimum of 25 utterances required for the measure of MLU during the standardized play.

DISCUSSION & CONCLUSION

Our results regarding the level of the quality of interactions in ECE settings are consistent with previous local and international research on quality of interactions in pre-kindergarten and kindergarten (e.g.

Bouchard et al., 2017; Duval et al., 2016; Early et al., 2017; Hu et al. 2016; Pianta et al., 2008; von Suchodoletz et al., 2014).

We identified two main hypotheses to explain the absence of significiance in our study, other than sample size.

First, conceptually closer instruments to measured outcomes, in this case language, could be

preferred (Zaslow et al., 2016). For example, a measure of the language stimulation techniques provided by

the caregiver could be considered.

Secondly, according to different authors (Burchinal et al., 2011; Weiland et al., 2013), the relationship between the

quality of ECE settings and the outcomes on child’s development is stronger when the quality level is within the high range.

•  In our study, Instructional Support, which is known to best support children’s language skills (Logan et al., 2011), ranges in the low level of quality.

•  A minimal level of quality of interactions must be reached in order to observe a positive impact on language skills (Weiland et al., 2013). Some researchers suggest that this minimal level of quality is

3.25 for the domain of Instructional Support (Burchinal et al., 2010). In our study, only 8.3% (n=2) of the

ECE settings met this criterion.

•  A slight improvement on the scale of quality of interactions allows to observe significant effects on children’s language skills (National Center on Quality Teaching and Learning, 2013).

•  The professional development of caregivers is thus an interesting research path to pursue to reach this minimum threshold for the domain of Instructional Support. Caregivers working with neglected children, whose language difficulties are of great concern, should be targeted for this intervention.

According to these findings, subsequent studies, conducted on a wider sample, will provide further insights into the relationship between language skills of neglected children and their ECE experience.

•  Association des Centres jeunesse du Québec. (2016). Les mauvais traitements psychologiques, un mal silencieux: bilan des directeurs de la protection de la jeunesse/directeurs provinciaux 2016. Retrieved from

https://www.inesss.qc.ca/fileadmin/doc/INESSS/ServicesSociaux/Bilan_DPJ/INESSS_Bilan_DPJ_Mauvais_traitements_psycho_2016.pdf

•  Blumenthal, A. (2015). Child Neglect I : Scope, consequences, and risk and protective factors. Canadian Child Welfare Research Portal, 141(E), 1-9. •  Eigsti, I-M. et Cicchetti, D. (2004). The impact of child maltreatment on expressive syntax at 60 months. Developmental Science, 7(1), 88-102.

•  Laurin, I., Guay, D., Bigras, N. & Fournier, M. (2015). Quel est l'effet de la fréquentation d'un service éducatif sur le développement de l'enfant à la maternelle selon le statut socioéconomique? Résultats de l'Enquête montréalaise sur l'expérience

préscolaire des enfants de maternelle (EMEP, 2012), (Fascicule 2 - Mars 2015). Montréal, Canada: Agence de la santé et des services sociaux de Montréal.

•  McDonald, J. L., Milne, S., Knight, J., & Webster, V. (2013). Developmental and behavioural characteristics of children enrolled in a child protection pre-school. J Paediatr Child Health, 49(2), E142-146. doi:10.1111/jpc.12029 •  Observatoire des tout-petits. (2017). Violence et maltraitance: Les tout-petits québécois sont-ils à l'abri?. Retrieved from http://tout-petits.org/media/1418/rapportcomplet_maltraitance_observatoiredestoutpetits.pdf

•  Pianta, R. C., La Paro, K. M. & Hamre, B. K. (2008). The Classroom Assessment Scoring System. Manual Pre-K. Baltimore, MA: Brookes.

•  Sylvestre, A. et Mérette, C. (2010). Language delay in severely neglected children: A cumulative or specific effect of risk factors?. Child Abuse and Neglect, 34, 414-428.

•  Von Suchodoletz, A. V., Fäsche, A., Gunzenhauser, C. & Hamre, B. K. (2014). A typical morning in preschool: Observations of teacher–child interactions in German preschools. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 509-519.

•  Weiland, C., Ulvestad, K., Sachs, J., & Yoshikawa, H. (2013). Associations between classroom quality and children’s vocabulary and executive function skills in an urban public prekindergarten program. Early Childhood Reserah Quarterly, 28(2), 199-209. doi : 10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.12.002

SELECTED REFERENCES

Relationship between quality of interactions in ECE settings

and language skills of neglected 4-year-olds :

Preliminary results

JULIEN, Catherine

1,2,3,4,5

, Ph.D. candidate,

BOUCHARD, Caroline

1,3,4,5

, Ph.D., &

SYLVESTRE, Audette

1,2,4,5

, Ph.D.

1Université Laval, 2Centre interdisciplinaire de recherche en réadaptation et intégration sociale, 3Centre de recherche et d’intervention sur la réussite scolaire, 4Centre de recherche sur les jeunes et les familles, 5Équipe de recherche Qualité des contextes éducatifs de la petite enfance

EECERA 27

th

Conference, Bologna (Italy)

Canada Annual incidents of 10 per 1000 children aged under 18 years old in 2008 (Blumenthal, 2015) Quebec Annual incidents of 7,1 per 1000 children

aged under 6 years old Nearly 5,000 neglected children in 2015-2016

(ACJQ, 2016; Observatoire des tout-petits, 2017)

①  Describe the quality of interactions in ECE settings frequented by 4 year-old neglected children

②  Examine the relationship between the domains of quality of interactions and language skills, as measured by MLU, among the population of 4 year-old

neglected children

METHOD

Recruitment in 4 Child Welfare Services in the Province of Quebec, including 2 University

affiliated centers

•  24 French-speaking neglected children (M = 48.3 months, SD = 0.39)

Exclusion : children presenting a biological condition associated with language disorder (e.g. autism, deafness)

PROCEDURE INSTRUMENTS STEP 1

60 min. in-home visit by a research

assistant

STEP 2

2-hour observation in ECE

setting by a certified observer Standardized play Jeu de village (Sylvestre et al., 2014) to collect a spontaneous language sample to measure MLU CLASS Pre-K (Pianta, La Paro, Hamre, 2008) : observational measure of the quality of teacher-child interactions

1

2

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Contact information : catherine.julien.5@ulaval.ca

5,92 1,56 5,84 5,49 5,68 6,22 5,06 1,98 2,73 3,15 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Positive climate Negative climate Teacher sensitivity

Regard for student perspective

Behavior management Productivity

Instructional learning formats Concept development

Quality of feedback

Language modeling

  Neglected children % (n)

Sex (male) 75.0 (18)

Non intact family 58.3 (14)

Family living under level of poverty (Quebec, 2014) 50.0 (12)

Level of education of principal parental figure < 12 years 50.0 (12)

MLU (words) under clinical treshold (< 3,3 words) 37.5 (9)

Quality level Low (1-2) Middle (3-4-5) High (6-7)

The CLASS evaluates 10 dimensions of the quality of interactions which are grouped into 3 domains. Emotional Support Positive climate Negative climate Teacher sensitivity Regard for student

perspectives Classroom Organization Behavior management Productivity Instructional learning formats Instructional Support Concept development Quality of feedback Language modeling 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Emotional Support Classroom Organization Instructional Support

5.92

(SD=0.52) 5.65

(SD=0.58)

2.62

Références

Documents relatifs

Children and adolescents whose parents held a middle or high educational title were 80% more likely to practice moderate or vigorous physical activity than subjects whose parents had

The second wound sample is, in terms of voltage taps, a fully-instrumented single pancake which allows recording the transition and its propagation inside the winding

The Child Food Rejection Scale: Development and validation of a new scale to assess food neophobia and pickiness among 2- to 7-year-old French children... The two strongest obstacles

Using a sorting procedure, Bovet and Vauclair (2001) observed that at age 3 the majority of the children successfully sorted food and toys pictures into different boxes, thus

We performed a clustering of gamblers with eight variables: age at initiation, age at onset of gambling problem, duration of gambling history, age at diagnosis of

Program a maximum simulated likelihood estimation procedure to estimate

Genericness of database schema can allow intercomparison of CSM (AgMIP) that require the same datasets with no common data structure.. DAPHNE has a wide application in pest