• Aucun résultat trouvé

6. Conclusions générales

6.2 Études futures

D’autres études devraient être faites avec un plus grand échantillon afin de pouvoir constituer des sous-groupes plus homogènes d’enfants sourds oralistes et de voir si les résultats de la présente étude se maintiennent pour tous les sous-groupes. De plus, les études futures devraient tenter d’élucider par quel mécanisme le niveau de langage peut entraîner des difficultés comportementales chez les enfants sourds. Il se peut que ces mécanismes soient différents de ceux qui sont documentés pour les enfants entendants. Par exemple, il serait intéressant d’examiner l’influence que pourrait avoir l’attention sur les relations entre l’étendue du vocabulaire et les problèmes de comportement. En effet, Barker et al. (2009) ont trouvé que le langage était associé à la capacité d’attention soutenue et aux difficultés comportementales chez les enfants sourds. Les auteurs expliquent cette association par le rôle que peut jouer le langage sur la régulation de l’attention. L’attention soutenue était, à son tour, corrélée aux comportements internalisés et externalisés dans leur étude, ce qui suggère que la régulation interne est un mécanisme par lequel le langage peut exercer son influence sur les problèmes de comportement. Une mesure qui pourrait être utilisée pour évaluer l’attention soutenue est le CPT (Continuous Performance Test), qui s’administre aussi bien aux enfants sourds qu’aux enfants entendants.

De plus, il serait important d’examiner comment l’implication du parent pourrait influencer les scores obtenus aux tests langagiers et indirectement, les mesures comportementales et socioémotionnelles. En effet, l’impact de la surdité sur la communication parent-enfant a été identifié comme étant un des facteurs qui contribuent le plus à la présence de problèmes comportementaux et émotionnels chez les enfants sourds (Meadow, 1975; Schlesinger & Meadow, 1972). À ce sujet, Van Eldik et ses collègues (2004) ont observé que les enfants sourds qui éprouvaient plus de difficultés à communiquer avec leurs parents (selon ces derniers) avaient des résultats plus élevés à l’échelle Total Problems du CBCL comparativement aux autres enfants sourds. Il se pourrait donc que la communication parent-enfant joue un rôle médiateur quant au lien entre les difficultés langagières des enfants sourds oralistes et leur fréquence plus élevée de comportements extériorisés. Il serait également intéressant d’examiner l’impact de l’implication des parents dans le développement socio-affectif et langagier de leur enfant sourd.

57 En somme, cette étude vient corroborer les études antérieures qui ont montré une incidence plus élevée de comportements extériorisés chez les enfants sourds par rapport aux enfants entendants et elle montre que cette incidence s’explique par le niveau langagier plus limité des enfants sourds. Il reste maintenant à comprendre comment ce lien se développe, mais aussi surtout, comment mettre en place les services nécessaires pour éviter qu’une surdité entraîne une cascade d’autres difficultés développementales.

59

Bibliographie

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). National Survey of Problems and Competencies among Four- to Sixteen-Year Olds: Parents' Reports for Normative and Clinical Samples. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 56(3), 1-120.

Arnold, S., Barrington, K., Breton, D., Gendron, M., Hurteau, A.-M., Janvier, A. (2007).

Le dépistage de la surdité chez le nouveau-né : Évaluation des avantages, des inconvénients et des coûts de son implantation au Québec. Récupéré sur http://www.inspq.qc.ca.

Baker, L., & Cantwell, D. P. (1987). A prospective psychiatric follow-up of children with speech/language disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 26(4), 546-553. Barker, D. H., Quittner, A. L., Fink, N. E., Eisenberg, L. S., Tobey, E. A., & Niparko, J. K. (2009). Predicting

behavior problems in deaf and hearing children: the influences of language, attention, and parent- child communication. Development and Psychopathology, 21(2), 373-392. doi:

10.1017/S0954579409000212

Bat-Chava, Y., Martin, D., & Kosciw, J. G. (2005). Longitudinal improvements in communication and

socialization of deaf children with cochlear implants and hearing aids: evidence from parental reports. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(12), 1287-1296.

Bates, E., Dale, P., & Thal, D. (1995). Individual differences and their implication for theories of language development. Dans P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Handbook of Child Language. Oxford: Blackwell.

Berk, S. B. (2003). Sensitive Period Effects on the Acquisition of Language : A Study of Language

Development. (Ph.D.), University of Connecticut, Connecticut. ProQuest UMI Dissertation Publishing database.

Berthelot, M., Camirand, J., Tremblay, R., & Cardin, J.-F. (2006). L'incapacité et les limitations d'activités au Québec : un portrait statistique à partir des données de l'Enquête sur la participation et les limitations d'activités 2001 (EPLA). Québec: Récupéré sur

http://www.stat.gouv.qc.ca/publications/sante/pdf2006/incapacite_limitations2001.pdf.

Blamey, P. J., Sarant, J. Z., Paatsch, L. E., Barry, J. G., Bow, C. P., Wales, R. J. (2001). Relationships among speech perception, production, language, hearing loss, and age in children with impaired hearing. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(2), 264-285. doi: 10.1044/1092- 4388(2001/022)

Calderon, R. (2000). Parental involvement in deaf children's education programs as a predictor of child's language, early reading, and social-emotional development. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 5(2), 140-155. doi: 10.1093/deafed/5.2.140

Calderon, R., & Greenberg, M. T. (2003). Social and and emotional development of deaf children: family, school and program effects. Dans M. Marschark & P. Spencer (Eds.), Deaf studies, language and education (pp. 177-189). New York: Oxford University Press.

Calderon, R., & Naidu, S. (1999). Further Support for the Benefits of Early Identification and Intervention for Children with Hearing Loss. Volta Review, 100(5), 53-84.

Camarata, S. M., Hugues, C. A., & Ruhl, K. L. (1988). Mild/Moderate Behaviorally Disordered Students : A Population at Risk for Language Disorders. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 19(2), 191-200.

Cantwell, D. P., & Baker, L. (1987a). Prevalence and type of psychiatric disorder and developmental disorders in three speech and language groups. Journal of Communication Disorders, 20(2), 151-160. doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(87)90006-2

Cantwell, D. P., & Baker, L. (1987b). Psychiatric symptomatology in language-impaired children: A comparison. Journal of Child Neurology, 2(2), 128-133.

Christiansen, J. B., & Leigh, I. W. (2002). Cochlear implants in children : ethics and choices. Washington, D.C.: Gallaudet University Press.

Cohen, N. J. (2002). Developmental language disorders. Dans P. Howlin & O. Udwin (Eds.), Outcomes in neurodevelopmental and genetic disorders (pp. 26-55). New York: Cambridge University Press. Connor, C. M., Craig, H. K., Raudenbush, S. W., Heavner, K., & Zwolan, T. A. (2006). The age at which young

deaf children receive cochlear implants and their vocabulary and speech-production growth: is there an added value for early implantation? Ear Hear, 27(6), 628-644. doi:

10.1097/01.aud.0000240640.59205.42

Connor, C. M., Hieber, S., Arts, H. A., & Zwolan, T. A. (2000). Speech, vocabulary, and the education of children using cochlear implants: oral or total communication? Journal of Speech Language Hearing Research, 43(5), 1185-1204.

Culp, R. E., Watkins, R. V., Lawrence, H., Letts, D., & et al. (1991). Maltreated children's language and speech development: Abused, neglected, and abused and neglected. First Language, 11(33, Pt 3), 377-389. doi: 10.1177/014272379101103305

Curtiss, S. (1977). Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern Day "Wild Child". New York: Academic Press. Daigle, D. (1998). Méthodes et philosophies d’enseignement Lecture, écriture et surdité : visions actuelles et

nouvelles perspectives (pp. 27-43). Montréal: Éditions Logiques.

Dale, P. S., & Goodman, J. C. (2005). Commonality and Individual Differences in Vocabulary Growth. Dans M. Tomasello & D. I. Slobin (Eds.), Beyond nature-nurture: Essays in honor of Elizabeth Bates. (pp. 41- 78): Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Dale, P. S., Simonoff, E., Bishop, D. V. M., Eley, T. C., Oliver, B., Price, T. S. (1998). Genetic influence on language delay in two-year-old children. Nature Neuroscience, 1(4), 324-328. doi: 10.1038/1142 Desrosiers, H., & Ducharme, A. (2006). Commencer l'école du bon pied. Facteurs associés à l'acquisition du

vocabulaire à la fin de la maternelle Étude longitudinale du développement des enfants du Québec (ÉLDEQ 1998-2010) (Vol. 1, fascicule 4). Québec: Institut de la statistique Québec.

Dionne, G. (2005). Language Development and Aggressive Behavior. Dans R. E. Tremblay, W. W. Hartup & J. Archer (Eds.), Determinants of Aggression (pp. 330-352). New York, NY, États-Unis: Guilford Press. Ducharme, D. A., & Mayberry, R. I. (2005). L'importance d'une exposition précoce au langage : la période

critique s'applique au langage signé tout comme au langage oral. Dans C. Transler, J. Leybaert & J.- É. Gombert (Eds.), L'acquisition du langage par l'enfant sourd : les signes, l'oral et l'écrit (pp. 15-28). Marseille: Solal.

Duchesne, L., Sutton, A., & Bergeron, F. (2009). Language achievement in children who received cochlear implants between 1 and 2 years of age: Group trends and individual patterns. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14(4), 465-485. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enp010

Dunn, L. M., Thériault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Échelle de vocabulaire en images Peabody (EVIP). Adaptation française du Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised, Manuel pour les formes A et B. Toronto: Psycan.

Durieux-Smith, A., Fitzpatrick, E., & Whittingham, J. (2008). Universal newborn hearing screening: A question of evidence. International Journal of Audiology, 47(1), 1-10. doi: 10.1080/14992020701703547 Eisenberg, L. S., Kirk, K. I., Martinez, A. S., Ying, E. A., & Miyamoto, R. T. (2004). Communication abilities of

children with aided residual hearing: comparison with cochlear implant users. Archives of Otolaryngology-Head & Neck Surgery, 130(5), 563-569. doi: 10.1001/archotol.130.5.563

El-Hakim, H., Papsin, B., Mount, R. J., Levasseur, J., Panesar, J., Stevens, D. (2001). Vocabulary acquisition rate after pediatric cochlear implantation and the impact of age at implantation. International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 59(3), 187-194. doi: 10.1016/S0165-5876(01)00481-5

Erber, N. P. (1974). Visual perception of speech by deaf children: Recent developments and continuing needs. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 39(2), 178-185.

Fagan, M. K., & Pisoni, D. B. (2010). Hearing experience and receptive vocabulary development in deaf children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 15(2), 149-161. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enq001

Fagan, M. K., Pisoni, D. B., Horn, D. L., & Dillon, C. M. (2007). Neuropsychological correlates of vocabulary, reading, and working memory in deaf children with cochlear implants. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 12(4), 461-471. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enm023

61 Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. (1993). Mac Arthur Communicative

Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publications. Fried, P. A., Watkinson, B., & Siegel, L. S. (1997). Reading and language in 9- to 12- year olds prenatally

exposed to cigarettes and marijuana. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 19(3), 171-183. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(97)00015-9

Fu, Q.-J., Shannon, R. V., & Wang, X. (1998). Effects of noise and spectral resolution on vowel and consonant recognition: Acoustic and electric hearing. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 104(6), 3586-3596.

Gallagher, T. M. (1999). Interrelationships among Children's Language, Behavior, and Emotional Problems. Topics in Language Disorders, 19(2), 1-15.

Gardner, M. (2000). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test. Novato, CA. États-Unis: Academic Therapy.

Geers, A. E., & Moog, J. (1994). Spoken language results: Vocabulary, syntax, and communication. The Volta Review, 96(5), 131-148.

Geers, A. E., Moog, J. S., Biedenstein, J., Brenner, C., & Hayes, H. (2009). Spoken language scores of children using cochlear implants compared to hearing age-mates at school entry. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14(3), 371-385. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enn046

Geers, A. E., Nicholas, J. G., & Moog, J. S. (2007). Estimating the influence of cochlear implantation on language development in children. Audiological Medicine, 5(4), 262-273. doi:

10.1080/16513860701659404

Gertner, B. L., & Rice, M. L. (1994). Influence of communicative competence on peer preferences in a preschool classroom. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 37(4), 913-923.

Goodman, R., Simonoff, E., & Stevenson, J. (1995). The impact of child IQ, parent IQ and sibling IQ on child behavioural deviance scores. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(3), 409-425. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01299.x

Greenberg, M. T., & Kusche, C. A. (1993). Promoting Social and Emotional Development in Deaf Children. The PATHS Project. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

Gregory, S., & Mogford, K. (1981). Early language development in deaf children. Dans B. Woll, J. Kyle & M. Deuchar (Eds.), Perspectives on British Sign Language and deafness. London: Croom Helm. Hayes, H., Geers, A. E., Treiman, R., & Moog, J. S. (2009). Receptive vocabulary development in deaf

children with cochlear implants: achievement in an intensive auditory-oral educational setting. Ear Hear, 30(1), 128-135. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181926524

Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Longitudinal genetic study of verbal and nonverbal IQ from early childhood to young adulthood. Learning and Individual Differences, 17(2), 97-114. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.05.005

Hogg, C., Rutter, M., & Richman, N. (1997). Revised Rutter Teacher Scale for School-Age Children. Dans I. Sclare (Ed.), Child Psychology Portfolio. Londres: nferNelson.

Hyde, M. L. (2005). Newborn Hearing Screening Programs : Overview. The journal of Otolaryngology, 34(Suppl. 2), S70-S78.

Ibertsson, T., Hansson, K., Maki-Torkko, E., Willstedt-Svensson, U., & Sahlen, B. (2009). Deaf teenagers with cochlear implants in conversation with hearing peers. International Journal of Language &

Communication Disorders, 44(3), 319-337. doi: 10.1080/13682820802052067 Itard, J. M. G. (1932). The wild boy of Aveyron. New York: Century.

Jensema, C., & Trybus, R. J. (1975). Reported Emotional/Behavioral Problems Among Hearing Impaired Children in Special Educational Programs. Washington, DC, États-Unis: Gallaudet College, Office of Demographic Studies.

Jerome, A. C., Fujiki, M., Brinton, B., & James, S. L. (2002). Self-esteem in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(4), 700-714. doi:

Kennedy, C. R., McCann, D., Campbell, M. J., Kimm, L., & Thornton, R. (2005). Universal newborn screening for permanent childhood hearing impairment: An 8-year follow-up of a controlled trial. Lancet, 366(9486), 660-662. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(05)67138-3

Kennedy, C. R., McCann, D. C., Campbell, M. J., Law, C. M., Mullee, M., Petrou, S. (2006). Language Ability after Early Detection of Permanent Childhood Hearing Impairment. New England Journal of Medicine, 354(20), 2131-2141. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa054915

Kirk, K. I., Miyamoto, R. T., Ying, E. A., Perdew, A. E., & Zuganelis, H. (2000). Cochlear implantation in young children: Effects of age at implantation and communication mode. The Volta Review, 102(4), 127- 144.

La.Presse.Canadienne. (17 juillet 2009). Dépistage universel de la surdité chez les nouveaux-nés, Le Devoir. Récupéré sur http://www.ledevoir.com/societe/sante/259355/depistage-universel-de-la-surdite-chez- les-nouveau-nes

Le Normand, M. T., Parisse, C., & Cohen, H. (2008). Lexical diversity and productivity in French preschoolers: Developmental, Gender and Sociocultural factors. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 22, 47-58. Leigh, G. (2008). Changing parameters in deafness and deaf education: Greater opportunity but continuing

diversity Deaf cognition: Foundations and outcomes. (pp. 24-51): New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.

Leybaert, J., & D'Hondt, M. (2003). Neurolinguistic development in deaf children: the effect of early language experience. International Journal of Audiology, 42(Suppl 1), S34-40.

Lindsay, G., & Dockrell, J. (2000). The behaviour and self-esteem of children with specific speech and language difficulties. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(4), 583-601. doi:

10.1348/000709900158317

Luria, A. R. (1967). Neuropsychology and Its Significance for Behavioral Sciences and Medicine. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 10(1), 1-6.

Maller, S. J. (2003). Intellectual assessment of deaf people: A critical review of core concepts and issues Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education. (pp. 451-463): New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.

Mason, A., & Mason, M. (2007). Psychologic Impact of Deafness on the Child and Adolescent. Primary Care: Clinics in Office Practice, 34, 407-426.

Mayne, A. M., Yoshinaga-Itano, C., & Sedey, A. L. (1998). Receptive Vocabulary Development of Infants and Toddlers Who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing. Volta Review, 100(5), 29-52.

Mayne, A. M., Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Sedey, A. L., & Carey, A. (1998). Expressive Vocabulary Development of Infants and Toddlers Who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing. Volta Review, 100(5), 1-28.

Meadow, K. P. (1975). The development of deaf children Hetherington, E. M. (1975). Review of child development research. Oxford, England: University of Chicago Press.

Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. A. (2004). Chasing the Mythical Ten Percent: Parental Hearing Status of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students in the United States. Sign Language Studies, 4(2), 138-163.

Miyamoto, R. T., Kirk, K. I., Svirsky, M. A., & Sehgal, S. T. (1999). Communication skills in pediatric cochlear implant recipients. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, Vo1 119(2), 219-224. doi: 10.1080/00016489950181701 Moeller, M. P. (2000). Early intervention and language development in children who are deaf and hard of

hearing. Pediatrics, 106(3), E43.

Moeller, M. P., Tomblin, J. B., Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Connor, C. M., & Jerger, S. (2007). Current state of knowledge : language and literacy of children with hearing impairment. Ear Hear, 28(6), 740-753. Naarden, K. V., Decouflé, P., & Caldwell, K. (1999). Prevalence and characteristics of children with serious

hearing impairment in metropolitan Atlanta, 1991-1993. Pediatrics, 103(3), 570-575.

Newport, E. L. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(1), 11-28.

NIH Consensus Statement. (1993, March 1-3). Early identification of Hearing Impairment in Infants and Young Children.

Osberger, M. J. (1986). Language and Learning Skills of Hearing-Impaired Students : ASHA Monographs (Vol. 23). Rockville, MD, États-Unis: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.

63 Oxford, M., & Spieker, S. (2006). Preschool Language Development among Children of Adolescent Mothers.

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 165-182.

Padden, C., & Ramsey, C. (2005). La langue des signes Américaine (ASL) et l’aptitude des enfants sourds à la lecture. Dans C. Transler, J. Leybaert & J.-É. Gombert (Eds.), L'acquisition du langage par l'enfant sourd: Les signes, l'oral et l'écrit (pp. 291-315). Marseille: Solal.

Périer, O. (1987). L'enfant à audition déficiente. Aspects médicaux, éducatifs, sociologiques et psychologiques. Acta Oto-Rhino-Laryngologica Belgica, 41, 125-420.

Picard, M. (1999). Étude rétrospective de l'audition chez les enfants avec problème auditif en fonction de l'âge, de la cause de surdité et de la durée de port des aides de correction auditive. Montréal, Québec: Étude de l'AQEPA (Association des enfants avec problèmes auditifs) avec la participation de l'Institut Raymond-Dewar et de l'Université de Montréal.

Robinshaw, H. M. (1995). Early intervention for hearing impairment: differences in the timing of communicative and linguistic development. British Journal of Audiology, 29, 315-334.

Rosenblum, L., Schmuckler, M. A., & Johnson, J. A. (1997). The McGurk effect in infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(3), 347-357.

Sarachan-Deily, A. B., & Love, R. J. (1974). Underlying grammatical rule structure in the deaf. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 17(4), 689-698.

Sattler, J. M. (1992). Assessment of children's intelligence Walker, Clarence Eugene; Roberts, Michael C (pp. Handbook of clinical child psychology (2nd ed.). Wiley series on personality processes. (pp. 85-100). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons. xx, 1145).

Schirmer, B. R. (2001). Psychological, social, and educational dimensions of deafness. Needham Heights, MA, US: Allyn & Bacon.

Schlesinger, H. S., & Meadow, K. (1972). Sound and sign. Childhood deafness and mental health. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

Scholes, R. J., Cohen, M., & Brumfield, S. (1978). Some possible causes of syntactic deficits in the congenitally deaf English user. American Annals of the Deaf, 123(5), 528-535.

Spencer, P. E. (2004). Individual differences in language performance after cochlear implantation at one to three years of age: child, family, and linguistic factor. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 9(4), 395-412. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enh033

Stevenson, J., McCann, D., Watkin, P., Worsfold, S., & Kennedy, C. (2010). The relationship between language development and behaviour problems in children with hearing loss. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(1), 77-83. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02124.x

Stokoe, W. C. (1960). Sign Language Structure : An Outline of the Visual Communication Systems of the American Deaf Studies in Linguistics : Occasional papers (No. 8). Buffalo: Department of Anthropology and Linguistics, University of Buffalo.

Taeschner, T., Devescovi, A., & Volterra, V. (1988). Affixes and function words in the written language of deaf children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9(4), 385-401.

Taylor, R. S., Paisley, S., & Davis, A. (2001). Systematic review of the clinical and cost effectiveness of digital hearing aids. British Journal of Audiology, 35(5), 271-288.

Thal, D., DesJardin, J. L., & Eisenberg, L. S. (2007). Validity of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories for measuring language abilities in children with cochlear implants. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16(1), 54-64. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2007/007) Tomblin, J. B., Spencer, L., Flock, S., Tyler, R., & Gantz, B. (1999). A Comparison of Language Achievement in Children with Cochlear Implants and Children Using Hearing Aids. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(2), 497-511.

Tremblay, R. E., Desmarais-Gervais, L., Gagnon, C., & Charlebois, P. (1987). The Preschool Behavior Questionnaire: stability of its factor structure between cultures, sexes, ages and socioeconomic classes. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 10(4), 467-484.

Van Eldik, T., Treffers, P. D. A., Veerman, J. W., & Verhulst, F. C. (2004). Mental Health Problems of Deaf Dutch Children As Indicated by Parents' Responses to the Child Behavior Checklist. American Annals of the Deaf, 148(5), 390-394.

Verhulst, F. C., Van der Ende, J., & Koot, H. M. (1997). Handleiding voor de CBCL/4-18 [Manual for the CBCL/4-18]. Rotterdam, Netherlands: Erasmus University, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

Vostanis, P., Hayes, M., Du Feu, M., & Warren, J. (1997). Detection of behavioural and emotional problems in deaf children and adolescents: Comparison of two rating scales. Child: Care, Health and

Development, 23(3), 233-246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.1997.tb00966.x

Vygotsky, L. S. (1988). On Inner Speech. Dans M. B. Franklin & S. S. Barten (Eds.), Child Language : A Reader (pp. 181-187). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Wake, M., Hughes, E. K., Poulakis, Z., Collins, C., & Rickards, F. W. (2004). Outcomes of children with mild- profound congenital hearing loss at 7 to 8 years: a population study. Ear Hear, 25(1), 1-8. doi: 10.1097/01.AUD.0000111262.12219.2F

Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence – Third Edition (WPPSI – III): Pearson Education.

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children — Fourth Edition (WISC-IV): Pearson. Williams, K. T. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test. Bloomington, MN: Pearson Education, Inc.

Willinger, U., Brunner, E., Diendorfer-Radner, G., Mag, J. S., Sirsch, U., & Elsenwort, B. (2003). Behaviour in Children with Language Development Disorders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 48(9), 607-614. Winsler, A., De León, J. R., Wallace, B. A., Carlton, M. P., & Willson-Quayle, A. (2003). Private speech in

preschool children: Developmental stability and change, across-task consistency, and relations with classroom behaviour. Journal of Child Language, 30(3), 583-608. doi: 10.1017/s0305000903005671 Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2003a). Early Intervention After Universal Neonatal Hearing Screening: Impact on

Outcomes. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 9(4), 252-266. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.10088

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2003b). From Screening to Early Identification and Intervention: Discovering Predictors to Successful Outcomes for Children With Significant Hearing Loss. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education. Special Issue: Selected papers from the 2000 International Congress on Education of the Deaf (ICED), 8(1), 11-30. doi: 10.1093/deafed/8.1.11

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2003c). Quels sont les avantages et les désavantages du dépistage et de l'intervention précoces en surdité infantile? Conférence présentée à Colloque nord-américain sur le dépistage de la surdité et l'intervention à la petite enfance, Québec.

Yoshinaga-Itano, C. (2004). Levels of evidence: Universal newborn hearing screening (UNHS) and early hearing detection and intervention systems (EHDI). Journal of Communication Disorders, 37(5), 451- 465. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2004.04.008

Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Coulter, D., & Thomson, V. (2000). The Colorado Newborn Hearing Screening Project: effects on speech and language development for children with hearing loss. Journal of Perinatology, 20(8:2), S132-137.

Yoshinaga-Itano, C., Sedey, A. L., Coulter, B. A., & Mehl, A. L. (1998). Language of early- and later-identified children with hearing loss. Pediatrics, 102(5), 216-231.

65

Documents relatifs