• Aucun résultat trouvé

Mild-to-moderate hearing loss and language impairment: how are they linked?

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Partager "Mild-to-moderate hearing loss and language impairment: how are they linked?"

Copied!
23
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Article

Reference

Mild-to-moderate hearing loss and language impairment: how are they linked?

TULLER, Laurice, DELAGE, Hélène

TULLER, Laurice, DELAGE, Hélène. Mild-to-moderate hearing loss and language impairment:

how are they linked? Lingua , 2014, vol. 139, p. 80-101

DOI : 10.1016/j.lingua.2013.10.009

Available at:

http://archive-ouverte.unige.ch/unige:150358

Disclaimer: layout of this document may differ from the published version.

1 / 1

(2)

Mild-to-moderate hearing loss and language impairment:

How are they linked?

Laurice Tuller

a,

* , Hélène Delage

b

aUniversitéFrançois-RabelaisdeTours,UMRInserm930,3ruedesTanneurs,37041ToursCedex01,France

bUniversitédeGenève,LaboratoiredePsycholinguistiqueExpérimentale,40boulevardduPontdArve,1205Genève, Switzerland

Received18October2011;receivedinrevisedform19September2013;accepted8October2013 Availableonline15November2013

Abstract

Whatisthenatureofthelinkbetweenprelingualmild-to-moderatehearingloss(MMHL)andimpairedlanguageinchildrenand adolescents?Althoughthescientificliteratureissparse,itisclearthatmanyexperienceconsiderabledifficultyacquiringlanguage,and thatthisdifficultyisnotlimitedtophoneticform.WereportonaseriesofstudieswehaveconductedinvolvinganumberofFrench- speakingchildrenandadolescents(N>80)aged6--16,allofwhomhavebilateralsensorineural,prelingualhearingloss.Usingavariety ofmethodologiestotestawiderangeoflanguageskills,wehavefoundthatmorphosyntacticdevelopmentinchildrenwithMMHLis highlylikelytobeimpaired,thatitisoftenveryseverelyimpaired,andthatdifficultiescontinueintoadolescence.Comparisonswithother contextsofatypicalacquisitionofFrench,bothwithpathology(SLI,epilepsy)andwithoutpathology(secondlanguageacquisition)show thataspectsofmorphosyntaxwhicharesubjecttodifficultyarenotspecifictothecontextofhearingloss.Althoughthereissomeevidence forcorrelationswithdegreeofhearingloss,thesearenotregularanddonotpredictmorphosyntacticperformance:thelinkbetween hearinglossandlanguageimpairmentisindirect.Itissuggestedthatthislinkmightbemediatedbyworkingmemoryandauditory attention,whichcouldthusbeexploredasaplausibleavenueforfindinganexplanationfortheheterogeneityinlanguageperformance observedinindividualswithMMHL.

©2013ElsevierB.V.Allrightsreserved.

Keywords:Mild-to-moderatehearingloss;Acquisition;Morphosyntax;French

1. Mild-to-moderatehearinglossasamodelofatypicallanguageacquisition

Mild-to-moderatehearingloss(MMHL)correspondstoanaveragehearingloss(asmeasuredbypuretoneaverage, PTA),intherangeof21--70decibels(21--40dBformild HL,41--70dBformoderateHL).Thisdegree ofcongenital, sensorineuralHLismuchmorefrequentinchildrenthansevereandprofoundHL.So,forexample,Russetal.(2003) foundthat75%ofagroupof134Australianchildrenfittedwithhearingaidsbetweentheageof0and6,hadmildor moderateHL(respectively42%and33%),comparedtosevereandprofoundHL(16%and9%).1Sincenotallchildren withmildHLusehearingaids,2thismeansthatthetotalproportionofchildrenwithmildHLisnecessarilybiggerthanthe www.elsevier.com/locate/lingua Availableonlineatwww.sciencedirect.com

ScienceDirect

Lingua139(2014)80--101

*Correspondingauthor.

E-mailaddress:tuller@univ-tours.fr(L.Tuller).

1SeealsoFortnumetal.(2002),whofound53%moderatehearinglossinagroupof17,160Britishchildrenages3--18withhearinglossover 40dB.

2InthecaseofHLlessthan30dB,ifthisHLisconsiderednottohavenegativeimpactonlanguagedevelopment,itispossiblethatnohearing aidsareproposedatall.

0024-3841/$--seefrontmatter©2013ElsevierB.V.Allrightsreserved.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2013.10.009

(3)

figurereportedinthisstudy.Theadvent,technicalimprovement,andgeneralizationofcochlearimplantsinchildrenwith profound(90--120dB),andevensevere(70--90dB)HL,entailsthatthenumber,andtheproportionofchildrenfunctioning withthislevelofHLisnowevengreater.HearinggainsduetocochlearimplantationresultinPTAsof30--45dB(Verbist, 2010),andthustolevelsequivalenttomildormoderateHL.3Thequestionoflanguagedevelopmentinthecontextof MMHLthereforecurrentlytakesonnewsignificance,intermsofthenumberofchildreninvolved,butalsointermsofthe relevanceof thispopulationas acontrol groupinstudies evaluatingchildrenwithcochlearimplants. Understanding languagedevelopmentinthiscontextthushasbroaderimplicationsandclinicalapplications.

AnimportantfactaboutMMHListhatitisfrequentlydetectedrelativelylate:aroundtheageoffiveincountrieswhere neo-natalhearingscreeningisnotyetuniversal,asinFrance(Delage,2008;DelageandTuller,2007,2010;Tullerand Jakubowicz,2004).Furthermore,neonatalscreening(whichusesbrainstemERPs)onlydetectsHLhigherthan40dB (Govaertsetal.,2002),and thuscannotidentifychildrenwithmild HL.4Onceagain, acertainparallelcanbemade betweenlanguagedevelopmentinthecontextofacochlearimplantandinthecontextofMMHL:theageofHLdetectionin childrenwithMMHLisakintothevariableofageofcochlear implant.Inbothofthesecases,prior toaidedhearing (whetherbyhearingaids,inthecaseofMMHL,orbycochlearimplant),languageinputisdegraded.LatedetectionofHL inchildrenwithMMHLthusmeansthatmanyofthesechildrenexperienceseveralyearsofdegradedlanguageinput, years which fall squarely within the temporalwindow typically regarded to constitutea criticalperiod for language acquisition(BishopandMogford,1993;BortfeldandWhiteburst,2001;Newportetal.,2002).Studiesoftheclassical contextforcriticalperiodeffects,thatofsecondlanguageacquisition(seeJohnsonandNewport,1989,1991),have shownthatlanguageacquisitionaftertheageof6--7doesnotresemblenativeacquisitioninitsoutcome,particularlyin formalaspectsoflanguage(phonology,morphosyntax),thoughtheinterpretationoftheseresultsisnotunanimous(see Abrahamsson and Hyltenstam,2009,forreview). Studiesofchildrenwithcochlear implantshavealsofound strong correlationsbetweenlanguageperformanceandage ofimplant, highlightingtheexistence ofcriticalperiodsfor both maturationoflanguage(NicholasandGeers,2004;Tomblinetal.,2005)andmaturationofaudition(Sharmaetal.,2002, 2005).Theliteraturesupportingtheexistenceofacriticalperiodforlanguageacquisitionincludesstudiesofdeafchildren withearlyexposuretosignlanguagecomparedtothosehavinghadlateexposure.Thesehavealsoshownstrongcritical periodeffects,forbothfirstlanguagesignlanguageacquisitionandforsecondlanguageacquisitionofanorallanguage (Newport,1990;MayberryandLock,2003).ChildrenwithMMHLaregenerallynotexposedtosignlanguage,andtheydo display(some)spontaneousorallanguagedevelopment(doubtlessoneofthecausesforthefrequentlateHLdetectionin thispopulation).MMHLthusconstitutesadifferentconstellationforexplorationofpotentialcriticalperiodeffects:earlyoral languageinputisclearlymoreaccessiblethanitistoseverelyorprofoundlydeafchildrenwholearnsignlanguagelate and/orwhoundergocochlearimplantation.Ontheotherhand,latesigninputisentirelyaccessibletoadeafchild,whereas late aidedauditory inputthroughhearing aidsisnotentirelyaccessible (evenifconventionalhearing aidshavealso undergoneconsiderabletechnicalimprovements).Inotherwords,bothearlytimingoflanguagedevelopmentandlong- lasting quality of inputare alteredin MMHL. Whatis thenthe combined effect ofearly degraded inputfollowed by continuedlessthanoptimalinput(evenafterhearingaidfitting)onlanguagedevelopment?Thisconstellationsuggests thataninterestingcomparisongroupwouldbethecaseofsecondlanguageacquisition,theclassicalcontextforstudyof criticalperiodeffects,inwhichinputstartslate(similaritythenintimingoflanguageexposure),butisentirelyaccessible.

Another,obvious,butimportantfactaboutchildrenwithMMHListhat,allotherthingsbeingequal,theyhaveanormal languageacquisitiondevice, meaningthat,puttingasideforthe momentthequestion ofco-morbidity,anylanguage impairmentfoundinthispopulationwouldbea consequenceoffactorsexternaltoUniversalGrammarandthusthis populationconstitutesaparticularlyinterestingcomparisongroupforchildrenwithSpecificLanguageImpairment(SLI), forwhichproposalsaboutthesourceoflanguageimpairmentincludethesuggestionthatlanguagecompetenceisaltered inthesechildren(vanderLelyetal.,2011).Suchcomparativestudiesaresofarratherscarce,thoughresultsobtainedso farpointtoremarkablesimilarities.

Summarizing,languagedevelopmentinchildrenwithMMHLposesinterestingquestionsforthestudyoflanguage acquisition,andconstitutesaparticularlypertinentmodelforcomparativestudyoflanguageacquisitionindeafchildren withcochlearimplants,but,moregenerally,ofatypicallanguageacquisitioninchildren.

PreviousstudiesfocusedonlanguageperformanceinchildrenwithMMHLhaveshownsignificantratesoflanguage impairment, especiallyin theareas ofphonologyand morphosyntax, althoughall ofthesestudies havepointedout considerableinter-subjectvariability(seeforexampleGilbertsonandKamhi,1995;Briscoeetal.,2001;Norburyetal., 2001,2002;TullerandJakubowicz,2004;Hanssonetal.,2004,2007;DelageandTuller,2007).Table1listspublished studies which have presented results onoral language development in children and adolescents with mild and/or

3ChildrenwithMMHLarenotCIcandidatessinceconventionalhearingaidsresultinaidedhearingat/belowlevelobtainedbyCI,and,of course,destructionofa(partially)functioningcochlearaisesclearethicalquestions.

4Currenttechnologyentailsanunacceptablenumberoffalsepositivesiflevelsbelow40dBareused.

(4)

moderateHL(seealsoDelageand Tuller,2007for a detailedreview).5Itisimmediatelyapparenthowrarestudies focusingspecificallyonindividualswithMMHLare.Indeed,eveninthestudieslistedbelow,someincludedchildrenwith moresevereHL(e.g.Borgetal.,2007).Moreover,thenumberofparticipantsinmanyofthesestudiesissmall,andvery fewincludelongitudinaldata.Whenagerangeisaddedtothepicture,somecleargapsemerge:childrenwithMMHLhave beenstudied(Briscoeetal.,2001;Norburyetal.,2001,2002,etc.),andsohaveadolescents(DelageandTuller,2007), butnolongitudinalstudiesbridgingtheseagegroupshavebeenpublished,andthusimportantquestionsremainopen abouthowlanguagedevelopsduringtheperiodcoveringbothchildhoodandadolescence.Forexample,howdoesthe oftennoticedinter-subjectvariabilityforlanguageevolvewithage?Movingtothelanguagedomainsexploredinprevious workonlanguageinchildrenwithMMHL,Table1pointstothefactthatstudiestargetingspecificlinguisticvariablesare ratherlimited.Moststudieshaveusedgloballanguagemeasures,anditisthusknownthattherearelanguagedifficulties Table1

Publishedstudiesonorallanguagedevelopmentreportingresultswhichspecificallytargetchildrenwithmildand/ormoderateHL.

References Nchildren

(language)

Age HL Linguisticdomain(method) Otherchildren

Briscoeetal.(2001), Norburyetal.(2001,2002)

19(English) 5--10 20--70 Morphosyntax(sentencecomprehension, sentencerepetition,elicitedproductionofverbal inflection),phonology(non-wordrepetition, phonologicalawarenessanddiscrimination), vocabulary(receptive,expressive).

SLI

Borgetal.(2007)a 81(Swedish) 4--6 21--60 Morphosyntax(sentencecomprehension, sentenceproduction),phonology(phoneme discrimination,rhymematching,rhyme construction),vocabulary(receptive,expressive).

Ø

DelageandTuller(2007), Tulleretal.(2011)

19(French) 11--15 27--69 OralLanguageBattery(receptivevocabulary, sentencecompetition,sentencecomprehension, wordrepetition),morphosyntax(elicited productionofpronominalclitics).

SLI,Epilepsy

GilbertsonandKamhi(1995) 20(English) 7--10 20--65 OralLanguageBattery(articulation, comprehension,expressivelanguage), phonology(nonwordrepetition),vocabulary (expressiveandreceptive,novelwordlearning).

Ø

HallidayandBishop(2005) 22(English) 6--13 21--67 Morphosyntax(sentencecomprehension), phonology(non-wordrepetition),vocabulary (receptiveandexpressive).

Ø

HallidayandBishop(2006) 16(English) 8--14 24--70 Phonology(non-wordrepetition;wordnaming).

Hanssonetal.(2004) 18(Swedish) 9--13 30--57 Morphosyntax(sentencecomprehension), phonology(non-wordrepetition)vocabulary (expressive,novelwordlearning),.

SLI

Hanssonetal.(2007), SahlénandHansson(2006)

12(Swedish) 5--8 30--69 Morphosyntax(sentencecomprehension), phonology(non-wordrepetition)vocabulary (expressive,novelwordlearning),.

SLI

Kiese-HimmelandReeh(2006) 16(German) 2--4 22--66 ExpressiveVocabulary.Longitudinalstudy. Ø McGuckianandHenry(2007) 10(English) 6--8 41--70 Morphosyntax(elicitedandspontaneous

productionofgrammaticalmorphemes)

Ø Moelleretal.(2010) 4(English) 2--7 26--54 Orallanguagebattery(expressiveandreceptive),

vocabulary(expressive),speechproduction (articulationtest,spontaneousproductiondata), morphologyandsyntax(spontaneousproduction data).Longitudinalstudy.

Ø

TullerandJakubowicz(2004) 20(French) 6--13 36--65 Morphosyntax(elicitedproductionofpronominal cliticsandverbalinflection)

SLI Wakeetal.(2006) 55(English) 7--11 16--40 Orallanguagebattery,phonology(non-word

repetition,phonemeawarenessand discrimination).

Ø

a ThisstudyalsoincludesthefollowinggroupsofchildrenwithsensorineuralHL:unilateral,0--20dB(minimalHL),and61--80dB(amixof moderateandsevereHL),aswellasgroupsofchildrenwithconductiveHL.

5Thereareseveralstudieswhichincludechildrenwithmildand/ormoderateHLinlargergroupsofchildrenwithalldegreesofHL(see,for example,Blameyetal.,2001;Davisetal.,1986;Stelmachowiczetal.,2004).

(5)

in this population, but little is known about their nature and in particular about the common denominator behind morphosyntacticpropertiesthat aredifficultforchildrenandadolescents withMMHL.Finally,exploringthe natureof impairmententailscomparingthelanguagedifficultiesinthispopulationwiththoseobservedinotherstudiedpopulations, andinparticularinSLI.AsTable1shows,comparativestudiesarefew(and,inthedomainofmorphosyntax,theyare restrictedtocomparisonswithSLIandepilepsyinourgroup(TullerandJakubowicz,2004;DelageandTuller,2007;Tuller etal.,2011)andwithSLIbyBishop,Norburyandcolleagues(Briscoeetal.,2001;Norburyetal.,2001,2002).Theexisting MMHL-SLIcomparativestudieshavefoundcomparable(low)performanceonsomelanguagemeasures(e.g.non-word repetition),andquantitativeinter-groupdifferences,withtheMMHLgroupoutscoringtheSLIgroup,forotherareas(e.g., morphosyntactic measures). However, each of these studies has underlined the high variability in language skills observedinchildrenwithMMHL,withsomechildrenhavinglanguageimpairmentassevereasthatfoundinchildrenwith SLI,andothersnolanguageimpairmentatall.Anothertypeofcomparativestudy,toourknowledge,issofarentirely absentfromtheliteratureonMMHL,despiteitspotentialrelevance:thecaseoflatelanguageexposureintheabsenceof impairedcapacityforlanguage,whichisfoundinsecondlanguageacquisition.

The general goal of this paper is to present data addressing these (little studied) questions about language developmentinchildrenandadolescentswithMMHL.WewilldrawondifferentstudieswehaveconductedonFrench- speakingchildrenand adolescentswithMMHL(altogether,theseinclude 82participants aged6--16, withprelingual, bilateral,sensorineuralHL,andwithPTAsrangingfrom27to69dB).Weproposetopresentherebothsummariesof resultsstemmingfromearlierstudies(DelageandTuller,2007;Scheidnesetal.,2009;TullerandJakubowicz,2004;

Tulleretal.,2011),andnewresults,includingresultsofanalysisofspontaneouslanguagedataandresultsoflongitudinal dataina(different)groupof296-to11-year-oldchildrenwithMMHL.Giventhelimitednumberofstudiesoflanguage developmentinchildrenwithMMHL,empiricalcoverageforconclusionsisanimportantconcern.Ourspecificgoalisto contributetobetterunderstandingoftheprevalence,thenatureandthespecificityofmorphosyntacticimpairmentinthis population,and,inparticular,oftheproblemofthecommonlyfoundhighinter-subjectvariability.Toachievethesegoals, wehaveevaluatedspecificaspectsofFrenchwhichwebelieveareparticularlyindicativeoflanguageimpairment.A growing body of literature has presented results which support that hypothesis that similarities between language propertieswhichtakelongertoemergeinyoungtypicallydeveloping(TD) childrenandthosewhichcauseparticular difficulties inchildrendevelopinglanguageinunusualcircumstances (suchas SLI)are duetothe complexityofthe linguisticcomputationunderlyingtheseproperties(Jakubowicz,2005,2011;JakubowiczandTuller,2008;Prévostetal., 2010; Jakubowiczand Strik,2008; Soares,2006). Inthesestudies,complexityhasbeenmeasuredbyreference to linguisticoperations:numberofinstancesofsyntacticmovementoperations,asoriginallyproposedbyJakubowicz),orof agreementoperations,morphologicaland phonologicalclustering,and,moregenerally,level ofsyntacticembedding (Hamannetal.,2007).Wewillpursuethislineofinvestigationheretoexplorethenatureofmorphosyntacticdifficulties experiencedbychildrenwithMMHL.

Section2,afterpresentinggeneralmeasuresofmorphosyntaxderivedfromstandardizedlanguageassessmenttools, focusesondifferentmeasuresofcomputationallycomplexaspectsofmorphosyntaxinFrench:productionofaccusative clitics, whichhasbeenargued to bea clinicalmarker oflanguage impairmentinFrench, and thenperformanceon syntacticstructuresentailingdifferentdegreesoflinguisticcomputationalcomplexity(passives,relatives,andsyntactic embeddingingeneral).Inkeepingwiththepointsoutlinedabove,wewillbecomparinglanguagedevelopmentinchildren withMMHLtoTDchildrenandadolescents,butalsotoindividualsdevelopinglanguageinotheratypicalsituations,with pathology(SLIandsevere-to-profoundHL)andwithout(secondlanguageacquisition).

Section3addressestheinter-subjectvariabilityfoundinthestudiespresentedinSection2,whichmirrorinthisrespect otherstudiesoflanguageinchildrenwithMMHL.WefocusonthevariablesofdegreeofHLandage.Agebringsupthe questionoftheevolutionoflanguageperformanceovertime,whichweaddressviaresultsofalongitudinalstudy.Wewill alsobrieflymentionthevariablesofnonverballevel,socioeconomicstatus,ageofHLdetectionandageoffittingwith hearingaids.Regardingthelattertwovariables,itisimportanttomakeclearattheoutsetthatthesevariablesarevery difficulttostudyinthecontextofMMHL.Firstofall,sincethelatterlogicallyandpracticallyfollowstheformer,wecanrefer toasinglevariable,althoughitshouldbenotedthatsomechildrenwithmildHLdonotwearhearingaidsatall.Although thisvariablehasbeenshowntobeimportantinthecaseofmoreseverehearingloss,withsignificantcorrelationstypically foundbetween agesofdetection/fittingand languageperformance,itsrelevancetothecaseofMMHLhasnotbeen shown.Thereasonsforthisaresimple.GreaterHLtendstobediagnosedearlierthanlesssevereHL,asitismore apparenttoparentsandotherpersonsassociatingwiththechild.Atthesametime,milderhearinglosscanbeexpectedto have less severe linguistic repercussions,particularly in childrenwith mild HL whohave never worn hearing aids.

Explorationofthelengthofaidedexposuretolanguageandlanguageperformancethuscannotreallybecarriedoutinthe caseofMMHL.Discussionofthisvariablewillthusnecessarilybelimited,thoughdirectlyaddressedinSection3.

AgeneraldiscussionispresentedinSection4,whichreturnstothebasicquestionofthenatureofthelinkbetween MMHLandlanguageimpairment,andsuggeststhatthislinkmaybetheresultofinterfacebetweenlanguageandareasof linguisticperformancesystems.

(6)

2. Morphosyntacticimpairment

ThissectionreportsonmorphosyntacticperformanceinseveralgroupsofFrench-speakingchildrenandadolescents withMMHL.First,wewillpresentresultsindicativeofgeneralperformance,asmeasuredbystandardizedtests.Wewill subsequentlyfocusonparticularsyntacticstructures,reportingonresultsusingavarietyofdifferentkindsofmeasures:

elicited production of accusative clitics, elicited production of subject versus object relative clauses, grammaticality judgmentsonmorphosyntacticproperties(passives,relativesclauses,personandnumberagreementonpronouns,and verbraisinginsimplesentences),andfrequencyandtypeofsyntacticembeddinginspontaneouslanguagesamples.

ClinicianswhoworkwithchildrenwithMMHLoftendescribethesechildrenasgivingtheillusionofnormality.Wehavesought tousedifferenttypesofmeasuresinanefforttounderstandwhatmightbebehindthisobservation.Do(someof)these childrenavoidstructureswhichcausethemdifficulties?Finally,ineachofthesestudies,wehavesoughttocomparethe morphosyntaxofchildrenandadolescentswithMMHLtothatoftheirTDpairs,butalso,crucially,tothatofindividuals acquiringFrenchasasecondlanguageoracquiringFrenchwithSLI.Inthissection,wewillfocusentirelyoncross-sectional results,reservingourlongitudinalstudyforSection3,whichdirectlyaddressesthequestionofinter-subjectvariability.

2.1. Generalperformance

How do children with MMHL fare on standardized language tests designed to assess children for language impairment?Toanswerthisquestion,wepresentresultsfromourstudyof32childrenwithMMHLaged6;1to11;11 (M=9;2,SD=1;9).Hearingthresholds(PTA)inthisgroupofchildrenrangedfrom29to69decibels.TheageofHL detectionvariedbetween0;2and8;8yearold(M=4;9,SD=1;9),ageofHLfittingbetween0;5and9;9yearold(M=5;6, SD=2;3).ThreechildrenwithmildHL(29--35dB)didnotwearhearingaids.Itisworthnotingthatthesechildrenwerenot recruitedfrom caseloads ofspeech-language pathologists,since our goalwas tostudya group ofchildrenwhose languagewastrulyrepresentativeofthatofentirepopulation ofchildrenwithMMHL.Wethereforerecruitedthrough systematicconsultationofalldossiersofchildrendiagnosedwithbilateral,prelingual,sensorineuralmildormoderate hearing lossat a regional pediatric hospitalotorhinolaryngology service. Ofthe 40families contacted, 2 refused to participateinthestudy,and6wereexcludedaftertestingbecauseofclearcomplicatingfactors(onechildwithcerebral palsy,andtheotherswithglobaldevelopmentaldelayand/orabsenceofregularschooling).The32childrenwithMMHL retainedforstudywerecomparedto10childrenwithSLIaged8;0to11;11(M=10;1,SD=1;4).6Therewasnosignificant differenceinagebetweenthetwogroups(U=120.5,p=0.2).7Allparticipantswereadministeredacomputerizedoral languagebattery(Khomsietal.,2007).Inthisbattery,expressivegrammarisevaluatedusingasentencecompletion task:Thechildlistenstoasentencecorrespondingtoapictureonacomputerscreenandthenhastocompletetheendof thesentencetofitwiththepicture(e.g.,Ici,lechienboitsonlait;làleschiens...boiventleurlait‘Herethedogisdrinking hismilk;There,thedogs...aredrinkingtheirmilk’).Thistestassessesabilitytoproduceavarietyofspecificgrammatical morphemes:nominal,adjectivalandverbalinflexion,irregularplurals,prepositions,passives,andpronominalclitics.The morphosyntacticcomprehensiontest consists ofa taskassessingability tojudgethe well-formednessofsentences presented orally with a related picture. The child has to decide whether the sentence heard with the picture is grammaticallycorrect,bygivingayesornoanswer(e.g.,apictureofseveralbirdssittingonatreebranchisshownand thechildhearsthefollowing(ungrammaticalsentence):*Lesoiseauxestsurlabranche‘Thebirdsisonthebranch’).

AlthoughthechildrenwithMMHLscoredsignificantlybetterthanthechildrenwithSLIoneachofthesetwotasks(for expressive grammar, U=45, p<001; for receptive grammar, U=65.5, p<01), their mean scores showed that morphosyntax,astestedbysuchgeneralmeasures,isimpairedinthesechildren:ThemeanSDwas 1.1forexpressive grammar( 3fortheSLIgroup)and 1.4forreceptivegrammar( 4fortheSLIgroup).Behindthesegroupmeans,there washighinter-subjectvariability:whileimpairmentcanbesevere,notallchildrenareseverelyimpaired,andsomearenot impairedatall.Toillustratethispoint,considerFig.1,whichpresentsindividualresultsofthechildrenwithMMHLforthe sentencecompletiontask:16childrenobtainedlowscores(asdefinedbyaSDbelow 1)and16childrenhadcompletely normalones(i.e.≥ 1SD).Fig.1alsoshowsthatmanyofthelow-scoringchildrenobtainedverylowscores:37%ofthem hadSDsbetween 1.65and 6.Overall,50%ofthechildrendisplayedsomedegreeoflanguageimpairment,arate whichistypicalforstudiesofchildrenwithMMHL(seeforexampleGilbertsonandKamhi,1995;Briscoeetal.,2001or Hanssonetal.,2007).

Thesetwomorphosyntactictasksweretwoofthesixtasksoftheorallanguagebatteryallparticipantswereadministered.

Theothertaskstestedphonology(awordrepetitiontask),expressivevocabulary(apicturenamingtask),lexicaljudgment

6Thiscomparativestudywaspartofthesecondauthors(unpublished)doctoraldissertation(Delage,2008).

7Notethat,duedothenon-normaldistributionofthedata,ouranalyseswereconductedwithnon-parametrictests,withMann--Whitneytestfor intergroupcomparisons,WilcoxontestforintragroupcomparisonsandfinallywithSpearmansrankcorrelation.

(7)

(aword--picturematchingtask)andsentencecomprehension(asentence--picturematchingtask).Interestingly,ifwefocus exclusivelyonthechildrenwithMMHLwhohadlowglobalresultsontheentirelanguagebattery(the16childrenhavingat least 2scores< 1.65SDamong the 6 scoresin the battery),childrenwe havedubbedas ‘‘MMHLwith Language Impairment(MMHL-LI)’’inFig.2,nostatisticaldifferenceswerefoundbetweentheMMHLandSLIgroupsforeitherofthetwo morphosyntactictasks(forexpressivegrammar,U=43andp=0.08orforreceptivegrammar,U=57.5andp=0.33),as illustratedbyFig.2.Inotherwords,asubgroupofchildrenwithMMHLhadperformancequantitativelyverysimilartothatof childrenwithSLI.Theimportantpointhereisthat,whereastheprevalenceofSLIisestimatedtobearound7%(Tomblinetal., 1997),theprevalenceoflanguageimpairmentintheMMHLgroupherereaches50%andthuscouldnotbetheresultofa simplephenomenonofcomorbidity(sincenomorethan7%ofdeafchildrenshouldalsohaveSLI).

Summarizing, standardized language measures of morphosyntax identified approximately 50% of this group of childrenwithMMHLashavingimpairedmorphosyntax.Thisresult,includingthisparticularrateofimpairment,coincides withthoseofpreviousstudies,includingourown.Alsoconsistentwithpreviousstudiesisthefindingthat,asagroup,the childrenwithMMHLwithover-allimpairedlanguageresembledchildrenwithSLI,asagroup,intermsoftheseverityof theirmorphosyntacticimpairment.

2.2. AclinicalmarkeroflanguageimpairmentinFrench:accusativecliticproduction

Standardizedtestsofthetypepresentedintheprecedingsectiongiveageneralpictureofthedegreeofimpairment.

However,theyarenotdesignedtoexplorewhichareasofmorphosyntaxcausedifficultiesandwhichonesdonot.Tasks focusedonspecificlanguagepropertiesallowforsuchexplorationandthusholdthepromiseoffurtheringunderstanding of thenatureoflanguageimpairmentin childrenwithMMHL.In otherwords, wewouldliketoknow whichareas of morphosyntaxareaffectedandwhetherthesecanbedirectlylinkedtoHL.

Fig.2. Expressiveandreceptivegrammar:MeanZ-scoresobtainedbychildrenwithMMHLwithlanguageimpairment(MMHL-LI)andbychildren withSLI.

Fig.1. Expressivegrammar:standarddeviationsobtainedbychildrenwithMMHLcomparedtomeanSDobtainedbychildrenwithSLI.

(8)

TullerandJakubowicz(2004)exploredthequestionofwhetherperceptuallimitationsresultingfromHLleadtodifficulty withgrammaticalelementswhosesurfaceformislesssalient.Todothis,severalsuchgrammaticalitemswerecompared, and,inparticular,definitedeterminersandthirdpersonaccusativeclitics(seeTable2).Theseparticularmorphemeswere chosen since, although their phonological form is identical, they differ in terms of the complexity of the linguistic computationrequiredtoderivethem.Notably,whiledefinitedeterminersappearinthecanonicalpositionfordeterminers inFrench(atthebeginningoftheDP),accusativecliticsdonotappearpostverballyinthecanonicaldirectobjectposition, butratherpreverbally(seeTulleretal.,2011,fordetaileddiscussionofthecomplexpropertiesofthirdpersonaccusative clitics,whichalsoincludethefactthattheygenerallyco-occurwithanominativecliticinspokenFrenchandthattheyare optionalunderspecificdiscourseconditionsinspokenFrench).

Elicitedproductionofdeterminerswascomparedtothatofthirdpersonaccusativecliticsin20childrenwithMMHL aged6;0to13;9comparedto10childrenwithSLIaged6;0to8;6andto36youngtypically-developing3-,4-,and6-year- olds(12childrenperagegroup).ThechildrenwithMMHLweredividedintotwoagegroups:10childrenaged6--8and10 childrenaged9--13.Anexperimentalprobewasusedinwhichchildrenwereshownapictureandaskedaquestion,asin (1),elicitingadefinitedeterminer,and(2),elicitinganaccusativeclitic.

(1) [pictureofMinniesprinklingagirlwithagardenhose]

Experimenter:QuiaroseMinnie?‘WhoissprinklingMinnie?’ Expectedresponse:lafille‘thegirl’

(2) [pictureofagirlaimingawaterpistolatMinnie]

Experimenter:QuefaitlafilleàMinnie?‘What’sthegirldoingtoMinnie?’

Expectedresponse:Ellelavise‘She’saimingather’

AsillustratedinFig.3,accusativecliticswerelesswellmasteredthandefinitedeterminersbychildrenwithMMHL,by children with SLI, and by 3-year-old control children. Performance in all groups of children reached ceiling for determiners,forwhichproductionrateswereabove95%,whereasaccusativecliticswereproducedatmuchlower ratesbychildrenwithMMHL(40%inyoungerchildrenand68%inolderchildren),byTD3-and4-year-olds(44%and 79%,respectively),andbychildrenwithSLI(only29%).TullerandJakubowicz(2004)concludedthattheseresults showedthatperceptualsalienceisnotthe(sole)factorwhichdeterminesmorphosyntacticperformanceinchildrenwith MMHL(orwithSLI),sinceonlyoneoftwophonologicallyidenticalmorphemeswithlowsalienceisasourceofdifficulty andsuggestedthereforethattheincreasedcomputationalcomplexityinvolvedinoneofthem(accusativeclitics)isakey factorinaccountingfordifficulty,inlearners,bothtypicalandatypical(withandwithouthearingloss).8Asmentioned above,ithasbeenarguedthataccusativecliticproductionisarobustmarkerofmorphosyntacticdifficultiesinchildren acquiringFrench.Notonlydodifficultiespersistintoadolescence(seeTulleretal.,2011),theyarefoundinthecontextof verydifferentpathologies,inMMHLandSLI,butalsoinbenignchildhoodepilepsy(seeTulleretal.,2011;Monjauze, 2007),andinchildrenacquiringFrenchasasecondlanguageinCanadianimmersionschools(ParadisandCrago, 2003;Paradis,2004).GiventhecircumstancesoflanguageacquisitioninchildrenwithMMHL,andinparticularthefact thattimingissuesareinvolved(accessibleinputbeginninglateformanyofthesechildren,andremaininglessthan optimal), we hypothesized inSection 1 that comparison with TD children learning a second language (L2) after early childhood would be appropriateinthatlanguage input forthese children alsostarts late. Is performance in thesethreedifferentgroups,childL2learners,childrenwithMMHL,andchildrenwithSLI,comparableforthismarkerof morphosyntacticdifficultyinFrench?

Table2

Definitedeterminersanddirectobjectpronouns(AccusativeClitics)inFrench.

Definitedeterminers Directobjectpronouns(accusativeclitics)

Singular Plural Singular Plural

Masculine Feminine Masculine Feminine

le la les le la les

legarcon theboy

lafille thegirl

lesfilles thegirls

Maxlevoit Maxhimsees

Maxlavoit Maxhersees

Maxlesvoit Maxthemsees

8TullerandJakubowicz(2004)notethatnosignificantdifferenceswerefoundbetweentheage-matchedchildrenwithMMHLandwithSLI (foraccusativecliticproduction,U=39.5,p=.43),thoughtheolderchildrenwithMMHLoutperformedtheyoungerchildrenwithMMHL(U=24, p<.05).WeaddressageeffectsdirectlyinSection3.2.

(9)

Webringtogetherhereresultsallowingforawideinter-groupcomparisonofelicitedproductionofaccusativeclitics:

The32childrenwithMMHL(ages6--11)presentedabove inSection2.1 werecompared to3 groupsofTDchildren (6-year-olds,8-year-olds,and11-year-olds),to18childrenwithSLI(ages6--12),andto29firstlanguageBritishEnglish- speaking childrenacquiring L2 French (ages 6--12), naturally,after familyimmigration to France.9Accusative clitic productionwastested,butalsoproductionoftwoothertypesofpronominalclitics,nominativeandreflexive.Nominative cliticsdo notinvolvemovement to anon-canonical position,and,reflexivecliticshave alocal antecedent,whereas accusativecliticsinvolvebothappearanceinanon-canonical(preverbal)positionandtheirantecedentisnecessarilynon- local (seeJakubowiczetal.,1998; Tulleretal.,2011).Giventhesedifferencesinthe computationofthesedifferent elements,weexpectedtofindthataccusativecliticswouldbelesswellproducedthantheothertwotypesofclitics.To elicittheseclitics,wedevelopedaProductionProbeforPronounClitics(PPPC),whichelicitsproductionofthirdperson nominative,reflexive,andaccusativeclitics,10byrequiringaresponsetoaquestionabouta drawingappearingona computerscreen,asin(3)and(4).

(3) Experimenter:QuefaitThomas? ‘What’sThomasdoing?’ Expectedresponse:Ilsepèse. ‘He’sweighinghimself’

(4) Experimenter:QuefaitMarieaveclechien? ‘What’sMarydoingtothedog?’ Expectedresponse:Ellelelave. ‘She’swashingit’

Table3presentstheresultsobtainedbyeachgrouptested.Accusativecliticswereproducedatlowerratesthanboth nominativeandreflexiveclitics,ineachofthe‘‘atypical’’groups---MMHL,SLI,andL2.Thiswasalsothecaseforthe youngestTDgroup,the6-year-olds,butnotfortheTD8andtheTD11groups,whichdisplayedceilingperformance.Inter- groupcomparisonsrevealedasignificantgroupeffect,withtheMMHLperformingworseonaccusativecliticsthanthe TD8(U=79,p<01)andtheTD11(U=55,p<001),thoughnotthantheTD6(U=163.5,p=0.4),andbetterthantheSLI

Fig.3. Meanproductionratesfordeterminersandaccusativeclitics:MMHL,SLI,TD-3,TD-4,TD-6.

9ThemeanageoffirstexposuretoFrench,uponarrivalinFrancewas7;0(SD1;10andrange4;5to10;9),andthemeanlengthofexposure was2;5(SD1;2).AsubsetoftheL2resultswasreportedinanunpublishedthesis(Haiden,2011).

10Thistaskalsoelicitsfirstpersonclitics,whichfarebetterthanthirdpersonclitics(seeTulleretal.,2011).

(10)

(U=182.5,p<05).Interestingly,therewasnosignificantdifferenceforproductionofthisclinicalmarkerbetweenchildren withMMHLandthegroupofL2children(U=441.5,p=0.7).

Insummary,twodifferentgroupsofFrench-speakingchildrenwithMMHL(atotalof52children,aged6--13,meanage 9)displayedthesamespecificdifficultywiththirdpersonaccusativecliticsfoundinyoungerTDchildren,inchildrenthe sameagewithSLI,andinchildrenthesameagelearningFrenchasasecondlanguageafteragefour.Whilechildrenwith MMHL,asagroup,performedbetterthanchildrenwithSLIofthesameage,theirperformancewasquitecomparableto thatoftheL1English/L2Frenchchildren.

2.3. Measuresofcomputationalcomplexity

AccusativecliticsarenotoriouslydifficultfordifferentlearnersofFrench,andthisdifficultyincludeschildrenwithMMHL, aswehavejustseen.Investigationofotherstructuresentailinghighlevelsofcomputationalcomplexitywillallowustoseeto whatextentthecontextofMMHLleadstolanguageimpairment.Wewillreportonresultstestingchildren’sabilitytojudgethe grammaticalityofrelativeclausesandpassives,ontheonehand,andsimplesentencesinvolvingagreementandordinaryV- to-Tenseraising,ontheother.Wethenfocusspecificallyonelicitedproductionofsubjectandobjectrelativeclauses,and turn,finally,totheproductionofdifferenttypesofclausalembeddinginspontaneouslanguagesamples.

2.3.1. Grammaticalityjudgmentsonconstructionsofvaryingcomplexity

Inordertoassessavarietyofstructuresimplyingdifferentdegreesofcomputationalcomplexity,weadministereda grammaticalityjudgmenttestto10adolescents withmoderateHL(50--70dB)compared toadolescentswithsevere (71--89dB)orprofoundHL(92--120dB),totypicallydevelopingadolescentswhowereage-matchedforthedeaflearners andalsotoL2adults.ThisL2populationwasmadeupof19nativespeakersofEnglishwhohadbegunacquiringFrench atameanageof13;11(SD3;3),whosemeannumberofyearsstudyingFrenchwas6;10(SD3;1),andwhowereallin Franceona study-abroadprogram (havingarrivedinFrance between1 and 6monthsprior totesting time).These learnersweredividedintoagroupof10advancedand9intermediatelearners,accordingtotheresultsofClozeand vocabularytests.InformationonparticipantsissummarizedinTable4.

Thegrammaticalityjudgmenttestconsistedof48items(24grammaticaland24ungrammatical),whichappearedona computerscreen,alongwith‘‘yes’’and‘‘no’’buttons;theparticipanthadtoclickaccordingtohis/her judgmentofthe sentence.Awrittentestwasusedinsteadofanoraltestinordertotestgrammaticalityjudgmentforsentenceswithdifferent

Table4

AgeandsexofTDadolescents,L2adults,andadolescentswithHL.

Groups N Sex Meanage Agerange

TDadolescents 51 28M23F 13;1 11;4--15;3

L2adults Advanced 10 1M9F 21;1 19;11--23;2

Intermediate 9 5M4F 21;4 19;9--25;11

AdolescentswithHL ModerateHL 10 8M2F 14;1 11;10--16;4

SevereHL 15 10M5F 14;1 11;1--15;9

ProfoundHL 26 13M13F 13;3 11;0--16;6

Table3

Production(%)ofnominative,reflexiveandaccusativeclitics:MMHL,TD,SLI,andL2.

Group Agerange Nominative Reflexive Accusative

MMHL(N=32) 6;1--11;11(M=9;2) 91%(SD=13) 87%(SD=27) 65%(SD=33) Accusative<nominative(p<001) Accusative<reflexive(p<001) SLI(N=18) 6;6--12;11(M=9;5) 76%(SD=36) 72%(SD=36) 42%(SD=36) Accusative<nominative(p<01)

Accusative<reflexive(p<01) L2(N=29) 6;4--12;7(M=9;5) 85%(SD=21) 72%(SD=35) 61%(SD=35) Accusative<nominative(p<.01)

Accusative<reflexive(p<.05) TD6(N=12) 6;1--6;7(M=6;4) 98%(SD=2) 100% 78%(SD=15) Accusative<nominative(p<01)

Accusative<reflexive(p<01) TD8(N=12) 7;9--8;7(M=8;2) 98%(SD=6) 100% 93%(SD=13)

TD11(N=12) 11;1--11;9(M=11;4) 94%(SD=9) 100% 98%(SD=5)

(11)

degreesofcomputationalcomplexitywithouttestingatthesametimeproficiencyinlip-readingintheadolescentswith severeandprofoundHL.Foursyntactic constructionswere evaluated(12 items each):relativeclauses(including subject,object,indirectobjectoradjunctrelativeclauses)inwhichtheungrammaticalsentencesinvolvedtheformofthe relativepronoun,asin(5),passives,asin(6),inwhichtheerrorinvolvedtheabsenceofpar‘by’intheby-phrase,simple sentencesinwhichgenderandnumberagreementonpronounswasmanipulated,asin (7),andsimplesentences thatvariedingrammaticalityaccordingtowhetherthetensedverbwasraisedtotheleftoftheadverbofnegationornot, asin(8).

(5) a. J’aidesamisquiarriventcesoir ‘Ihavefriendsthat[subject]arecomingtonight’ b. *J’aidescopainsqueviennentcesoir ‘Ihavefriendsthat[object]arecomingtonight’ (6) a. L’arbreestcachéparlebâtiment ‘Thetreeishiddenbythebuilding’

b. *Lebâtimentestcachélesarbres ‘Thebuildingishiddenthetrees’

(7) a. Lamoto?Jelagaredansleparking ‘Themotorbike[fem]?Iit[acc-fem]parkintheparkinglot’ b. *Lavoiture?Jelegaredanslegarage ‘Thecar[fem]?Iit[acc-masc]parkinthegarage’

(8) a. Legarçonneveutplusfairesesdevoirs ‘Theboynolongerwantstodohishomework’

b. *Mescopainsnepassortentcesoir ‘Myfriendsnotgoouttonight’

Scheidnesetal.(2009)attributedvaryingdegreesofcomplexitytothesefourconstructions,accordingtowhetherornot their derivationentails Internal Merge(syntactic movement), in accordance withthe Derivational ComplexityMetric (Jakubowicz,2005).Sincerelativeclausesinvolvewh-movementofawh-operatortotheleftperipheryoftherelative clause,andsyntacticpassivesA-movementofthedirectobjecttosubject,thesebothentailahighdegreeofcomplexity.

Ontheotherhand,thesimplesentencestestinggenderandnumberagreementonpronounsandadverbplacementdo notinvolveeitherwh-movementorNP-movement.Theselatterdoentailraisingoftheverbtoinflection(resultinginthe surfacepositionoftheadverbaftertheinflectedverb),butthismovementisrequiredinalltensedclausesinFrench.These considerationstakentogethermeanthatthesimplesentencesinwhichonlynumberandgenderagreementonobject clitics(agreement)orverb-adverborderaremanipulatedarecomputationallylesscomplexthanthesentencescontaining relativeclausesorpassives.

Fig.4presentsthemeanglobalscoresonthegrammaticalityjudgmenttaskforeachofthegroupstested:groupresults wereallabovechance(ascertainedtobeat30/48viaabinomialtest).TheadolescentswithmoderateHLshowedpoorer performance(M38.4/48)thanthe TDadolescents (M43.1/48) andthanthe advancedL2 adults(M44.9/48). They performedbetterthantheadolescentswithprofoundHL(M32/48;U=63.5;p<.01),butaswell/asbadlyasboththe IntermediateL2learners(M38.4/48;U=45;p=1.00)andasadolescentswithsevereHL(39.1/48;U=69;p=0.74).

Interestingly,andwereturntothispointinSection3.1,withinthegroupofadolescentswithmoderateHL,asignificant negativecorrelationwasfoundbetweenthesescoresanddegreeofhearingloss(rs= 0.607).

Comparisonsofitemtypes(Table5)revealedthatnoneofthesub-scoresobtainedbyadolescentswithmoderateHL differed significantly from those of adolescents with severe HL.However, adolescents with moderate HL performed significantlyworsethantheintermediateL2adultsonitemsinwhichnumberandgenderagreementweremanipulatedon accusativecliticsandonrelativeclauses.Infact,allthreegroupsofparticipantswithHLhaddifficultiesinjudgingagreement onaccusativecliticsinsimplesentences:Theyfailedtodetectagreementviolations(7b)significantlymoreoftenthanthe adolescentcontrolsandthanbothgroupsofL2learners.Wesupposethatthismaybeaneffectofthepersistentdifficulty associatedwithaccusativeclitics(seeSection2.2).Thedifficultywithrelativeclauses,theitemsarguedtoinvolvethehighest degreeofcomputationalcomplexity,issharedbyallgroupsofatypicallearners,whoseperformancewassignificantlybelow

Fig.4. Scoresongrammaticalityjudgmenttask:meannumberofcorrectresponses/48.

(12)

thatoftheTDadolescents.TheadolescentswithmoderateHLalsoperformedsignificantlybeloweventheintermediateL2 adults.And,likebothgroupsofL2adults,andtheothergroupsofadolescentswithHL,grammaticalityofpassivesentences waslesswelldetectedbyadolescentswithmoderateHLthanintheTDadolescents.Thepatternoflowerscoresforrelative clausesandhigherscoresforverbraisingisparticularlynoteworthy.TheL2andthedeafgroupsalldisplaythissame pattern.11ThisisstrikingbecauseitmeansthattheL2groupshadmoredifficultycorrectlyjudgingthegrammaticalityofthe constructionswhichweremorecomplex,butsimilartotheirL1(relativeclauses)thantheydidjudgingthegrammaticalityof constructionswhichwerelesscomplex,butdifferentfromtheirL1(V-Raising).Scheidnesetal.(2009)arguedthatthisresult wouldseemtoindicatethatL1transferfromEnglishplaysalimitedrole,atleastinthistypeoftask.

Ageneralpicture thusemergesin whichthe computationallymost complexitemswere moredifficult forbothL2 learners and learners with HL, including adolescents with moderate HL. This fundamental similarity between the L2learnersandthelearnerswithHLisallthemorenoteworthygiventhedifferencesinageandquantityandqualityof exposurebetweenthesegroups.

2.3.2. Elicitedproductionofrelativeclauses

Relativeclauseshavebeenwellstudiedinbothtypicalandatypicallanguageacquisition(seeFriedmannetal.,2009,fora review).Thesyntaxofrelativeclausesinvolvesbothclausalembeddingandwh-movementofanoperatorfromthethematic positiontoaclause-initialposition,twosourcesofcomputationalcomplexityundertheapproachbeingadoptedhere.While bothsubject(9)andobject(10)relativeshavethesetwoproperties,objectrelativesinlanguageslikeFrench,inaddition, entailadependencybetweenthethematicpositionoftheobject(thecanonicaldirectobjectposition)andthewh-operator whichislongerthanthatbetweenthesubjectpositionanditsantecedent.AsFriedmannetal.(2009)pointout,thepath betweenthethematicpositionanditsantecedent,crucially,isinterruptedbyanothernounphrase(thesubject),whichthey arguerendersthesyntacticcalculationinobjectrelativesconsiderablymorecomplexthanthatinsubjectrelatives.

(9) Je connais le garçoni[quiitipousse le chien] ‘I know the boy who is pushing the dog’

(10) Je connais le garçoni[0i que le chien pousse t t] ‘I know the boy who the dog is pushing’

Weevaluatedtheproductionofsubjectandobjectrelativeclausesin29ofthe32childrenandadolescentswithMMHL presentedinsection2.1,whentheywereagedfrom7;11to13;11(M=11;2)andcomparedtheirresultstothoseof126- year-oldcontrolchildrenandto8childrenwithSLIaged8;0to11;11(M=9;6).12TheparticipantswithMMHLweredivided intotwoagegroups:14childrenwithMMHLagedfrom7to11(M=9;8)and15adolescentswithMMHLagedfrom11to 13(M=12;6).Weusedthe sametaskasFriedmannandNovrogodsky(2004),foreliciting productionofsentences containingasubjectoranobjectrelativeclause(asin(11)foranobjectrelative)

Table5

Sub-scores on grammaticality judgmenttask items: mean numberof correctresponses/12 (above chance performance=9/12, chance performanceinboldface).

TD11--15 Adv.L2 Inter.L2 ModerateHL SevereHL ProfoundHL

V-Raising M/12 10.7 11.3 11.0 ns 10.6 10.4 9.1

SD 0.8 1.6 1.3 0.8 1.1 1.8

Agreement M/12 10.8 10.9 10.9 * 9.1 9.6 7.6

SD 2 1.2 1.2 1.9 2.0 2.6

Passives M/12 11 10.7 9.6 ns 10.7 10.5 8.5

SD 1.7 2.7 2.2 1.8 2.0 2.3

Relatives M/12 10 9.5 9.8 * 7.5 8.0 6.5

SD 1.2 1.5 2.6 1.3 2.2 2.0

11Awithin-groupcomparisonshowsthattheL2ersscoredsignificantlyloweronRelativesclausescomparedtoV-raising(Z=2.56,p<0.05) andAgreement(Z=2.10,p<0.01).Thesameinter-constructiondifferencesweresignificantinthedeaf:relativeswereworsethanV-Raising (Z=5.59,p<.0001)andAgreement(Z=3.01,p<.01).

12Thiscomparativestudywaspartofthesecondauthorsdoctoraldissertation(Delage,2008).

(13)

(11) Elicitationofanobjectrelative:

Experimenter:Voiciunéléphantetungarçon‘Hereareanelephantandaboy’ Icil’éléphantpeintlegarçon‘Heretheelephantispaintingtheboy’

Icilegarçonpeintl’éléphant‘Heretheboyispaintingtheelephant’ Celui-là,c’estqueléléphant?‘Whichelephantisthis?’

[whilepointingtotheelephantinthebottompicture]

Expectedanswer:C’estl’éléphantquelegarçonpeint

‘That’stheelephantthattheboyispainting’

Fig.5presentsproductionratesofsubjectandobjectrelativeclausesforeachgrouptested.Despitethepresenceofhigh inter-subjectvariabilityinallgroups,itemergesthatchildrenwithMMHLandchildrenwithSLIproducedsignificantlyfewer subjectrelativeclausesthantheolderparticipantswithMMHL(U=23.5,p<.001)andthecontrols(U=3.5,p<.01).The subjectrelativerateoftheyoungerchildrenwithMMHLdidinfactnotdiffersignificantlyfromthoseofthechildrenwithSLI.

Theobjectrelativeratesaremuchlowerthanthesubjectrelativeratesinallgroupsofchildren.

Thissubject--object relativeasymmetryiswellknowninthe acquisitionliterature(seeFriedmannetal.,2009,for review).Whatisinterestinghereistoseewhatkindsofstructureschildrenproducedinsteadofanobjectrelative.Fig.6 showsthedifferenttypesofresponsesproducedbychildreninthefourgroups.

6-year-oldcontrolchildrenandolderchildrenwithMMHLgavesimilarresponses,producingcausativepassiveswith thereflexiveformsefaire‘makeoneself’andpassivesubjectrelativeclausesasin(12bandc),inlieuofanobjectrelative

Fig.6. Structuresproduced(%)insteadofanobjectrelativeclause:MMHL,SLI,TD-6.

Fig.5.Production(%)ofsubjectandobjectrelativeclauses:MMHL,SLI,TD-6.

(14)

(12a).Althoughthis kindofresponse isnot what waselicited in the warm-up, itissemantically, pragmatically and grammaticallycorrect,and,assuch,isexpected.Althoughproductionofanappropriatesubjectrelativeinthiscontext requiresusingapassivestructure,itdoesnotentailaconfigurationinwhichthepathbetweenthethematicpositionandits antecedentisinterruptedbythesubjectnounphrase.Productionofapassivesubjectrelativeconstitutesnotonlythemost appropriatealternativetoanobjectrelative,itisthesyntacticallymostsophisticatedalternative,inthatitinvolvesboth productionofarelativeclauseandapassivestructure.75%ofthenon-objectrelativeanswersproducedbycontrolsand 92%ofthoseproducedbyolderchildrenwithMMHLwereresponsesofthistype.

(12) a. C’estle roi quele garçonpeigne it’s thekingthattheboy combs

‘Itisthekingwhotheboyiscombing’

b. C’estle roi qui se fait peigner parle garçon it’s the kingwho himselfmakesto-combby theboy

‘Itisthekingwhohashimselfcombedbytheboy’

c. C’estle roi qui estpeigné parle garçon it’s thekingwhois combedby theboy

‘It’sthekingwhoiscombedbytheboy’

However,theyoungerchildrenwithMMHL(aged7--11)andthechildrenwithSLIproduced,respectively,only54%and 31% non-object relative responses of this type. Instead, they tended to produce less complex structures: simple sentenceswithoutanyrelativeclauseatall(42%inchildrenwithSLIand18%intheyoungerchildrenwithMMHL),and evenactivesubjectrelatives,whichthusinvolvedthematicrolereversal(5%intheyoungerchildrenwithMMHLand23%

inthe childrenwithSLI).The youngerchildrenwithMMHL alsoproducedadverbialrelativeclauses (22%)usingoù

‘where’,both grammatical, as in (15a),with a direct objectpronoun, and thus nowh-movement of the object, and

ungrammatical,asin(16a),withamissingdirectobjectpronoun.

(13) a. Lelionmouillel’éléphant SimpleSentence

‘Thelionissprinklingtheelephant’

b. C’estl’éléphantquelelionmouille TargetObjectRelative

‘Itistheelephantthatthelionissprinkling’

(14) a. C’estlechatquimordlechien ActiveSubjectRelative

‘It’sthecatthatbitesthedog’

b. C’estlechatquelechienmord TargetObjectRelative

‘It’sthechatthatthedogbites’

(15) a. C’estcelleoùlagirafelalave Where-relativewithpronominaldirectobject

‘Itistheonewherethegiraffeiswashingher’

b. C’estlagirafequelafillelave TargetObjectRelative

‘Itisthegiraffethatthegirliswashing’

(16) a. C’estlegarçonoùlechevalpeigne Where-relativewithoutpronominaldirectobject

‘Itistheboywherethehorseiscombing’

b. C’estlegarçonquelechevalpeigne TargetObjectRelative

‘Itistheboywhothehorseiscombing’

Althoughwhere-relativeswerenot observedin the SLIgroup orinthe TD6 groups,we foundpreciselythis typeof responseinTD5-year-olds,inasimilarelicitedproductiontask(Friedmannetal.,2013),andLabelle(1990)foundthemin elicitedproductioninTD3-to6-year-olds.Thisstrategyalsoallowsthechildtoavoidwh-movementofthedirectobject, therebysimplifyingthederivation.ThefactthatthisparticularavoidancestrategyisnotobservedinolderMMHLchildren, andalsothefactthattheolderchildrenwithMMHLproducedmorepassivesubjectrelativesarebothindicativeofwhat appearstobeanageeffect,withtheolderchildrenwithMMHLhavingmorematurelanguage.13

13Areviewersuggeststhatthiscouldalsomeanalinkwiththelengthofhearingaiduse.AsnotedinSection1,thisvariablemaycorrelatewith degreeofHL.ThishypothesiswasconfirmedinthestudybyDelage(2008),whereitwasfoundthatdegreeofHLinthese32childrenwithMMHL inverselycorrelatedwithageofHLdetection(rs= 0.41,p<.05)andfitting(rs= 0.44,p<.05).Thisresult,inconjunctionwiththefactthatsome childrenwithmildHLhaveneverwornhearingaidsatall,thuscomplicatesanydirectexplorationoflanguageperformanceandlengthofaided hearing.AswewillseeinSection3,nosuchlinkswerefound.

Références

Documents relatifs

Here, we report on the corona architecture formed after incubation of positive or negative silica particles with Curosurf®, a biomimetic pulmonary surfactant of porcine origin..

For other infrequent structural forms, children with SLI produced fewer Conjunctions, Pronouns, Relative pronouns + Interrogative pronouns + Interrogative adverbs

The best theory appeared to be pure phonological theory, but one parameter of this theory was not validated (salience) and the results obtained could not be fully explained by

Results showed that children with SLI had poorer results than children with TLD (typical language development), indicating that children with SLI have greater difficulty

Une recherche fédérale initiée par l’Institut National d’Assurance Maladie Invalidité (INAMI) a été menée afin d’évaluer les projets d’intégration des aides-

établissements secondaires avec pour ambition de reconfigurer ces lieux et de changer les missions des professionnels de la documentation en prenant en compte la

Considering that working memory capacities are important in perceiving complex rhythmic sequences (Tierney &amp; Kraus 2015) and that children with HL suffer from verbal

The cause of the language problems in  SLI is unknown but recent evidence  suggests that they might be secondary  to more general cognitive processing  limitations