• Aucun résultat trouvé

Chronic exposure to WIN55212-2 affects more potently spatial learning and memory in adolescents than in adult rats via a negative action on dorsal hippocampal neurogenesis

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Chronic exposure to WIN55212-2 affects more potently spatial learning and memory in adolescents than in adult rats via a negative action on dorsal hippocampal neurogenesis"

Copied!
8
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Chronic exposure to WIN55,212-2 affects more potently spatial learning and memory in adolescents than in adult rats via a negative action on dorsal hippocampal neurogenesis

Oualid Abboussia,

, Abdelouahhab Tazib, Eleni Paizanisc, Soumaya El Ganounia

aLaboratory of Biochemistry and Neurosciences, University Hassan 1er. Settat, Morocco

bDepartment of Pharmacology, Faculty of Medicine and Pharmacy. Casablanca, Morocco

cMemory and Behavioural Plasticity Group, University Caen Basse-Normandie, Caen, France

a b s t r a c t a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:

Received 8 October 2013

Received in revised form 20 February 2014 Accepted 20 February 2014

Available online 26 February 2014

Keywords:

Adolescence Cannabinoids Morris water maze Shuttle-box

Hippocampal neurogenesis

Several epidemiological studies show an increase in cannabis use among adolescents, especially in Morocco for being one of the major producers in the world. The neurobiological consequences of chronic cannabis use are still poorly understood. In addition, brain plasticity linked to ontogeny portrays adolescence as a period of vulner- ability to the deleterious effects of drugs. The aim of this study was to investigate the behavioral neurogenic effects of chronic exposure to the cannabinoid agonist WIN55,212-2 during adolescence, by evaluating the emotional and cognitive performances, and the consequences on neurogenesis along the dorso-ventral axis of the hippocampus in adult rats. WIN55,212 was administered intraperitoneally (i.p.) once daily for 20 days to adolescent (27–30 PND) and adult Wistar rats (54–57 PND) at the dose of 1 mg/kg. Following a 20 day washout period, emotional and cognitive functions were assessed by the Morris water maze test and the two-way active avoidance test. Twelve hours after, brains were removed and hippocampal neurogenesis was assessed using the doublecortin (DCX) as a marker for cell proliferation. Our results showed that chronic WIN55,212-2 treatment significantly increased thigmotaxis early in the training process whatever the age of treatment, induced spatial learning and memory deficits in adolescent but not adult rats in the Morris water maze test, while it had no significant effect in the active avoidance test during multitrial training in the shuttle box. In addition, the cognitive deficits assessed in adolescent rats were positively correlated to a decrease in the number of newly generated neurons in dorsal hippocampus. These data suggest that long term exposure to cannabinoids may affect more potently spatial learn- ing and memory in adolescent compared to adult rats via a negative action on hippocampal plasticity.

© 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

Cannabis is the most commonly used illicit substances in the world, especially among adolescents and young adults[1,2]. In Morocco, being one of the major producers in the world, and despite all the repressive and prevention measures deployed, the social and health issues related to the consumption of these drugs took on a large scale since thirty years ago[3]. Several epidemiological studies report the increasing consump- tion of cannabis in all social strata, especially among adolescents due to its easy access, and the fact that users generally perceive these drugs as relatively harmless[3].

The psychoactive effects of cannabis are mediated primarily by the cannabinoid CB1 receptor which is the most abundant of the Gi/o protein coupled receptors[4]. The CB1 cannabinoid receptor is highly expressed in the limbic regions that are involved in other functions, in learning and memory processes [5]. Several studies have assessed the

neurobehavioral consequences of cannabis. These studies have dem- onstrated that chronic exposure to cannabinoids, both in humans and animal models, results in behavioral decits in several physiological do- mains including; locomotor activity, attention, working memory, visuo- spatial skills, and executive functioning[68]. While the majority of these behavioral studies have been conducted mainly in adults, very lit- tle is known about the long term effects of chronic cannabis use during adolescence[9].

Adolescence is a critical phase for brain development characterized by a plethora of plastic neuronal changes which include; a sprouting and pruning of synapses, myelination of nervebers, changes in neuro- transmitter concentrations and their receptor levels in brain areas essential for the homeostasis of brain physiology[10,11]. The impor- tance of the endocannabinoidsignalling in brain development is well established. Endocannabinoids are fundamental cues controlling cell proliferation, migration and differentiation[12]. Interestingly, the role of endocannabinoids in shaping brain organization is operational dur- ing both pre and post-natal life[1214]. Suchndings suggest that long-term cannabinoid exposure during adolescence may produce

Corresponding author.

E-mail address:oualid.ab@gmail.com(O. Abboussi).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2014.02.014 0091-3057/© 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Contents lists available atScienceDirect

Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior

j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / p h a r m b i o c h e m b e h

(2)

maladaptive plasticity in brain structures involved in learning and memory, such as the hippocampus.

The role of adult neurogenesis in hippocampal plasticity has received considerable attention recently because of its important role in improv- ing and maintaining cognitive functions[1518]. This developmental process which was described in multiple species including; rodents, non-human primates and, most recently, in humans[1922]continue throughout adult life, in both the dorsal and ventral parts of the hippo- campus[23]. The hippocampal neurogenesis starts when the proliferat- ing neuroprogenitors of the dentate gyrus exit cell cycle and begin expressing neuronal markers, such as the doublecortin (DCX)[24].

Within approximately one month, the neural hippocampal progenitor cells located in the subgranular zone of the dentate gyrus give rise to neuroblasts that migrate into the granule cell layer, where they differ- entiate into functional hippocampal granule cells and some glial cells [2527]. This form of adult neurogenesis is modulated by several intrin- sic and/or extrinsic factors including genetic background, neurotrans- mitters (such as dopamine and serotonin), behavior, stress and drugs [25,2831]. However, the neural mechanisms underlying the cognitive impairments and brain structure's aberrations induced by chronic activation of CB1 receptors remain less clear[9,32,33]. Despite the high frequency of cannabis use, and the great pharmaceutical interest in developing new therapies from CB1 agonists[34], the effect of chronic activation of cannabinoid receptors (CB1) on neural cell pro- liferation and functioning in adolescent and adult rats, and their long-term developmental and neurobehavioral consequences have not received their due interest. Therefore, the aim of the present study is to investigate the effects of a potent synthetic cannabinoid receptor agonist (WIN55,212-2) on learning and memory performances and hippocampal neurogenesis by quantifying the expression of a reliable molecular marker of neurogenesis, the DCX, in the dorsal and the ven- tral parts of the hippocampus[35,36]. Our hypothesis is that the delete- rious effects of cannabinoids on cognitive functions may result from serious impairments in the hippocampal plasticity[37].

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Animals

A total of 42 adolescent (2730 PND) and adult (5457 PND) male Wistar rats were used in all the experiments (Fig. 1). They were housed in groups of four in each cage, with free access to food and water, and kept at a constant temperature (22 ± 2 °C) under a 12 h/12 h light dark cycle (light beginning at 7:00 a.m.). All experiments were carried out during the light phase between 9 a.m. and 5 p.m.

This study was conducted in conformity with approved institutional protocols, and carried out in accordance with the guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioral research and teaching[38].

2.2. Drug treatments

WIN55,212-2 (Tocris Bioscience, France), a potent aminoalkylindole CB1 receptor agonist (Ki = 62.3 and 3.3 nM for human cloned CB1 and CB2 receptors respectively), was dissolved in saline solution (0.9% NaCl solution) containing 5% Tween 80 and 5% DMSO.

Animals were assigned to 2 groups: adolescent and adult groups.

They were treated for 20 days with WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg, according to a preliminary study from our laboratory and others[3941]) or vehicle. The last drug treatment was followed by a 20 day drug washout period,rst to allow the adolescent groups to reach adulthood, and then to exclude the confounding effects of drug withdrawal (Fig. 1).

2.3. Behavioral assays

2.3.1. Spatial learning and memory task

Spatial learning and memory were tested using the Morris water maze paradigm (MWM)[42]. Animals were trained in a circular swim- ming pool (170 cm in diameter and 50 cm in deep). The maze was placed in a room surrounded by constant visual cues (posters, doors, win- dows) andlled with opaque water (22 ± 2 °C) to a depth of 35 cm.

The water maze was divided virtually into four equal quadrants (north, south, east, and west). In the center of the north quadrant a platform (12 cm in diameter) was placed at 12 cm beneath the surface of the water.

Animals were given four trials per day forve consecutive days. Each trial began from one of the four starting positions which are changed across days. During each trial, the rat was placed in the water facing the wall of the maze and given 90 s to locate the platform. If the animal fails to locate the platform, it was gently guided and allowed 20 s on top of the platform. After each trial, the animal is placed in a holding cage containing dry towels for 30 s before the next trial.

On the 6th day following the last day of training, a probe trial was conducted to assess retention of the platform location. During this trial, the platform is removed from the maze. Rats were given 90 s of free swimming. The animals which remember the position of the plat- form are expected to spend more time in the goal quadrant (north), and to cross it several times.

A webcam connected to a tracking system software Any-Maze (Stoelting) was used to analyze the swim path of each animal, the thig- motaxis (time of swimming in the outer 10% close to the pool walls), the time spent in goal quadrant, and the latency tond the hidden platform.

2.3.2. Two-way active avoidance learning

Emotional memory was assessed in a Shuttle Box LE916 (58 × 36 × 30.5 cm), which consists of two similar compartments equipped with in- dependent electriable gridoors, separated by a plank with a square hole (8 × 10 cm) in the center. Rats were allowed an easy access from one compartment to the other to avoid an electric shock (0.5 mA and

Rats Treated during adolescence: Win (n=10),

control (n=10)

Rats Treated during adulthood: Win (n=11),

control (n=11)

Washout

MWM test Win55

1mg/kg or Vehicle

IHC analysis

24h 12h

54-57 PND 74-77 94-97 >97

Adult (271.2±7.6 g)

27-30 PND 47-50 67-70 >70

Adolescent

(82±3.3 g) Adult (261.2±9.1 g)

Shuttle box test

Fig. 1.Schematic representation of the experimental design used to assess the behavioral effects of chronic administration of WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg) during adolescence and adulthood in Morris water maze (MWM) and shuttle box tests, followed by immunohistochemical (IHC) analysis. (PND: post natal days).

(3)

5 s of duration)delivered by the gridoor (unconditioned stimulus) directly after a visual and vocal stimulus (conditioned stimulus) gener- ated in the compartment in which the animal is detected by a weight transducer located above the grid.

A conditioned stimulus followed by an unconditioned stimulus is considered as a trial. The trials were given with a 25 s intertrial interval [43], and grouped into sessions consisting of 20 trials each.

Before testing, rats were placed in the apparatus for 10 min to explore the apparatus and to be familiar with the learning environment [44]. The behavioral assay lastedve days, with one session per day. The number of active avoidance that reects the learning performance of the animals was recorded.

2.3.3. Immunohistochemical staining

Twelve hours after the last session of each group, the rats were deeply anesthetized with pentobarbital sodium (60 mg/kg, i.p.) transcardially perfused with sodium nitrite solution (NaCl 0.9%, NaNO20.1%), followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) freshly prepared in phosphate sodium buffer (PBS 1×, pH 7.4). Brains were removed from the skulls, postxed overnight in PFA 4%. Sets of serial coronal sections along the axis of the rostrocaudal hippocampus were cut into 40-μm-thickness on a vibrat- ing blade microtome (Leica VT 1000S; Leica Microsystems, Nussloch, Germany), collected in PBS 1× and stored at20 °C in cryoprotectant solution (glycerol 30% vol/vol, ethylene glycol 30% vol/vol, 40% vol/vol PBS 1×).

The immunohistochemical staining was performed on free-oating sections rinsed in PBS 1 × three times for 5 min. After incubation in PBS 1 ×/3% H2O2/methanol 10%, followed by three washes for 5 min by PBS 1 ×, they were immersed for 1 h in a mixture of BSA (bovine serum albumin) 3%, 0.3% Triton X100 in 1× PBS (blocking buffer) before incubation overnight at 4 °C with the primary anti-DCX antibody (poly- clonal-goat 1:100 Santa Cruz Biotechnology, USA) diluted in blocking buffer. The day after, the sections were thoroughly rinsed in 1 × PBS (4 washes of 10 min) before being placed for 2 h at room temperature in blocking buffer, supplemented with the biotinylated anti-goat anti- body 1:200 (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA). They were then rinsed 3 times (10 min for each) in 1 × PBS before incubation for 2 h at room temperature with theavidinbiotin complex (A and B 1:200, Vectastain Elite kit, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) in 0.1% Triton X-100/PBS 1× mixture. Afterwards sections were rinsed 3 times for 10 min in 1× PBS, andnally revealed by incubation in 3,3- diaminobenzidine 0.05%/H2O20.01% in PBS 1× for 5 min followed by 3 washes of 10 min in 1 × PBS. Sections were then mounted on

super frostslides, air dried, rinsed in double distilled water, and dehydrated after a cresyl violet counterstaining, by incubation in increasing concentrations of alcohol (70%, 90% and 100%) solutions before being cover slipped using a polymerizing resin (Eukitt, Kindler, Freiburg, Germany).

2.3.4. Cell counting

Morphological quantications were performed blindly using a high resolution slide scanner Olympus VS120-SL (20 × objective), DCX- labeled cells were counted separately in the dorsal (interaural 3.70 to 6.20 mm); and ventral (interaural 2.28 to 3.70 mm) hippocampus[45].

In every 10th section at 320μm intervals in a total of 10 sections per animal (n = 20) as described by Banasr et al.[46]. Granule cell layer (GCL) volume was calculated according to the Cavalieri principle[47].

The area of the GCL in each section was computed using a package ImageJ software 1.42q (Wayne Rasband National Institutes of Health, USA) and the total number of DCX-positive cells per granule cell layer (GCL) vol- ume was determined.

2.3.5. Data analysis

Repeated-measures analysis of variance (two-way ANOVA) with treatment (vehicle or WIN55,212-2) and days of testing as dependent variables was used to analyze latency, thigmotaxis, and active avoidances.

When signicant, a post hoc LSD (least signicant difference) test was performed to compare treatment groups. While one-way ANOVA was used for the effects of WIN55,212-2 treated animals during adolescence and adulthood on Probe Trial Performance and DCX expression. Rela- tionships between behavioral performances and immunohistochemical data were evaluated by simple linear regression analysis. The level of signicance was set atpb0.05. Statistical analyses were carried out using StatView software (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). All analyses were conducted separately in adolescent and adult rats.

3. Results

3.1. Morris water maze

Therst objective of this study was to establish whether chronic exposure to WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg, for 20 days followed by 20 days washout period) would affect the anxiety-like behavior and acquisition of spatial memory in adolescent and adult rats tested in MWM task.

The results of WIN55,212-2 effect on spatial memory acquisition and thigmotaxis in adolescent and adult rats are summarized inFigs. 2 and 3 respectively. The time spent to locate the escape platform was grouped according to days (1 session per day, 4 trials per session).

Repeated measure ANOVA analyses showed a signicant effect of days on all groups. All animals learned to locate the hidden platform after 5 days of testing in less than 20 s (Fadolescents(1,86) = 100.002, pb0.0001, Fadults(1,86) = 139.214,pb0.0001,Fig. 2). A main effect of drug treatment (Fadolescents (1,86) = 34.42, p b0.0001) and

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80

1 2 3 4 5

Adults Vehicle Adults Win55 212-2

Escape latency (sec)

Days

B

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80

1 2 3 4 5

Adolescents Vehicle Adolescents Win55 212-2

Escape latency (sec)

Days

***

A

***

Fig. 2.The effect of chronic administration (i.p.) of WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg) during adoles- cence (A) and adulthood (B) on spatial memory acquisition in Morris water maze expressed by escape latency. Results are presented as means ± SEM. ***pb0.001 (post hoc Fisher's LSD).

(4)

treatmentday interaction (Fadolescents(4,86) = 7.853,pb0.0001) on spatial acquisition were observed in WIN55,212-2-treated rats during adolescence, as they spent longer time to reach the plat- form compared to their respective control group, while there was no signicant treatment effect among rats treated during adulthood (Fadults(1,86) = 1.960,pb0.1649).

One way ANOVA analysis of probe trial (Fig. 4) revealed that reten- tion of spatial memory was impaired in WIN55,212-2-treated rats during adolescence compared to the vehicle. Furthermore, Fisher's LSD post hoc comparison showed a signicant increase in the latency to swim over the previous location of the escape platform (p= 0.0002,Fig. 4A), no clear preference to swim in the goal quadrant con- taining the platform during learning sessions (p= 0.0004,Fig. 4B), and a decrease in time spent in the goal quadrant (p= 0.0005,Fig. 4C). On the contrary, no signicant effects on WIN55,212-2-treated rats during adulthood compared to their corresponding control group (p= 0.4032, p= 0.3186,p= 0.1197 respectively) were observed.

Thigmotaxis data analysis (Fig. 3) revealed a marked decline in time spent hugging the wall of the water maze in WIN55,212-2-treated rats during adolescence and adulthood, presenting a main effect of drug treatment (Fadolescents(1,86) = 13.627, p= 0.0004, Fadults(1,86) = 26.451, pb0.0001), days (Fadolescents(1,86) = 76.66,p = 0.0004, Fadults(1,86) = 212.051,pb0.0001), and treatmentday interac- tion (Fadolescents(4,86) = 16.465,p= 0.0001, Fadults(4,86) = 5.5442, pb0.0002). Post hoc LSD analysis comparing WIN55,212-2-treated rats to their respective control groups signicantly conrmed the

main effect of WIN55,212-2treatment that occurred particularly during the 1st and the 2nd day of testing (p0.0013,Fig. 3).

3.2. Active avoidance task

Active avoidance performance was assessed using the same groups of animals. All rats successfully learned to avoid the electric shock (Fig. 5); there was no signicant Win55 212-2-treatment ef- fect, (Fadolescents(1,20) = 0.072,p= 0.7911) (Fadults(1,20) = 0.353, p=0.5588), nor treatmentday interaction (Fadolescents(4,20) = 0.513, p= 0.7261) (Fadults(4,20) = 1.750,p= 0.8854). Only a main effect of days (Fadolescents(1,20) = 104.789,pb0.0001) (Fadults(1,20) = 94.795,pb0.0001) among rats treated during adolescence and adulthood was observed.

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80

Day1 Day2 Day3 Day4 Day5

Adults Vehicle Adults Win55 212-2

Thigmotaxis (sec)

***

***

B

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80

Day1 Day2 Day3 Day4 Day5

Adolescents Vehicle Adolescents Win55 212-2

***

***

A

Thigmotaxis (sec)

Fig. 3.The effect of chronic administration (i.p.) of WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg) during adoles- cence (A) and adulthood (B) on thigmotaxis expressed by the time of swimming in the outer 10% close to walls of the Morris water maze. Results presented by means ± SEM.

***pb0.001 (post hoc Fisher's LSD).

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14

Adolescents Adults

Vehicle

Win55 212-2

Latency (sec)

A

***

0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Adolescents Adults

Vehicle

Win55212-2

Number of crossing

B

***

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70

Adolescents Adults

Vehicle Win55 212-2

% Time spent in goal quadrant

***

C

Fig. 4.The effect of chronic administration (i.p.) of WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg) during adoles- cence and adulthood on spatial memory retrieval expressed by latency (A), number cross- ing (B) and time spent in the goal quadrant (C) in Morris water maze. Results are presented as means ± SEM. ***pb0.001.

(5)

3.3. Immunohistochemical results

Quantication of DCX-positive cells revealed a signicant effect of chronic administration of Win55 2122 on hippocampal neurogenesis in rats treated at adolescence (p= 0.0252) with a main effect on the dorsal part (pb0.0003) and non on the ventral part (p= 0.6981) as was conrmed by ANOVA post hoc analysis (Fig. 6A). In WIN55,212- 2-treated rats during adulthood, no signicant effects were found, as they exhibited similar rate of DCX-positive cell along the rostrocaudal axis in comparison to their respective control group(p= 0.3803, Fig. 6B).

3.4. Correlation between spatial learning and hippocampal neurogenesis The above results indicated that chronic treatment of adolescent rats by Win55 212-2 treatment impairs spatial learning and memory in Morris water maze test, and reduces the number of DCX-positive cell expressed in the dorsal part of the hippocampus, which can suggest a relationship between task performance and neurogenesis. Therefore, we analyzed the correlation between behavioral performance tested in the Morris water maze and cell proliferation using simple regression analysis. A positive correlation was found between the exhibited perfor- mances during acquisition and the rate of DCX-positive cell in the dorsal

hippocampus. Analysis of variance indicated that the linear regression linet to the data was statistically signicant (r(10) = 0.66,p= 0.0003), showing that the effect of chronic treatment on spatial learning and memory in adolescents is linked to a decrease in the neurogenesis in the dorsal hippocampus (Fig. 7).

4. Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to examine whether chronic exposure to cannabinoids may persistently and differentially affect emotional and cognitive behaviors and neuronal activity in adolescent and adult rats. In line with our hypothesis, chronic exposure to the can- nabinoid receptor agonist WIN55,212-2 at the dose of 1 mg/kg during adolescence but not during adulthood, leads to long-term deleterious effects on spatial learning and memory in Morris water maze paradigm.

This effect is likely to be underlined by an attenuation of dorsal hippo- campal neurogenesis.

4.1. Effects on anxiety-like behavior

We found that long-term exposure to WIN55,212-2during adoles- cence (2848 PND) and adulthood (7595 PND) signicantly increased time of thigmotaxis during therst and second days of Morris water maze testing carried out 20 days after the last treatment. This was dem- onstrated by a longer time spent by treated animals hanging on the wall of the MWM in comparison to their respective control groups. After the 2nd day of the training session, all animals demonstrated a signicant reduction in thigmotaxis across days.

Thigmotaxis is a reliable indicator of anxiety-like behavior during the Morris water maze testing in rodents. Anxiolytic agents are known to reduce the total duration of thigmotaxis[48,49]while anxiogenic stimuli such as systemic corticosteroids administration conversely in- crease thigmotaxis[5052]. Additionally, a correlation between the per- formances in the elevated plus maze and thigmotaxis was reported[51].

Taken together, the increase in thigmotaxis seen in our treated animals early in the training process is unlikely to reect a change in behavioral strategy to locate the platform. Rather, it would most likely reect an anxiogenic-like effect of WIN55,212-2. This conclusion is corroborated by other studies employing the openeld and social interaction tests to measure anxiety-like outputs, which report that chronic exposure to cannabinoids such as THC or synthetic cannabinoids (such as WIN55,212; CP55,940 or HU210) leads to increased anxiety- like behavior[5357]. Our study shows that in line with this, both ado- lescents and adults are equally sensitive to the deleterious effects of chronic exposure to cannabinoids.

4.2. Effects on cognition

The obtained results concerning the effects of chronic exposure to cannabinoids on cognitive behavior suggest that chronic exposure to WIN55,212-2 impairs more potently spatial learning and memory per- formance in adolescent than in adult rats. Animals treated during ado- lescence consistently performed poorly during the learning process and spatial memory retention in comparison to their respective control group. On the contrary, no signicant differences were observed in rats treated during adulthood.

However, no signicant effect of chronic exposure to cannabinoids during adolescence and adulthood on the acquisition of contextual information in a fear conditioning paradigm in the shuttle box was ob- served. This test is an operational measure for implicit memory in which the performances depend on the integrity of the hippocampus and the amygdala[5860]. Therefore, the lack of effect of WIN55,212-2 in the shuttle-box test may be explained by a differential effect of chronic exposure to cannabinoids during adolescence on the development of hippocampal neuronal circuitries and/or on the fact that the pro- cessing of distinct stimuli (such as a vocal signal) requires the 0

2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16

1 2 3 4 5

Adults Vehicle Adults Win55 212-2

Number of Active Avoidances

Days

B

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16

1 2 3 4 5

Adolescents Vehicle Adolescents Win55 212-2

Days

Number of Active Avoidances

A

Fig. 5.The effect of chronic administration (i.p.) of Win55,212-2 (1 mg/kg) during adoles- cence (A) and adulthood (B) on two-way active avoidance acquisition in shuttle-box.

Results presented by means ± SEM. *pb0.05.

(6)

amygdala, but not the hippocampus[61]. Nevertheless, our results suggest that chronic exposure to cannabinoids during adolescence may have differential effects on learning and memory depending on the aversivity of the context.

In agreement with our results, Quinn and O'shea suggest that chron- ic exposure to cannabinoids during adolescence (2850 PND) but not during adulthood (7090 PND) causes greater lasting memory decits in hippocampal-dependent tests[53,56]. However, May et al. reported no signicant lasting effects of chronic exposure to THC during

adolescence on either spatial or non-spatial learning in Sprague Dawley rats[62]. Likewise Higuera-Matas et al. found no signicant changes in anxiety and working memory tested using MWM and novel object recognition tests in adult Wistar rats treated at adolescence (2838 PND) with the cannabinoid agonist CP 55,940 (0.4 mg/kg)[63].

The discrepancies in these reports could be related to the different ago- nists used (WIN55,212-2 a full agonist while THC acts as a partial ago- nist), the duration of the treatment, or the duration of abstinence between the end of the chronic treatment and the behavioral testing and the specic test employed (see for review[64]). Generally, ournd- ings are in agreement with the residual deleterious effects on adult atten- tion and working memory assessed in early human cannabis users (b16 years)[6567].

4.3. Effects on hippocampal neurogenesis

Interestingly, the deleterious effect of chronic exposure to WIN55,212- 2 during adolescence on spatial learning and memory resembles the effects of hippocampal lesions on this form of memory[6870]suggest- ing that cannabinoid exposure during adolescence may alter memory formation via an interference with the normal maturation and/or function of the hippocampus. It has long been known that the hippo- campus is one of the brain areas with the highest density of CB1 receptors and a large amount of endocannabinoids (anandamide and 2-arachidonoylglycerol) which are responsible for the establishment of long-term potentiation through modulation of cAMP/PKA activity, a puta- tive mechanism for synaptic plasticity (for review, see[71]). Moreover, the endocannabinoid system plays a major role in axonal sprouting, hippocampal neural plasticity and adult neurogenesis[7274]. Thus, Interaural

6.20

Interaural 3.70

Interaural 3.70

Interaural 2.28

Dorsal Ventral

C

Ventral sections Dorsal sections

Control WIN55,212-2

D

A B

Fig. 6.Box plots showing individual jitter values of the expression of DCX-positive cells in dorsal (DH) and ventral (VH) hippocampal subregions of animals (n = 20) undergoing chronic WIN55,212-2 (1 mg/kg) treatment (i.p.) during adolescence (A) and adulthood (B). Results are presented as means ± SEM, *pb0.05, ***pb0.001.. (C) Illustration of dorsal and ventral hippocampus sections used for DCX cell quantification[40], and (D) representative photomicrographs of positive DCX-labeled cells in dorsal and ventral hippocampal sections from treated animals during adolescence.

DCX labelled cells/1mm3 4

5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14

Escape latency (sec)

20000 22000 24000 26000 28000 30000 32000 34000 r= 0.66, p=0.0003

Fig. 7.Correlation between escape latency during spatial training and number of DCX- labeled cells in dorsal hippocampus for rats treated during adolescence.

(7)

the long-term deleterious effects induced by adolescent exposure to WIN55,212-2 on spatial learning and memory could result from a perturbation in the normal development and/or maturation of the neu- ral hippocampal circuitries. In light of these facts we investigated the effects of chronic exposure to cannabinoids during adolescence on hip- pocampal neurogenesis. Our results showed that chronic exposure to WIN55,212-2 during adolescence but not during adulthood reduced cell proliferation in the hippocampus as demonstrated by a specic decrease in the number of positive-DCX cells expressed in the dorsal part of the hippocampus in comparison to the control group. No signif- icant changes were observed in the ventral part of the hippocampus.

This decrease in cell proliferation in the dorsal part of the hippocampus was positively correlated with the behavioral decits of the animals in spatial learning in the Morris water maze. These results suggest that chronic exposure to WIN55,212-2 may differentially alter hippocampal- dependent learning during spatial training in the Morris water maze via a negative action on dorsal hippocampus neurogenesis. Interestingly, functional segregation between the dorsal and the ventral parts of the hippocampus is well established with the dorsal hippocampus mediating primarily cognitive functions while the ventral hippocampus is more involved in emotional and neuroendocrine functioning[75,76]. This sub-regional functional segregation along the dorsalventral axis of the hippocampus could account as well for the lack of effects on the acquisi- tion of contextual information in a fear conditioning paradigm in the shuttle box. Few studies have investigated the effect of chronic exposure to WIN55,212-2 during adolescence on hippocampal neurogenesis. In line with ourndings Rueda et al. have shown that in vivo and in vitro chronic administration of an endogenous cannabinoid (the anandamide) signicantly inhibits the new born neurons in the rat dentate gyrus through attenuation of the Rap1/B-Raf/ERK pathway[77]. In contrast, other reports suggest that chronic exposure to cannabinoids actually promote hippocampal neurogenesis (for review[14,78]). Wen Jiang et al. have shown that repeated administration of a potent synthetic cannabinoid HU210 (100μg/kg) for 10 days increased the hippocampal neurogenesis[79]. Similarly, pharmacological and genetic disruption of the endogenous cannabinoid signaling enhances adult hippocampal neurogenesis[8082]. The inconsistencies between all these studies might be related principally to the methodological differences, the opposing effects induced by high and low doses of cannabinoids used [83,84], and the duration between treatment and the assessment of adult hippocampal neurogenesis[9].

5. Conclusion

In conclusion, our study adds to the increasing awareness of the vulnerability of the adolescent brain to the deleterious effects of cannabi- noids that affect potently spatial learning and memory in a hippocampal- dependent task[85,86]. This effect could be mediated through a negative action on cell proliferation in the dorsal part of the hippo- campus. Further investigations with careful consideration to the phylogenetic differences in hippocampus organization between rats and human[87]are however, required for a better understanding of the neuronal mechanisms underlying the adverse effects of cannabis on brain development.

References

[1] UNODCUnited Nations Office On Drugs And CrimeWorld Drug Report; 2013].

p. 20–48.

[2] Ramo DE, Liu H, Prochaska JJ.Tobacco and marijuana use among adolescents and young adults: a systematic review of their co-use. Clin Psychol Rev 2012];32(2):

105–21.

[3] El Omari F, Toufiq J.Chapitre 8. Le cannabis au Maroc: historique et épidémiologie.

Carrefour des psychothérapies; 2008]. p. 137–46.

[4] Herkenham M, Lynn AB, Johnson MR, Melvin LS, De Costa BR, Rice KC.Characteriza- tion and localization of cannabinoid receptors in rat brain: a quantitative in vitro au- toradiographic study. J Neurosci 1991];11(2):563–83.

[5] Devane WA, Dysarz III FA, Johnson MR, Melvin LS, Howlett AC.Determination and characterization of a cannabinoid receptor in rat brain. Mol Pharmacol 1988];34(5):

605–13.

[6] Akirav I.The role of cannabinoids in modulating emotional and non-emotional memory processes in the hippocampus. Front Behav Neurosci 2011];5.

[7] Marsicano G, Lafenêtre P.Roles of the endocannabinoid system in learning and memory. Behavioral neurobiology of the endocannabinoid system. Springer; 2009].

p. 201–30. (edn.).

[8] Ranganathan M, D'Souza DC.The acute effects of cannabinoids on memory in humans: a review. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006];188(4):425–44.

[9] Lorenzetti V, Lubman DI, Whittle S, Solowij N, Yücel M.Structural MRIfindings in long-term cannabis users: what do we know? Subst Use Misuse 2010];45(11):

1787–808.

[10] Rice D, Barone Jr S.Critical periods of vulnerability for the developing nervous sys- tem: evidence from humans and animal models. Environ Health Perspect 2000];

108(Suppl. 3):511.

[11] Spear LP.The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosc Biobehav Rev 2000];24(4):417–63.

[12] Mulder J, Aguado T, Keimpema E, Barabás K, Rosado CJB, Nguyen L, Monory K, Marsicano G, Di Marzo V, Hurd YL.Endocannabinoid signaling controls pyramidal cell specification and long-range axon patterning. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2008];105(25):

8760–5.

[13] Berghuis P, Dobszay MB, Wang X, Spano S, Ledda F, Sousa KM, Schulte G, Ernfors P, Mackie K, Paratcha G.Endocannabinoids regulate interneuron migration and morpho- genesis by transactivating the TrκB receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005];102(52):

19115–20.

[14] Berghuis P, Rajnicek AM, Morozov YM, Ross RA, Mulder J, Urbán GM, Monory K, Marsicano G, Matteoli M, Canty A.Hardwiring the brain: endocannabinoids shape neuronal connectivity. Sci Signal 2007];316(5828):1212.

[15] Van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao C, Gage FH.Exercise enhances learning and hippocam- pal neurogenesis in aged mice. J Neurosci 2005];25(38):8680–5.

[16] Lemaire V, Koehl M, Le Moal M, Abrous DN.Prenatal stress produces learning defi- cits associated with an inhibition of neurogenesis in the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2000];97(20):11032–7.

[17] Shors TJ, Miesegaes G, Beylin A, Zhao M, Rydel T, Gould E.Neurogenesis in the adult is involved in the formation of trace memories. Nature 2001];410(6826):372–6.

[18] Drapeau E, Mayo W, Aurousseau C, Le Moal M, Piazza P-V, Abrous DN.Spatial mem- ory performances of aged rats in the water maze predict levels of hippocampal neurogenesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2003];100(24):14385–90.

[19] Altman J, Das GD.Autoradiographic and histological evidence of postnatal hippo- campal neurogenesis in rats. J Comp Neurol 1965];124(3):319–35.

[20] Kaplan M, Hinds J.Neurogenesis in the adult rat: electron microscopic analysis of light radioautographs. Science 1977];197(4308):1092–4.

[21] Gould E, Tanapat P, McEwen BS, Flügge G, Fuchs E.Proliferation of granule cell pre- cursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1998];95(6):3168–71.

[22] Eriksson PS, Perfilieva E, Bjork-Eriksson T, Alborn AM, Nordborg C, Peterson DA, Gage FH.Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus. Nat Med 1998];4(11):

1313–7.

[23] Herrera DG, Yagüe AG, Johnsen-Soriano S, Bosch-Morell F, Collado-Morente L, Muriach M, Romero FJ, García-Verdugo JM.Selective impairment of hippocampal neurogenesis by chronic alcoholism: protective effects of an antioxidant. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2003];100(13):7919–24.

[24] Gleeson JG, Lin PT, Flanagan LA, Walsh CA.Doublecortin is a microtubule-associated protein and is expressed widely by migrating neurons. Neuron 1999];23(2):257–71.

[25] Zhao C, Deng W, Gage FH.Mechanisms and functional implications of adult neurogenesis. Cell 2008];132(4):645–60.

[26] Suh H, Consiglio A, Ray J, Sawai T, D'Amour KA, Gage FH.In vivo fate analysis reveals the multipotent and self-renewal capacities of Sox2+neural stem cells in the adult hippocampus. Cell Stem Cell 2007];1(5):515–28.

[27] Charles GG.Neurogenesis in the adult brain: death of a dogma. Nat Rev Neurosci 2000];1(1):67–73.

[28] Gould E, Beylin A, Tanapat P, Reeves A, Shors TJ.Learning enhances adult neurogenesis in the hippocampal formation. Nat Neurosci 1999];2(3):260–5.

[29] Kim S-E, Ko I-G, Kim B-K, Shin M-S, Cho S, Kim C-J, Kim S-H, Baek S-S, Lee E-K, Jee Y-S.Treadmill exercise prevents aging-induced failure of memory through an in- crease in neurogenesis and suppression of apoptosis in rat hippocampus. Exp Gerontol 2010];45(5):357–65.

[30] Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Gage FH.More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature 1997];386(6624):493–5.

[31] Kempermann G. Neurogenesis in the intact adult brain. In: Editor-in-Chief: Larry RS, editor. Encyclopedia of neuroscience. edn. Oxford: Academic Press; 2009: p. 443–447.

[32] Powles T, Poele RT, Shamash J, Chaplin T, Propper D, Joel S, Oliver T, Liu WM.

Cannabis-induced cytotoxicity in leukemic cell lines: the role of the cannabinoid re- ceptors and the MAPK pathway. Blood 2005];105(3):1214–21.

[33] Cousijn J, Wiers RW, Ridderinkhof KR, Van Den Brink W, Veltman DJ, Goudriaan AE.

Grey matter alterations associated with cannabis use: results of a VBM study in heavy cannabis users and healthy controls. Neuroimage 2012];59(4):3845–51.

[34] Bambico FR, Gobbi G.The cannabinoid CB1 receptor and the endocannabinoid anan- damide: possible antidepressant targets. Expert Opin Ther Targets 2008];12(11):

1347–66.

[35] Moser MB, Moser EI, Forrest E, Andersen P, Morris RG.Spatial learning with a minislab in the dorsal hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1995];92(21):9697–701.

[36] Bannerman DM, Rawlins JNP, Mchugh SB, Deacon RMJ, Yee BK, Bast T, Zhang WN, Pothuizen HHJ, Feldon J.Regional dissociations within the hippocampus—memory and anxiety. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004];28(3):273–83.

(8)

[37] Abush H, Akirav I.Cannabinoids modulate hippocampal memory and plasticity. Hip- pocampus 2010];20(10):1126–38.

[38] Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teaching. Anim Behav 2012];83(1):301–9.

[39] Polissidis A, Chouliara O, Galanopoulos A, Marselos M, Papadopoulou-Daifoti Z, Antoniou K.Behavioural and dopaminergic alterations induced by a low dose of WIN 55,212-2 in a conditioned place preference procedure. Life Sci 2009];85(5–6):

248–54.

[40] Baek Jh, Zheng Y, Darlington CL, Smith PF.The CB1 receptor agonist, WIN 55,212-2, dose-dependently disrupts object recognition memory in adult rats. Neurosci Lett 2009];464(1):71–3.

[41] Bambico FR, Nguyen N-T, Katz N, Gobbi G.Chronic exposure to cannabinoids during adolescence but not during adulthood impairs emotional behaviour and monoamin- ergic neurotransmission. Neurobiol Dis 2010];37(3):641–55.

[42] Morris R.Developments of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learning in the rat. J Neurosci Methods 1984];11(1):47–60.

[43] Carageorgiou H, Zarros A, Tsakiris S.Selegiline long-term effects on brain acetylcho- linesterase, (Na+, K+)-ATPase activities, antioxidant status and learning perfor- mance of aged rats. Pharmacol Res 2003];48(3):245–51.

[44] Torras-Garcia M, Costa-Miserachs D, Morgado-Bernal I, Portell-Cortés I.Improve- ment of shuttle-box performance by anterodorsal medial septal lesions in rats.

Behav Brain Res 2003];141(2):147–58.

[45] Paxinos G, Watson C.The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego, CA: Aca- demic; 1986].

[46] Banasr M, Soumier A, Hery M, Mocaër E, Daszuta A.Agomelatine, a new antidepres- sant, induces regional changes in hippocampal neurogenesis. Biol Psychiatry 2006];

59(11):1087–96.

[47] West MJ, Gundersen H.Unbiased stereological estimation of the number of neurons in the human hippocampus. J Comp Neurol 1990];296(1):1–22.

[48] Treit D, Fundytus M.Thigmotaxis as a test for anxiolytic activity in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1988];31(4):959–62.

[49] Simon P, Dupuis R, Costentin J.Thigmotaxis as an index of anxiety in mice. Influence of dopaminergic transmissions. Behav Brain Res 1994];61(1):59–64.

[50] Beiko J, Lander R, Hampson E, Boon F, Cain DP.Contribution of sex differences in the acute stress response to sex differences in water maze performance in the rat. Behav Brain Res 2004];151(1):239–53.

[51] Herrero AI, Sandi C, Venero C.Individual differences in anxiety trait are related to spatial learning abilities and hippocampal expression of mineralocorticoid receptors.

Neurobiol Learn Mem 2006];86(2):150–9.

[52] Snihur AW, Hampson E, Cain DP.Estradiol and corticosterone independently impair spatial navigation in the Morris water maze in adult female rats. Behav Brain Res 2008];187(1):56–66.

[53] Quinn HR, Matsumoto I, Callaghan PD, Long LE, Arnold JC, Gunasekaran N, Thompson MR, Dawson B, Mallet PE, Kashem MA.Adolescent ratsfind repeatedΔ9-THC less aversive than adult rats but display greater residual cognitive deficits and changes in hippocampal protein expression following exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007];33(5):1113–26.

[54] Schneider M, Koch M.Chronic pubertal, but not adult chronic cannabinoid treat- ment impairs sensorimotor gating, recognition memory, and the performance in a progressive ratio task in adult rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003];28(10):

1760–9.

[55] Schneider M, Koch M.The effect of chronic peripubertal cannabinoid treatment on deficient object recognition memory in rats after neonatal mPFC lesion. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2007];17(3):180–6.

[56] O'Shea M, Singh ME, Mcgregor IS, Mallet PE.Chronic cannabinoid exposure pro- duces lasting memory impairment and increased anxiety in adolescent but not adult rats. J Psychopharmacol 2004];18(4):502–8.

[57] Giuliani D, Ferrari F, Ottani A.The cannabinoid agonist HU 210 modifies rat behav- ioural responses to novelty and stress. Pharmacol Res 2000];41(1):45–51.

[58] Desmedt A, Marighetto A, Garcia R, Jaffard R.The effects of ibotenic hippocampal le- sions on discriminative fear conditioning to context in mice: impairment or facilita- tion depending on the associative value of a phasic explicit cue. Eur J Neurosci 2003];

17(9):1953–63.

[59] Kim JJ, Fanselow MS.Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science 1992];256(5057):675–7.

[60] Selden N, Everitt B, Jarrard L, Robbins T.Complementary roles for the amygdala and hippocampus in aversive conditioning to explicit and contextual cues. Neuroscience 1991];42(2):335–50.

[61] Phillips RG, Ledoux JE.Lesions of the dorsal hippocampal formation interfere with background but not foreground contextual fear conditioning. Learn Mem 1994];1(1):

34–44.

[62] Cha YM, White AM, Kuhn CM, Wilson WA, Swartzwelder HS.Differential effects of delta9-THC on learning in adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2006];83(3):448–55.

[63] Higuera-Matas A, Botreau F, Miguens M, Del Olmo N, Borcel E, Perez-Alvarez L, García-Lecumberri C, Ambrosio E.Chronic periadolescent cannabinoid treatment enhances adult hippocampal PSA-NCAM expression in male Wistar rats but only has marginal effects on anxiety, learning and memory. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2009];93(4):482–90.

[64] Viveros M, Llorente R, Moreno E, Marco E.Behavioural and neuroendocrine effects of cannabinoids in critical developmental periods. Behav Pharmacol 2005];16(5–6):

353–62.

[65] Solowij N, Battisti R.The chronic effects of cannabis on memory in humans: a re- view. Curr Drug Abuse Rev 2008];1(1):81–98.

[66] Bolla KI, Brown K, Eldreth D, Tate K, Cadet J.Dose-related neurocognitive effects of marijuana use. Neurology 2002];59(9):1337–43.

[67] Pope Jr HG, Gruber AJ, Hudson JI, Cohane G, Huestis MA, Yurgelun-Todd D.

Early-onset cannabis use and cognitive deficits: what is the nature of the associa- tion? Drug Alcohol Depend 2003];69(3):303–10.

[68] Moser E, Moser M-B, Andersen P.Spatial learning impairment parallels the magni- tude of dorsal hippocampal lesions, but is hardly present following ventral lesions.

J Neurosci 1993];13(9):3916–25.

[69] Aggleton J, Hunt P, Rawlins J.The effects of hippocampal lesions upon spatial and non-spatial tests of working memory. Behav Brain Res 1986];19(2):133–46.

[70] Hampson RE, Deadwyler SA.Cannabinoids reveal the necessity of hippocampal neu- ral encoding for short-term memory in rats. J Neurosci 2000];20(23):8932–42.

[71] Heifets BD, Castillo PE.Endocannabinoid signaling and long-term synaptic plasticity.

Annu Rev Physiol 2009];71:283–306.

[72] Oudin MJ, Hobbs C, Doherty P.DAGL-dependent endocannabinoid signalling: roles in axonal pathfinding, synaptic plasticity and adult neurogenesis. Eur J Neurosci 2011];34(10):1634–46.

[73] Chevaleyre V, Takahashi KA, Castillo PE.Endocannabinoid-mediated synaptic plas- ticity in the CNS. Annu Rev Neurosci 2006];29:37–76.

[74] Lee TTY, Wainwright SR, Hill MN, Galea LA, Gorzalka BB.Sex, drugs, and adult neurogenesis: sex‐dependent effects of escalating adolescent cannabinoid exposure on adult hippocampal neurogenesis, stress reactivity, and amphetamine sensitiza- tion. Hippocampus 2014];24(3):280–92.

[75] Fanselow MS, Dong H-W.Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally dis- tinct structures? Neuron 2010];65(1):7–19.

[76] Bannerman D, Rawlins J, Mchugh S, Deacon R, Yee B, Bast T, Zhang W-N, Pothuizen H, Feldon J.Regional dissociations within the hippocampus—memory and anxiety.

Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004];28(3):273–83.

[77] Rueda D, Navarro B, Martı́nez-Serrano A, Guzmán M, Galve-Roperh I. The endocannabinoid anandamide inhibits neuronal progenitor cell differentiation through attenuation of the Rap1/B-Raf/ERK pathway. J Biol Chem 2002];277(48):

46645–50.

[78] Jin K, Xie L, Kim SH, Parmentier-Batteur S, Sun Y, Mao XO, Childs J, Greenberg DA.

Defective adult neurogenesis in CB1 cannabinoid receptor knockout mice. Mol Pharmacol 2004];66(2):204–8.

[79] Jiang W, Zhang Y, Xiao L, Van Cleemput J, Ji S-P, Bai G, Zhang X.Cannabinoids pro- mote embryonic and adult hippocampus neurogenesis and produce anxiolytic- and antidepressant-like effects. J Clin Invest 2005];115(11):3104–16.

[80] Palazuelos J, Aguado T, Egia A, Mechoulam R, Guzmán M, Galve-Roperh I.Non- psychoactive CB2 cannabinoid agonists stimulate neural progenitor proliferation.

FASEB J 2006];20(13):2405–7.

[81] Aguado T, Romero E, Monory K, Palazuelos J, Sendtner M, Marsicano G, Lutz B, Guzmán M, Galve-Roperh I.The CB1 cannabinoid receptor mediates excitotoxicity- induced neural progenitor proliferation and neurogenesis. J Biol Chem 2007];282(33):23892–8.

[82] Fogaca MV, Galve-Roperh I, Guimaraes FS, Campos AC.Cannabinoids, neurogenesis and antidepressant drugs: is there a link? Curr Neuropharmacol 2013];11(3):

263–75.

[83] Tzavara ET, Wade M, Nomikos GG.Biphasic effects of cannabinoids on acetylcholine release in the hippocampus: site and mechanism of action. J Neurosci 2003];23(28):

9374–84.

[84] Sulcova E, Mechoulam R, Fride E.Biphasic effects of anandamide. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1998];59(2):347–52.

[85] Rubino T, Parolaro D.Long lasting consequences of cannabis exposure in adoles- cence. Mol Cell Endocrinol 2008];286(1–2, Supplement 1):S108–13.

[86] Schneider M.Puberty as a highly vulnerable developmental period for the conse- quences of cannabis exposure. Addict Biol 2008];13(2):253–63.

[87] Sasaki M, Tohyama K, Matsunaga S, Nakamura M, Tomizawa N, Inoue T, Ogawa H, Ehara S, Ogawa A.MRI identification of dorsal hippocampus homologue in human brain. NeuroReport 2004];15(14):2173–6.

Références

Documents relatifs

We display in Fig. 8 the raster plot of the parietal map’s reach cells and the trajectory in space of the left hand in which we superimposed the activity of four cells with

Consistent with the earlier reports of beneficial effects of CLZ on other prenatal infection-induced behavioral and cognitive abnormalities (Shi et al. 2006 ), we found that chronic

directional firing of hippocampal neurons. Goal-related activity in hippocampal place cells. Accumulation of hippocampal place fields at the goal location in an annular watermaze

Blockade of extracellular S100A4 in R-SMCs with S100A4 neutralizing antibody induced a transition from R- to S-phenotype, decreased proliferative activity and upregulation of

(c) Cumulative distribution of the population correlation values across spatial bins for pairs of control and delayed ensembles of place cells recorded in

ACCORD-MIND: The Memory in Diabetes ( MIND) sub study of the Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes ( ACCORD); ADA: American Diabetes Association; CCCE:

Although we are the first to demonstrate the protective effect of Cit on the expression of functional plasticity in the ageing hippocampus, other studies have already assessed

L’ensemble des résultats obtenus dans ce chapitre conforte notre stratégie pour l’optimisation des coûts de production dans le réseau électrique, nous notons que