• Aucun résultat trouvé

What goes up must come down: EEG phase modulates auditory perception in both directions.

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "What goes up must come down: EEG phase modulates auditory perception in both directions."

Copied!
4
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-00797497

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00797497

Submitted on 6 Mar 2013

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access

archive for the deposit and dissemination of

sci-entific research documents, whether they are

pub-lished or not. The documents may come from

teaching and research institutions in France or

abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est

destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents

scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non,

émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de

recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires

publics ou privés.

What goes up must come down: EEG phase modulates

auditory perception in both directions.

Rufin Vanrullen, Douglas Mclelland

To cite this version:

Rufin Vanrullen, Douglas Mclelland. What goes up must come down: EEG phase modulates

audi-tory perception in both directions.. Frontiers in Psychology, Frontiers, 2013, 4, pp.16.

�10.3389/fp-syg.2013.00016�. �hal-00797497�

(2)

GENERAL COMMENTARY

published: 28 January 2013 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00016

What goes up must come down: EEG phase modulates

auditory perception in both directions

Rufin VanRullen1,2* and Douglas McLelland1,2

1Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition, Université Paul Sabatier, Université de Toulouse, Toulouse, France 2

CNRS, CerCo, Toulouse, France

*Correspondence: rufin.vanrullen@cerco.ups-tlse.fr

Edited by:

Gregor Thut, University of Glasgow, UK

Reviewed by:

Gregor Thut, University of Glasgow, UK

A commentary on

A precluding but not ensuring role of entrained low-frequency oscillations for auditory perception

by Ng, B. S., Schroeder, T., and Kayser, C. (2012). J. Neurosci. 32, 12268–12276. Recent years have seen mounting evidence that the phase of low-frequency EEG oscillations—particularly in the theta (4–8 Hz) and alpha (8–13 Hz) frequency ranges—is closely related to visual per-ception (Busch et al., 2009; Mathewson et al., 2009; Busch and VanRullen, 2010; Drewes and VanRullen, 2011; Dugue et al., 2011; VanRullen et al., 2011; Chakravarthi and VanRullen, 2012; Hamm et al., 2012; Romei et al., 2012; Schyns et al., 2012). In a recently published article, Ng and colleagues report, for the first time, that auditory perception also depends on the phase of theta EEG oscillations (Ng et al., 2012)—an exciting novel find-ing suggestfind-ing that brain oscillations have perceptual consequences in multi-ple sensory modalities. This successful demonstration after previously failed attempts by other groups (Zoefel and Heil, unpublished) (Ilhan and VanRullen, unpublished) may be owed, in part, to the analysis of EEG oscillations that were not spontaneously produced by the brain, but rather evoked (or “entrained”) by an audi-tory background stimulation (Large and Jones, 1999; Lakatos et al., 2008; Henry and Herrmann, 2012). In line with this idea, another recent report indicated that entrainment of brain oscillations by 10 Hz periodic transcranial electric stimulation of auditory regions induces a periodic fluctuation of auditory sensitivity at the same frequency (Neuling et al., 2012).

While the primary finding of a peri-odic modulation of auditory perception by EEG oscillations is a major novel result that we would not wish to contest, there is another conclusion of the study by

Ng et al.(2012) that merits elaborating: the EEG phase modulation they reported was higher for misses (trials in which the auditory target was present but unde-tected) than for hits (detected targets). Based on these unexpected results they proposed a “precluding” model of phase modulation, in which the “duty cycle” of phase modulation would be signifi-cantly longer than 50% (we define “duty cycle” as the proportion of a full cycle for which performance is enhanced com-pared to baseline; a sinusoidal modula-tion would result in a 50% duty cycle). In their model, the short period of the cycle during which performance is impaired (less than 50% of the cycle) would cre-ate a “window of no-opportunity” for auditory perception, while the remain-ing phases (the majority of the cycle) would all be equally favorable to percep-tion. Based on this “precluding” model the authors were able to account for the more intense modulation observed for misses.

There is an inherent conceptual contra-diction, however, in proposing that misses can be more modulated by oscillatory phase than hits. Since any target-present trial is either a hit or a miss, any increase in the probability of a miss at certain phases should normally be accompanied by an equivalent decrease in the proba-bility of a hit, and vice-versa. The prob-abilities of hits and misses should thus be modulated by oscillatory phase by the same amount, only in opposite direc-tions. How couldNg et al.(2012) observe

a larger effect for misses than for hits? We suggest that this happened because these authors based their calculations on the relative number of hits and misses observed around each phase value, with-out taking into account the fact that all phase values were not equally sampled in their paradigm (Figure 1). In mathemat-ical terms, they measured p(miss&phase), the joint probability of observing nega-tive performance at a given phase, but drew conclusions about p(miss|phase), the actual probability of observing negative performance given a certain phase; as according to Bayes’ law, p(miss|phase) = p(miss&phase)/p(phase), it appears these authors neglected to take into account the prior distribution of phases across trials, p(phase). If we postulate that the non-uniform prior distribution of phases was, perhaps by chance, biased toward a phase value that was detrimental for auditory perception (Figure 1A; see also Figure 3D in Ng et al., 2012), then the alignment of the prior distribution peak with the peak of the miss probability will result in an enhanced phase mod-ulation of the joint probability distribu-tion for misses; in contrast, the align-ment of the prior distribution peak with the trough of the hit probability will suppress the phase modulation of the joint probability distribution for hits. In this way, the results of Ng et al. (2012) can be accounted for (Figures 1B and

C) without resorting to a “precluding” model with an unconventional duty cycle (indeed, our simulations relied on a stan-dard sinusoidal modulation with a 50% duty cycle). We readily acknowledge, nat-urally, that this alternative account does not categorically rule out a possible imbal-ance in the duty cycle—only future direct

(3)

VanRullen and McLelland EEG phase and auditory perception

experimental measurements of this duty cycle can resolve this question.

Why was the phase non-uniformly distributed across trials? The clever paradigm used by Ng et al. (2012) should in theory guarantee such a uni-form distribution. They presented their auditory target at one of six different moments following the onset of an audi-tory noise sequence (1500–2500 ms in steps of 200 ms). With the assumption that the sequence onset would reset a theta oscillation at 4 Hz, these chosen moments should have provided a uni-form sampling of 4 Hz oscillatory phases. Unfortunately, this ingenious paradigm critically depends on the assumption that the relevant oscillation—the one that is phase-reset by stimulus onset and subsequently modulates auditory perception—is precisely at 4 Hz. The cor-responding oscillatory band was extracted from EEG data filtered between 2 and 6 Hz, implying that the relevant oscilla-tion might lie anywhere within this range. Any departure from the assumed value of 4 Hz would unfortunately have pro-duced a non-uniform phase distribution (for example, with a relevant oscilla-tion at 5 Hz, which was also the rate of sampling of auditory performance in this paradigm, only a single phase value would be recorded across the different samples).

If our account is correct, then the asym-metric effect of phase on auditory percep-tion (stronger for misses than for hits) is a direct consequence of the unfortunately biased sampling of auditory performance around a phase value that impairs perfor-mance. We thus predict that this asymme-try will be reversed if the experiment is repeated but sampling auditory perception at six delays between 1600 and 2600 ms (by steps of 200 ms) instead of between 1500 and 2500 ms. At these delays, the non-uniform sampling of phases will be biased toward a phase that favors hits rather than misses. This prediction should be prop-erly tested before embracing the less par-simonious “precluding” model of phase modulation. In the meantime, we renew our congratulations to Ng and colleagues for demonstrating that brain oscillations have perceptual consequences not just in the visual, but also in the auditory domain.

FIGURE 1 | (A) An equal modulation of hit and miss rates by oscillatory phase (left) can result in an apparent imbalance between hits and misses if it is accompanied by a non-uniform prior probability distribution of phase values sampled across all trials (right). This non-uniform distribution may arise from a phase-reset of oscillations by the experimental stimulation and/or an inadequate sampling of auditory performance. (B) Because the most likely phase value also happens to correspond to the angle that minimizes hit rate (and maximizes miss rate), the number of hits across phases (red curve) is near-uniform; in contrast, the number of misses (blue curve) is strongly biased toward this expected phase. The data in this panel correspond to one simulated subject in the experiment by

Ng et al.(2012). The simulation directly relies on the phase modulation and the prior distribution of phases depicted in panel (A). (C) As in the study byNg et al.(2012), the difference between the distributions of hits and misses for 12 simulated subjects were averaged after each subject’s phase angles were aligned to the phase that maximized misses (blue curve) or hits (red curve). Error bars denote s.e.m. across subjects. A clear modulation is only apparent when phases are aligned to the angle that maximizes misses, not hits.

(4)

VanRullen and McLelland EEG phase and auditory perception

REFERENCES

Busch, N. A., Dubois, J., and VanRullen, R. (2009). The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations predicts visual perception. J. Neurosci. 29, 7869–7876. Busch, N. A., and VanRullen, R. (2010). Spontaneous

EEG oscillations reveal periodic sampling of visual attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 16048–16053.

Chakravarthi, R., and VanRullen, R. (2012). Conscious updating is a rhythmic pro-cess. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 10599–10604.

Drewes, J., and VanRullen, R. (2011). This is the rhythm of your eyes: the phase of ongoing electroencephalogram oscillations modu-lates saccadic reaction time. J. Neurosci. 31, 4698–4708.

Dugue, L., Marque, P., and VanRullen, R. (2011). The phase of ongoing oscillations mediates the causal relation between brain excitation and visual perception. J. Neurosci. 31, 11889–11893. Hamm, J. P., Dyckman, K. A., McDowell, J. E., and

Clementz, B. A. (2012). Pre-cue fronto-occipital alpha phase and distributed cortical oscillations predict failures of cognitive control. J. Neurosci. 32, 7034–7041.

Henry, M. J., and Herrmann, B. (2012). A preclud-ing role of low-frequency oscillations for audi-tory perception in a continuous processing mode.

J. Neurosci. 32, 17525–17527.

Lakatos, P., Karmos, G., Mehta, A. D., Ulbert, I., and Schroeder, C. E. (2008). Entrainment of neu-ronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science 320, 110–113.

Large, E. W., and Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: how people track time-varying events.

Psychol. Rev. 106, 119–159.

Mathewson, K. E., Gratton, G., Fabiani, M., Beck, D. M., and Ro, T. (2009). To see or not to see: prestimulus alpha phase predicts visual awareness.

J. Neurosci. 29, 2725–2732.

Neuling, T., Rach, S., Wagner, S., Wolters, C. H., and Herrmann, C. S. (2012). Good vibrations: oscil-latory phase shapes perception. Neuroimage 63, 771–778.

Ng, B. S., Schroeder, T., and Kayser, C. (2012). A precluding but not ensuring role of entrained low-frequency oscilla-tions for auditory perception. J. Neurosci. 32, 12268–12276.

Romei, V., Gross, J., and Thut, G. (2012). Sounds reset rhythms of visual cortex and corresponding

human visual perception. Curr. Biol. 22, 807–813.

Schyns, P. G., Thut, G., and Gross, J. (2012). Cracking the code of oscillatory activity. PLoS Biol. 9:e1001064. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001064 VanRullen, R., Busch, N. A., Drewes, J., and

Dubois, J. (2011). Ongoing EEG phase as a trial-by-trial predictor of perceptual and atten-tional variability. Front. Psychology 2:60. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00060

Received: 19 December 2012; accepted: 08 January 2013; published online: 28 January 2013.

Citation: VanRullen R and McLelland D (2013) What goes up must come down: EEG phase modulates audi-tory perception in both directions. Front. Psychology

4:16. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00016

This article was submitted to Frontiers in Perception Science, a specialty of Frontiers in Psychology. Copyright © 2013 VanRullen and McLelland. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of theCreative Commons Attribution License, which per-mits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

Figure

FIGURE 1 | (A) An equal modulation of hit and miss rates by oscillatory phase (left) can result in an apparent imbalance between hits and misses if it is accompanied by a non-uniform prior probability distribution of phase values sampled across all trials

Références

Documents relatifs

• AV localization and activity detection: the theoretical CMM framework was ap- plied to the task of localization of multiple AV objects; we consider the AV integra- tion task in the

IOMATCH() must be disabled to prevent a read from terminating prematurely when the binary data appears as a decimal 10 (ie.. The format of the data stream into the Logic

The boot record program uses this data to determine the drive being booted from and the location of the partition on the disk. If no active partition table enty is found,

Mr Koïchiro Matsuura welcomed the Chairperson of the first session of the General Assembly, the Chairperson of the Intergovernmental Committee and

Ne croyez pas que le simple fait d'afficher un livre sur Google Recherche de Livres signifie que celui-ci peut etre utilise de quelque facon que ce soit dans le monde entier..

first, we quantified BOLD oscillations to sound processing in general since we aimed at fi nding general dynamic BOLD oscillations to tempo- rally extended sounds; second, we quanti fi

The Canadian Primary Care Sentinel Surveillance Network, a Pan-Canadian project led by the CFPC that conducts standardized surveillance on selected chronic

Even though polycentric spatial planning has been the core concept of most policies at national and regional level since the 1950's, it has hardly ever