• Aucun résultat trouvé

How to measure food liking in a dynamic way in 8-10 year-old children? A new method

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "How to measure food liking in a dynamic way in 8-10 year-old children? A new method"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-01577107

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01577107

Submitted on 5 Jun 2020

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

How to measure food liking in a dynamic way in 8-10 year-old children? A new method

Christine Lange, Claire Chabanet, Michel Visalli, Sophie Nicklaus, Camille Schwartz

To cite this version:

Christine Lange, Claire Chabanet, Michel Visalli, Sophie Nicklaus, Camille Schwartz. How to measure

food liking in a dynamic way in 8-10 year-old children? A new method. Annual meeting of the Society

for the Study of Ingestive Behavior (SSIB), Jul 2017, Montreal, Canada. 1 p. �hal-01577107�

(2)

t = 1.49, df = 44, P= 0.14 Static Liking

Snack #1

Dynamic Liking Snack #2

Static Liking Snack #3*

Dynamic Liking Snack #4*

How to measure food liking during the course of a meal in 8-10 year-old children? A new method

Christine Lange, Claire Chabanet, Sophie Nicklaus, Michel Visalli, Camille Schwartz*

1

UMR Centre des Sciences du Goût et de l'Alimentation, AgroSup Dijon, CNRS, INRA, Université Bourgogne Franche-Comté, DIJON, France

Objectives

Results

Ethics committee of Est I Bourgogne (2016- A00498-43)

• Two primary schools from Dijon - 16 children from school 1 and 31 from school 2

• Afternoon snack time (i.e. two rounds:

4.15 pm and 5pm)

• Children were split into 4 groups of 4 or 5 children, supervised by one experimenter each

• In the canteen of the school

1/ To develop a new method to measure the temporal aspects of liking assessment in 8-10 year-old children in ecological conditions of consumption and to evaluate the reproducibility over two sessions (Dynamic liking).

2/ To compare a regular liking evaluation (static liking) with the newly developed temporal evaluation of liking (Static liking vs Dynamic liking).

3/ To explore the links between variations in liking (in the dynamic method) and variations in the level of hunger (Dynamic liking (individual slope) and level of hunger).

Food liking is most often assessed once after the consumption of a small amount of product.

Nevertheless, eating a food is not a matter of a single bite and liking may change over consumption as a result of satiation.

In adults, changes in liking can be assessed with a continuous approach - at any change throughout the consumption (at the discretion of the participant then) or with a discontinuous approach - at predefined time points or particular events (e.g. after swallowing) over the course of consumption (see for a review: Delarue & Blumental, 2015). To date, no dynamic methodology is available to assess liking through portion consumption over the course of a meal in children (Laureati, 2015).

*Sessions 3 and 4 were the replication of sessions 1 and 2 respectively

Timeline of the study:

Participants (N=47) 19 boys - 28 girls

2/ Static liking vs Dynamic liking 3/ Dynamic liking and level of hunger **

Measures:

Liking (both static & dynamic)

Level of Hunger

Timeline of each type of measure:

Static Liking 1- Level of hunger 2- Training liking 3- Static liking assessment 4- Preference ranking 5- Level of hunger

Dynamic Liking

1- Training changes in color brightness (video) 2- Level of hunger

3- Story to imagine how children would enjoy each supplementary portion of french fries for lunch + training changes in food liking (video)

4- Dynamic liking assessment: The children were informed about the number of jars and that it was the same fruit puree only if they asked the question.

5- Level of hunger

• Although the results are very encouraging, some refinements could be made to the dynamic liking assessment: children should be told that this is the same food product, more videos should be displayed with more examples of putative changes in liking.

• More research is needed to explore the relationships between the level of hunger and temporal liking evaluations in children. It would be interesting to evaluate dynamic liking in children with more or less satiating foods to observe whether this will impact dynamic liking over the consumption of a portion.

• More research is needed to explore the underlying reasons for a positive slope in some children. An exposure effect during consumption might explain this result.

“You have just tasted a jar of purée, did you enjoy it ?”

Acknowledgements :

Mélanie Buffet, Gwendoline Rainette, Catherine Pedron et Emilie Szleper

Funding: CSGA grant (API)

camille.schwartz@inra.fr / christine.lange@inra.fr Liking score (0-10)

adapted from Faith et al., 2002 and Bennett & Blissett, 2014 2 scales one for girls, one for boys

“Right now, how hungry are you? Is your stomach empty or full?”

Hunger score (1-5) level of hunger = hunger after–before tasting (if > 0 = hunger decreases)

80% of the children have a negative slope (liking=f(number of jars)

→The average individual slope is -0.31 The standard deviation of individual slopes is 0.34

Linear mixed model (REML estimator) with children effect considered as random:

liking=intercept + jar + snack + jar*snack

→The jar effect was significantF(1,465) = 23.52; P < 0.0001

→The interaction jar*snack was not significant

→On average liking decreased over consumption. This was reproducible over two sessions.

1/ Validation of the dynamic liking method

Linear mixed model (REML estimator) with children effect considered as random:

liking=intercept + order + snack + products+ experimenter + order*snack + product*snack + experimenter*snack

→Significant differences in liking were observed among products (F(2,194) = 11.73; P < 0.0001) apple+blackcurrant was significantly less liked than apple+cherry and apple+pear

→The product*snack interaction was not significant

No significant difference in liking between static liking and liking for jar 1 with the dynamic liking for the same apple+ pear puree, but jar 6 was less liked

Distribution of the individual slopes

level of hunger: Mean= 1.48 ±1.25 (min -2.5 ; max 3.5) in Liking¤: Mean= -0.30 ±0.42 (min -1.1 ; max 0.6) Regression:

in Liking = -0.10 level of hunger P=0.0475

• A majority of children had a negative slope, denoting a liking decrease over consumption. At the group level, the dynamic liking assessment was reproducible over two snacks. The evaluation of the first jar with the dynamic liking method was not significantly different from the liking evaluation of the same puree with the static liking method.

• Although not strongly, the decrease in hunger is linked to the decrease in liking over consumption.

• The numerous pretests carried out in two leisure children centers with 25 other children aged 8-10 years old beforehand enabled to establish children-tailored scales and instructions together with the experimental conditions. Training with videos were very helpful to introduce children with the notion that liking may change over time. In this regard, the developed question to evaluate liking was “You have just tasted a jar of purée, did you enjoy it ?” rather than “How much did you like it now”. The former enabled children to evaluate liking several successive times.

→→

→→On average, the level of hunger decreases so as the liking.

The decrease in hunger is significantly associated with the decrease in liking

apple + pear apple + cherry apple + blackcurrant

40g / jar whatever the product

6 jars 6 jars

Method

1.1 Static Liking

Jar

Liking (95% confidence intervals)

0 2 4 6 8 10

1 2 3 4 5 6

1.2 Dynamic Liking

¤ in Liking = mean of the 2 individual slopes from the linear mixed model from the two snacks, for each child

** The analysis was restricted to children who participated in the two dynamic liking assessment snacks n=40

Context

Conclusions Perspectives

0 2 4 6 8 10

Our liking scale enables to higlight differences in liking for different fruit purees ; this was reproducible over two sessions. Our scale can be used for the dynamic method.

Liking Frequencyand estimateddensity

Liking

Liking (mean ± SD)

I am VERY hungry! My stomach is really empty and is rumbling a lot

I am a little hungry!

My stomach is a little empty and is rumbling a little

I am a neither hungry nor

full

I am not very hungry. My stomach is a little full but I still have some room

I am not hungry AT ALL. My stomach is completely full and I can not eat anymore

95% Confidence interval

A B B

Static Dyn – Jar 1 Dyn – Jar 6

-1.5 -1.0 -0.5 0.0 0.5

1 2 3 4 5 6

Références

Documents relatifs

explain the complicating events (first and second push) and how; 2. make reference to the internal states of the characters, with particular attention to the epistemic

dantes, avant compensation (à gauche), après compensation par conjugaison de phase (au milieu) et après compensation par dark hole non linéaire (à droite) ; En bas, profils de

We have presented a new method for estimating ionospheric electric fields and currents using ground magnetic recordings and ionospheric electric conductances as input data..

Because the 3-MST, CBS, and CSE inhibitors used here would inhibit the activity of the respective enzymes in host (mouse) cells, which could potentially affect the host response

The Child Food Rejection Scale: Development and validation of a new scale to assess food neophobia and pickiness among 2- to 7-year-old French children... The two strongest obstacles

We concentrated on French children aged 2-7 years, as no such scale exists for this young population, and on the two known dimensions of food rejection, namely

Overall, the eight CHILDREN pillars were discussed by both deliverer and governor organisations as well as those working at the local national and international levels. The eight

Using a sorting procedure, Bovet and Vauclair (2001) observed that at age 3 the majority of the children successfully sorted food and toys pictures into different boxes, thus