• Aucun résultat trouvé

Stereological analysis of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Stereological analysis of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex"

Copied!
18
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Stereological analysis of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex

Olivia Piguet

1

| Loïc J. Chareyron

2

| Pamela Banta Lavenex

1

| David G. Amaral

3,4

|

Pierre Lavenex

1,2

1

Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Development, Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, 1015 Lausanne, Switzerland

2

Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Development, Department of Medicine, University of Fribourg, 1700 Fribourg, Switzerland

3

Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, MIND Institute, University of California, Davis, California

4

California National Primate Research Center, University of California, Davis, California Correspondence

Pierre Lavenex, Laboratory of Brain and Cognitive Development, Institute of Psychology, University of Lausanne, 1015 Lausanne, Switzerland.

Email: pierre.lavenex@unil.ch Funding information

National Institutes of Health, Grant/Award Number: MH041479NS16980; Swiss National Science Foundation, Grant/Award Number: 310030_143956P00A-106701PP00P3-124536; California National Primate Research Center, Grant/Award Number: OD011107

Abstract

The entorhinal cortex is a prominent structure of the medial temporal lobe, which plays a pivotal role in the interaction between the neocortex and the hippocampal formation in support of declarative and spatial memory functions. We implemented design-based stereological tech-niques to provide estimates of neuron numbers, neuronal soma size, and volume of different layers and subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex in adult rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta; 5–9 years of age). These data corroborate the structural differences between different subdivi-sions of the entorhinal cortex, which were shown in previous connectional and cytoarchitec-tonic studies. In particular, differences in the number of neurons contributing to distinct afferent and efferent hippocampal pathways suggest not only that different types of informa-tion may be more or less segregated between caudal and rostral subdivisions, but also, and per-haps most importantly, that the nature of the interaction between the entorhinal cortex and the rest of the hippocampal formation may vary between different subdivisions. We compare our quantitative data in monkeys with previously published stereological data for the rat and human, in order to provide a perspective on the relative development and structural organization of the main subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex in two model organisms widely used to decipher the basic functional principles of the human medial temporal lobe memory system. Altogether, these data provide fundamental information on the number of functional units that comprise the entorhinal-hippocampal circuits and should be considered in order to build realistic models of the medial temporal lobe memory system.

K E Y W O R D S

hippocampal formation, neuron number, neurofilament, RRID:AB_2313581, RRID: AB_2314904

1 | I N T R O D U C T I O N

The entorhinal cortex is a prominent structure of the medial temporal lobe, which plays a pivotal role in the interaction between the neocor-tex and the hippocampal formation in support of declarative and spa-tial memory functions (Amaral, Insausti, & Cowan, 1987; Amaral & Lavenex, 2007; Chareyron, Banta Lavenex, Amaral, & Lavenex, 2017; Lavenex & Amaral, 2000; Witter, Doan, Jacobsen, Nilssen, & Ohara, 2017; Witter & Moser, 2006). The entorhinal cortex is the main entry-way for much of the neocortical information reaching the hippocam-pal formation. It is also the main conduit for information processed by the hippocampus to be sent back to the neocortex. The entorhinal cortex, however, is far more than simply a relay station allowing

information to be transferred between the hippocampus and the rest of the brain. Indeed, an important network of associational connec-tions and intrinsic circuits between neurons located in different layers contribute to information processing carried out by the entorhinal cor-tex (Chrobak & Amaral, 2007; Lavenex & Amaral, 2000; Witter & Moser, 2006). Given its central role in memory function, the entorhi-nal cortex has been the focus of very intense investigation in animal models of human memory processes, in particular in rats and mon-keys. However, although the general functional organization of the entorhinal cortex is conserved across species (Insausti, Herrero, & Witter, 1997; Witter et al., 2017), there are clear differences in the number, the relative development, and the structural characteristics of different subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex between rats,

http://doc.rero.ch

Published in "Journal of Comparative Neurology 526(13): 2115–2132, 2018"

which should be cited to refer to this work.

(2)

monkeys, and humans (Amaral et al., 1987; Amaral & Lavenex, 2007; Insausti et al., 1997; Insausti, Tunon, Sobreviela, Insausti, & Gonzalo, 1995). It is therefore important to obtain reliable estimates of the funda-mental neuroanatomical characteristics of the entorhinal cortex in these different species in order to be able to extrapolate the findings obtained in experimental studies in animals and create realistic models of the basic principles of human memory function (Witter & Moser, 2006).

1.1 | Subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex

Based on the organization of the afferent and efferent connections of the entorhinal cortex in the cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fascicularis), together with the distribution of acetylcholinesterase histochemistry, heavy metal distribution and Golgi-impregnated preparations, Amaral et al. (1987) defined seven subdivisions in the monkey entorhinal cor-tex: Eo, the olfactory field of the entorhinal cortex; Er, the rostral divi-sion of the entorhinal cortex; El, the lateral dividivi-sion of the entorhinal cortex, which comprises the lateral rostral (Elr) and lateral caudal (Elc) subdivisions; Ei, the intermediate division of the entorhinal cortex; Ec, the caudal division of the entorhinal cortex; and Ecl, the caudal limit-ing division of the entorhinal cortex.

Based on the organization described in monkeys, Insausti et al. (1995) defined eight subdivisions in the human entorhinal cortex: Eo, the olfactory field; Er, the rostral field; Elr, the lateral rostral field; Emi, the medial intermediate field; Ei, the intermediate field; Elc, the lateral caudal field; Ec, the caudal field; and Ecl, the caudal limiting field. This parcellation of the human entorhinal cortex is thus largely consistent with the one originally described in monkeys. The different fields of the primate entorhinal cortex may be associated with specific functions, and susceptibility to pathology. Recent functional magnetic resonance imag-ing (fMRI) studies in humans have defined two major functional subre-gions, the anterolateral entorhinal cortex and the posteromedial entorhinal cortex (homologous to the rodent lateral entorhinal cortex [LEC] and medial entorhinal cortex [MEC], respectively; see below), based on their preferential connectivity with the perirhinal (PRC) and parahippocampal (PRH) cortices (Maass, Berron, Libby, Ranganath, & Duzel, 2015), or their global connectivity patterns (Schroder, Haak, Jime-nez, Beckmann, & Doeller, 2015), which had been previously defined in rats and monkeys (Amaral & Lavenex, 2007; van Strien, Cappaert, & Wit-ter, 2009). Based on the connectivity patterns established in monkeys (Insausti, Amaral, & Cowan, 1987; Suzuki & Amaral, 1994b) and the topo-logical organization described in monkeys and humans (Amaral et al., 1987; Insausti et al., 1995), one may surmise that in humans LEC prises the fields Eo, Er, Elr, Elc, and a portion of Ei, whereas MEC com-prises a portion of Ei and the fields Emi, Ec, and Ecl.

Similarly, following the scheme developed in primates and the detailed analysis of the connectivity of this region, Insausti et al. (1997) described six subdivisions in the rat entorhinal cortex: the dorsal lateral entorhinal field (DLE), the dorsal intermediate field (DIE), the amygdalo-entorhinal transitional field (AE), the ventral intermedi-ate entorhinal field (VIE), the medial entorhinal field (ME), and the cau-dal entorhinal field (CE). Despite the similarities in the overall organization of the hippocampal-cortical connectivity in rats and mon-keys, this nomenclature is rarely used (Witter et al., 2017). Instead, most neuroanatomical and functional studies consider a simpler

parcellation of the rat entorhinal cortex that includes the lateral ento-rhinal cortex (LEC), which comprises the fields DLE, DIE, AE, and VIE, and the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC), which comprises the fields ME and CE. Note that this simplified parcellation of the entorhinal cortex in rodent functional studies (Witter & Moser, 2006) contrib-uted to the use of this simplified parcellation of the entorhinal cortex in human functional studies (Reagh et al., 2018) and some compara-tive neuroanatomical studies (Ding et al., 2017; Naumann et al., 2016).

1.2 | Different functional circuits

A simplified description of the connectivity of the entorhinal cortex, which is consistent across species, indicates that its superficial layers (II and III) represent the main entryways for much of the sensory infor-mation processed by the hippocampal forinfor-mation, whereas its deep layers (V and VI) provide the main conduit through which processed information is sent back to the neocortex (Amaral & Lavenex, 2007; Witter et al., 2017).

In monkeys, the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices provide about two-thirds of the cortical projections reaching the entorhinal cortex, but the projections from these two cortices are directed pref-erentially toward different subdivisions (Suzuki & Amaral, 1994a). The projections from the perirhinal cortex terminate predominantly in the rostral two-thirds of the entorhinal cortex, in particular areas Eo, Er, Elr, Elc, and Ei. The projections from the parahippocampal cortex, in contrast, terminate predominantly in the caudal two-thirds of the entorhinal cortex, particularly in areas Ei, Ec, and Ecl. Other cortical projections originate in the temporal lobes, in the frontal cortex, the insula, the cingulate, and retrosplenial cortices (Insausti et al., 1987). Consistent with the fact that the entorhinal cortex is not a homoge-neous structure, the projections originating from these cortical regions each preferentially terminate in different subdivisions of the entorhi-nal cortex. Direct projections from the insula, the orbitofrontal cortex,and the anterior cingulate cortex are directed predominantly toward rostral areas Eo, Er, Elr, and Ei, whereas the projections from the retrosplenial cortex and the superior temporal gyrus are directed predominantly toward caudal areas Ei, Ec, and Ecl. Similarly, the pro-jections originating from the amygdala, which are thought to contrib-ute to the emotional regulation of memory, are directed toward the rostral subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex, including areas Eo, Er, Elr, and the rostral portions of areas Ei and Elc, with essentially no amyg-dala projections to the caudal areas, Ec and Ecl (Pitkänen, Kelly, & Amaral, 2002).

The entorhinal cortex projections to the dentate gyrus and the hippocampus also exhibit clear patterns of laminar and topographical organization, which suggests distinct functional circuits (Amaral, Kondo, & Lavenex, 2014; Witter & Amaral, 1991; Witter, Van Hoe-sen, & Amaral, 1989). Entorhinal cortex projections to the dentate gyrus, and the CA3 and CA2 fields of the hippocampus originate mainly from cells in layer II, whereas projections to CA1 and the subi-culum originate mainly from cells in layer III. In monkeys, lateral por-tions of the entorhinal cortex project to caudal levels of the dentate gyrus and hippocampus, whereas medial portions of the entorhinal cortex project to rostral levels. In addition, the projections from the

(3)

entorhinal cortex to the dentate gyrus exhibit a different laminar dis-tribution depending on the rostrocaudal location of the cells of origin. The rostral entorhinal cortex projects more heavily to the outer third of the molecular layer, whereas the caudal entorhinal cortex projects more heavily to the middle third of the molecular layer. Similarly, the projections from the entorhinal cortex to the hippocampus exhibit a different topographical distribution depending on the rostrocaudal location of the cells of origin. Projections from the rostral part of the entorhinal cortex terminate at the border of CA1 and the subiculum, whereas projections from the caudal part of the entorhinal cortex ter-minate in the portion of CA1 closer to CA2 and in the portion of the subiculum closer to the presubiculum.

The dentate gyrus and CA3 do not project back to the entorhinal cortex (Amaral & Lavenex, 2007). In contrast, CA1 and the subiculum project to the deep layers of the entorhinal cortex, following a topo-graphical organization that largely reciprocates the entorhinal cortex projections to these regions (Amaral & Lavenex, 2007). In monkeys, the rostral entorhinal cortex receives projections originating from pyramidal cells located at the border of CA1 and the subiculum, whereas the caudal entorhinal cortex receives projections originating in the portion of CA1 closer to CA2 and the portion of the subiculum closer to the presubiculum. One final but important characteristic of the connectivity of the entorhinal cortex is the direct projections from the presubiculum to layer III of the caudal subdivisions of the entorhi-nal cortex, areas Ec and Ecl. Interestingly, this connection is also a defining feature of the rat MEC, which is particularly involved in spa-tial information processing (Knierim, Neunuebel, & Deshmukh, 2014; Witter & Moser, 2006).

1.3 | Aim of the current study

Despite all that is already known regarding the structural organization of the monkey entorhinal cortex, there is little quantitative informa-tion about its structural characteristics, including reliable estimates of the number of neurons and their features in the different layers of its different subdivisions. The aim of the current study was to provide these normative data for the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) entorhi-nal cortex. We implemented modern, design-based stereological tech-niques to provide estimates of neuron numbers, neuronal soma size, and volume of different layers and subdivisions of adult macaque monkeys (5–9 years of age). We further compared our quantitative data with previously published stereological data for the rat and human entorhinal cortex, in order to provide a perspective on the rela-tive development and structural organization of the main subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex in two model organisms widely used to deci-pher the basic functional principles of the medial temporal lobe mem-ory system in humans.

2 | M A T E R I A L S A N D M E T H O D S

2.1 | Experimental animals

Four rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta (two males: 5.3 and 9.4 years of age; two females: 7.7 and 9.3 years of age), were used for this

study. Monkeys were born from multiparous mothers and raised at the California National Primate Research Center. They were mater-nally reared in 2,000 m2outdoor enclosures and lived in large social

groups until they were killed. These monkeys were the same animals used in quantitative studies of the monkey hippocampal formation (Jabes, Banta Lavenex, Amaral, & Lavenex, 2010, 2011) and amygdala (Chareyron, Banta Lavenex, Amaral, & Lavenex, 2011; Chareyron, Banta Lavenex, Amaral, & Lavenex, 2012). All experimental proce-dures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Com-mittee of the University of California, Davis.

2.2 | Histological processing

2.2.1 | Brain acquisition

Monkeys were deeply anesthetized with an intravenous injection of sodium pentobarbital (50 mg/kg i.v.; Fatal-Plus; Vortech Pharmaceuti-cals, Dearborn, MI) and perfused transcardially with 1% and then 4% paraformaldehyde in 0.1 M phosphate buffer (PB; pH 7.4) following protocols previously described (Lavenex, Banta Lavenex, Bennett, & Amaral, 2009). Serial coronal sections were cut with a freezing micro-tome in sets of seven sections, where the first six sections were 30-μm thick, and the seventh section was 60-μm thick (Microm HM 450, Microm International GmbH, Walldorf, Baden-Württemberg, Germany). The 60-μm sections were collected in 10% formaldehyde solution in 0.1 M PB (pH 7.4) and postfixed at 4C for 4 weeks prior to Nissl staining with thionin. All other series were collected in tissue collection solution (TCS) and kept at−70C until further processing.

2.2.2 | Nissl staining with thionin

The procedure for Nissl-stained sections followed our standard labo-ratory protocol described previously (Lavenex et al., 2009). Sections were taken out of the 10% formaldehyde solution, thoroughly washed 2× 2 hr in 0.1 M PB, mounted on gelatin-coated slides from filtered 0.05 M PB (pH 7.4), and air-dried overnight at 37C. Sections were then defatted 2× 2 hr in a mixture of chloroform/ethanol (1:1, vol.), and rinsed 2× 2 min in 100% ethanol, 2 min in 95% ethanol and air-dried overnight at 37 C. Sections were then rehydrated through a graded series of ethanol, 2 min in 95% ethanol, 2 min in 70% ethanol, 2 min in 50% ethanol, dipped in two separate baths of dH2O, and

stained 20 s in a 0.25% thionin (Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, cat# T-409) solution, dipped in two separate baths of dH2O, 4 min in 50%

ethanol, 4 min in 70% ethanol, 4 min in 95% ethanol + glacial acetic acid (1 drop per 100 mL of ethanol), 4 min in 95% ethanol, 2× 4 min in 100% ethanol, 3× 4 min in xylene, and coverslipped with the mounting medium DPX (BDH Laboratories, Poole, UK).

2.2.3 | SMI-32 immunohistochemistry

The immunohistochemical procedure for visualizing nonphosphory-lated high-molecular-weight neurofilaments was carried out on free-floating sections using the monoclonal antibody SMI-32 (Sternberger Monoclonals, Lutherville, MD, cat# SMI-32, lot 16; RRID: AB_2314904), as previously described (Lavenex et al., 2009; Lavenex, Banta Lavenex, & Amaral, 2004). This antibody was raised in mouse against the nonphosphorylated 200 kDa heavy neurofilament. On conventional immunoblots, SMI-32 visualizes two bands (200 and

(4)

180 kDa) which merge into a single line on two-dimensional blots (Goldstein, Sternberger, & Sternberger, 1987; Sternberger & Sternber-ger, 1983). This antibody has been shown to react with nonpho-sphorylated high-molecular-weight neurofilaments of most mammalian species, including rats, cats, dogs, monkeys, and humans (de Haas Ratzliff & Soltesz, 2000; Hof & Morrison, 1995; Hornung & Riederer, 1999; Lavenex et al., 2004; Siegel et al., 1993), and may also show some limited cross-reactivity with nonphosphorylated medium-molecular-weight neurofilaments (Hornung & Riederer, 1999).

Sections that had been maintained in TCS at−70C were rinsed 3× 10 min in 0.05 M Tris buffer (pH 7.4) with 1.5% NaCl, treated against endogenous peroxidase by immersion in 0.5% hydrogen perox-ide solution in 0.05 M Tris/NaCl for 15 min and rinsed 6× 5 min in Tris/NaCl buffer. Sections were then incubated for 4 hr in a blocking solution made up of 0.5% Triton X-100 (TX-100; Fisher Scientific, Wal-tham, MA; cat# BP151-500), and 10% normal horse serum (NHS; Bio-genesis, Poole, UK; cat# 8270-1004) in 0.05 M Tris/NaCl buffer at room temperature. Sections were then incubated overnight with the primary antibody SMI-32 (1:2,000) in 0.3% TX-100 and 1% NHS in 0.05 M Tris/NaCl at 4C. Sections were then washed 3× 10 min in 0.05 M Tris/NaCl buffer with 1% NHS, incubated with a secondary antibody, biotinylated horse antimouse IgG (1:227; Vector Laborato-ries, Burlingame, CA; cat# BA-2000; RRID:AB_2313581) in 0.3% TX-100 and 1% NHS in 0.05 M Tris/NaCl buffer, rinsed 3× 10 min in 0.05 M Tris/NaCl buffer containing 1% NHS, incubated for 45 min in an avidin-biotin complex solution (Biostain ABC kit, Biomeda, Foster City, CA; cat# 11-001), washed 3× 10 min in Tris/NaCl, incubated in secondary antibody solution for another 45 min, washed 3× 10 min, incubated in avidin-biotin complex solution for 30 min, washed 3× 10 min in Tris/NaCl, incubated for 30 min in a solution containing 0.05% diaminobenzidine (Sigma-Aldrich Chemicals, St. Louis, MO; cat# D9015-100MG), 0.04% H2O2 in 0.05 M Tris buffer, and washed

3× 10 min. Sections were then mounted on gelatin-coated slides from filtered 0.05 M PB (pH 7.4) and air-dried overnight at 37C. Reaction product was then intensified with a silver nitrate–gold chloride method. Sections were defatted 2× 2 hr in a chloroform/ethanol (1:1, vol.) solution, rehydrated through a graded series of ethanol, and air-dried overnight at 37C. Sections were then rinsed 10 min in running dH2O, incubated for 40 min in a 1% silver nitrate (AgNO3) solution at

56C, rinsed 10 min in dH2O, incubated for 10 min in 0.2% gold

chlo-ride (HAuCl43H2O) at room temperature with agitation, rinsed 10 min

in dH2O, stabilized in 5% sodium thiosulfate (Na2S2O3) for 15 min with

agitation, rinsed in running dH2O for 10 min, dehydrated through a

graded series of ethanol and xylene, and coverslipped with the mount-ing medium DPX.

2.3 | Stereological analyses

2.3.1 | Neuron number

The total number of neurons in the different layers (II, III, V, VI) of the seven subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex (Eo, Er, Ei, Elr, Elc, Ec, Ecl) was determined using the optical fractionator method on the Nissl-stained sections cut at 60μm (West, Slomianka, & Gundersen, 1991). Neurons were counted when their nucleus came into focus within the counting frame, as it was moved through a known distance of the

section thickness. We estimated neuron numbers using the following formula: N =PQ× 1/ssf × 1/asf × 1/tsf (ssf: section sampling fraction; asf: area sampling fraction; tsf: thickness sampling fraction). This design-based method allows an estimation of the number of neurons that is independent of volume estimates. About 43 sections per animal (240μm apart) were used for the estimation of the total number of principal neu-rons in the different layers/subdivisions. We estimated neuron numbers in the left hemisphere for half of the animals and in the right hemisphere for the other half. No difference was observed between left and right hemisphere; reported estimates are unilateral values. We used a 100× Plan Fluor oil objective (N.A. 1.30) on a Nikon Eclipse 80i microscope (Nikon Instruments Inc., Melville, NY) linked to PC-based StereoInvesti-gator 9.0 (MBF Bioscience, Williston, VT). The sampling scheme was established to obtain individual estimates of neuron number with esti-mated coefficients of error around 0.11 (Table 1; CE = sqrt(CE2(PQ) +

CE2(t)); CE(PQ) = sum (Q

i) + ((3× (sum (Qi× Qi) - sum (Qi))-(4× sum

(Qi× Qi + 1) + (Qi× Qi + 2))/12; CE(t) = standard deviation

(section thickness)/average (section thickness)). We identified neurons based on morphological criteria identifiable in Nissl preparations, as described in more details in previous publications (Chareyron et al., 2011; Fitting, Booze, Hasselrot, & Mactutus, 2008; Grady, Charleston, Maris, Witgen, & Lifshitz, 2003; Hamidi, Drevets, & Price, 2004; Morris, Jordan, & Breedlove, 2008; Palackal, Neuringer, & Sturman, 1993). Briefly, neurons are darkly stained and comprise a single large nucleolus. Astrocytes are relatively smaller in size and exhibit pale staining of the nucleus. Oligodendrocytes are smaller than astrocytes and contain round, darkly staining nuclei that are densely packed with chromatin. Microglia have the smallest nucleus, dark staining, and an irregular shape that is often rod-like, oval or bent.

2.3.2 | Volume estimates

We estimated the volume of the individual layers of the seven subdivi-sions of the monkey entorhinal cortex based on the outline tracings per-formed with Stereoinvestigator 9.0 for the estimation of neuron numbers. We used the section cutting thickness (60μm) to determine the distance between sampled sections, which was then multiplied by the total surface area delineated for neuron counts to calculate the volume.

2.3.3 | Neuronal soma size

The volume of neuronal somas was measured on Nissl-stained prepa-rations, using the nucleator probe of StereoInvestigator 9.0 (MBF Bio-science, Williston, VT). We measured an average of 291 neurons per layer per subdivision, sampled at every counting site during the optical fractionator analysis (Table 1). Briefly, the nucleator can be used to estimate the mean cross-sectional area and volume of cells. A point within the nucleus was selected randomly, and three rays at 120 angles were drawn in a random orientation to intersect the cell boundary. When the rays extended into proximal cell processes, the cell boundary was defined as the continuation of the adjacent cell boundary at the base of the process. The length of the intercept from the point to the cell boundary (l) is measured and the cell volume is obtained by V = (4/3× 3.1416) × l3. Essentially, this is the formula

used to determine the volume of a sphere with a known radius. Note that the nucleator method provides accurate estimates of neuron size

(5)

TABLE 1 Parameters us ed for the stereolo gica l analysis of the mo nkey entorh inal cor tex Entorhinal area Number of sections (range)

Distance between sections

(μ m) Scan grid (μ m) a Counting frame (μ m) Disector height (μm) Guard zones (μm) Average section thickness (μ m) b Number of neurons counted (range) Coefficients of error (CE) Eo II 18.25 (17 – 21) 240 130 × 130 40 × 40 5 2 12.07 (8.9 – 24.2) 393 (320 – 442) 0.13 (0.10 – 0.15) III 18.25 (17 – 21) 240 350 × 350 40 × 40 5 2 12.43 (9.9 – 17.0) 390 (320 – 446) 0.10 (0.09 – 0.12) V – VI 18.25 (17 – 21) 240 170 × 170 40 × 40 5 2 13.26 (10.4 – 17.1) 345 (286 – 409) 0.10 (0.09 – 0.11) Er II 15.00 (13 – 16) 240 150 × 150 40 × 40 5 2 12.49 (9.3 – 16.7) 357 (252 – 429) 0.11 (0.10 – 0.11) III 15.00 (13 – 16) 240 350 × 350 40 × 40 5 2 13.48 (10.0 – 17.3) 341 (274 – 401) 0.10 (0.09 – 0.11) V 15.00 (13 – 16) 240 150 × 150 40 × 40 5 2 13.75 (10.9 – 18.6) 287 (219 – 338) 0.10 (0.09 – 0.12) VI 15.00 (13 – 16) 240 250 × 250 40 × 40 5 2 13.92 (11 – 1-18.1) 298 (231 – 325) 0.10 (0.09 – 0.11) Elr II 15.25 (14 – 16) 240 140 × 140 40 × 40 5 2 13.75 (10.4 – 22.4) 276 (226 – 347) 0.12 (0.09 – 0.16) III 15.25 (14 – 16) 240 220 × 220 40 × 40 5 2 14.38 (11.4 – 22.1) 234 (210 – 254) 0.12 (0.10 – 0.14) V 15.25 (14 – 16) 240 120 × 120 40 × 40 5 2 14.76 (11.3 – 22.1) 274 (224 – 323) 0.11 (0.09 – 0.15) VI 15.25 (14 – 16) 240 170 × 170 40 × 40 5 2 14.51 (11.0 – 19.0) 314 (296 – 335) 0.10 (0.08 – 0.12) Ei II 13.25 (12 – 14) 240 300 × 300 40 × 40 5 2 13.32 (10.4 – 16.8) 231 (202 – 268) 0.10 (0.10 – 0.10) III 13.25 (12 – 14) 240 500 × 500 40 × 40 5 2 13.82 (10.4 – 17.3) 258 (199 – 292) 0.10 (0.08 – 0.11) V 13.25 (12 – 14) 240 300 × 300 40 × 40 5 2 14.09 (11.0 – 18.0) 251 (217 – 292) 0.09 (0.09 – 0.10) VI 13.25 (12 – 14) 240 450 × 450 40 × 40 5 2 14.32 (11.6 – 18.6) 226 (186 – 253) 0.09 (0.09 – 0.10) Elc II 11.50 (10 – 12) 240 130 × 130 40 × 40 5 2 14.43 (11.0 – 18.4) 313 (306 – 329) 0.11 (0.10 – 0.13) III 11.50 (10 – 12) 240 210 × 210 40 × 40 5 2 15.19 (11.9 – 18.1) 250 (200 – 307) 0.10 (0.09 – 0.10) V 11.50 (10 – 12) 240 140 × 140 40 × 40 5 2 15.12 (12.3 – 23.0) 235 (200 – 282) 0.10 (0.09 – 0.12) VI 11.50 (10 – 12) 240 170 × 170 40 × 40 5 2 14.98 (12.2 – 23.3) 292 (241 – 353) 0.11 (0.09 – 0.13) Ec II 11.75 (9 – 14) 240 200 × 200 40 × 40 5 2 14.30 (10.9 – 22.4) 296 (265 – 344) 0.12 (0.10 – 0.16) III 11.75 (9 – 14) 240 300 × 300 40 × 40 5 2 15.10 (11.4 – 27.4) 324 (272 – 356) 0.13 (0.10 – 0.18) V 11.75 (9 – 14) 240 200 × 200 40 × 40 5 2 15.14 (11.9 – 28.4) 291 (247 – 333) 0.12 (0.10 – 0.18) VI 11.75 (9 – 14) 240 300 × 300 40 × 40 5 2 15.19 (11.8 – 29.1) 271 (222 – 307) 0.13 (0.10 – 0.20) Ecl II 16.25 (13 – 19) 240 250 × 250 40 × 40 5 2 14.61 (10.4 – 26.2) 242 (220 – 266) 0.13 (0.10 – 0.17) III 16.25 (13 – 19) 240 320 × 320 40 × 40 5 2 15.59 (11.1 – 24.9) 264 (247 – 277) 0.12 (0.09 – 0.13) V 16.25 (13 – 19) 240 200 × 200 40 × 40 5 2 15.66 (11.4 – 23.5) 225 (206 – 242) 0.13 (0.12 – 0.15) VI 16.25 (13 – 19) 240 220 × 220 40 × 40 5 2 15.92 (10.9 – 22.7) 385 (330 – 433) 0.11 (0.08 – 0.13) aScan grid was placed in a random orientation. b Section thickness was measured at every other counting site.

http://doc.rero.ch

(6)

when isotropic-uniform-random sectioning of brain structures is employed (Gundersen, 1988). In our study all brains were cut in the coronal plane. Estimates of cell size might therefore be impacted by the nonrandom orientation of neurons in the different layers and sub-divisions of the entorhinal cortex, which could lead to an over-estimation or under-over-estimation of cell size in any given structure.

2.4 | Photomicrographic production

Low-magnification photomicrographs were taken with a Leica DFC420 digital camera on a Leica MZ9.5 stereomicroscope (Leica Microsystems GmbH, 35578 Wetzlar, Germany). High-magnification photomicrographs were taken with a Leica DFC490 digital camera Leica Microsystems GmbH, 35578 Wetzlar, Germany on a Nikon Eclipse 80i microscope (Nikon Instruments, Tokyo 108-6290, Japan). Artifacts located outside the sections were removed, and levels were adjusted in Adobe Photoshop CS4 V11.0.2 (Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA) to improve contrast and clarity.

3 | R E S U L T S

3.1 | Structural organization of the monkey

entorhinal cortex

The nomenclature, topographical and cytoarchitectonic organization of the entorhinal cortex have been described previously for the cyno-molgus monkey (Macaca fascicularis) (Amaral et al., 1987). The monkey entorhinal cortex comprises seven subdivisions: Eo, the olfactory field of the entorhinal cortex; Er, the rostral entorhinal cortex; El, the lateral entorhinal cortex, which comprises the lateral rostral (Elr) and lateral caudal (Elc) subdivisions; Ei, the intermediate division of the entorhinal cortex; Ec, the caudal division of the entorhinal cortex; and Ecl, the caudal limiting division of the entorhinal cortex. The cytoarchitectonic characteristics of the cynomolgus monkey entorhinal cortex are very similar to those that we observed in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) entorhinal cortex. However, subtle differences in the relative development of individual layers in certain subdivisions (see below), as observed in Nissl preparations, as well as the additional information provided by the immunohistochemical analysis of nonphosphorylated high-molecular-weight neurofilament distribution (SMI-32), prompted us to provide a thorough description of the cytoarchitectonic charac-teristics of the different subdivisions of the adult rhesus monkey ento-rhinal cortex (Figures 1 and 2).

3.2 | Overview of the laminar organization

Layer I, the outermost layer, corresponds to the molecular or superfi-cial plexiform layer found in other cortical fields, and is relatively free of neurons. In SMI-32 preparations, the apical dendritic arborization of layer II neurons can be observed in the deeper half of the layer, with only occasional stained fibers visible in the superficial half of layer I. The remainder of layer I is at background level of staining.

Layer II is a narrow, cellular layer that varies considerably in appearance at different rostrocaudal levels. Its major cell type, gener-ally described as “stellate,” is in fact a type of modified pyramidal

neuron. Neuron size varies across subdivisions, ranging from an aver-age volume of about 1,200μm3in Eo to 2,700

μm3in area Ec (see

below for estimates in other subdivisions). In SMI-32 preparations, the cell bodies and the apical dendrites of layer II neurons are heavily stained. There are, however, two superimposed gradients in SMI-32 staining intensity that appear to correlate with gradients in cell size. First, staining intensity increases from rostral to caudal levels. Second, at rostral levels, labeling is higher laterally than medially. There is no obvious mediolateral gradient at caudal levels.

Layer III is the thickest of the entorhinal cell layers (Table 2; Figure 3). At rostral levels it has a patchy appearance, but it becomes increasingly more homogeneous and somewhat“columnar” at caudal levels. Neurons in the superficial portion of layer III are pyramidal neu-rons similar to layer II cells, whereas deeply located neuneu-rons are multi-polar, round or fusiform neurons. In some subdivisions, layer III is quite sharply separated from layer II by a narrow, cell-free zone. In SMI-32 preparations, only a small proportion of layer III cells are labeled. At the most rostral levels, SMI-32-positive neurons are located in the deep portion of layer III, whereas at more caudal levels SMI-32-positive neurons are located in the superficial portion of layer III. In addition, there is a mediolateral gradient at rostral levels; medi-ally SMI-32-positive cells are located deeper in layer III, whereas later-ally SMI-32-positive neurons are located more superficilater-ally in layer III. This mediolateral gradient is not obvious at caudal levels, as all SMI-32-positive cells are located in the superficial portion of layer III.

Layer IV in the entorhinal cortex is usually referred to as the lamina dissecans. This is a cell-sparse zone that is rich in myelinated fibers espe-cially at mid-rostrocaudal levels. In the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta), layer IV is generally more visible throughout most of the entorhinal cor-tex, as compared to the cynomolgus monkey (Macaca fascicularis). In SMI-32 preparations, layer IV appears largely unstained, except for the apical dendrites of neurons originating in layers V and VI.

Layer V has a stratified appearance over much of its extent in the cynomolgus monkey and can be divided in three laminae, Va, Vb, and Vc. This lamination is not as obvious in rhesus monkeys and it varies significantly at different rostrocaudal levels. Layers Va and Vb are often difficult to distinguish in Nissl preparations, and layer Vc, a cell sparse zone separating layer V from layer VI, appears more clearly only at mid rostrocaudal levels. In SMI-32 preparations, layer V also appears largely homogenous. The cell bodies of layer V neurons are only lightly stained, while their dendrites are moderately stained; the neuropil of layer V is moderately stained.

Layer VI is a striking, multi-laminated cellular layer. At some levels, as many as four distinct bands of cells can be distinguished in layer VI, and these bands often have a characteristically“coiled” appearance when seen in coronal Nissl-stained sections. In SMI-32 preparations, cell bodies of layer VI neurons are moderately stained, while their api-cal dendrites are darkly stained. The sublaminae of layer VI are there-fore also clearly visible in SMI-32-preparations.

Deep to layer VI are scattered neurons in the subcortical white matter adjacent to the angular bundle. Their number and distribution do not appear sufficient to justify identifying them as a separate, sev-enth layer. These neurons have various morphologies in Nissl-stained preparations; some of these neurons as well as a number of individual fibers are SMI-32-positive.

(7)

3.3 | Detailed characteristics of individual

subdivisions

Eo: Layer I is a rather thin layer in Eo. Layer II is either absent or very thin; this serves to distinguish the area and stands in marked contrast to the prominent cell islands typical of area Er. Where layer II is recog-nizable in Nissl preparations, its cells are distinctly smaller than those

in Er (Table 2) and there is generally a cell-free zone beneath them that demarcates layer II from layer III. Layer III is formed by clusters of neurons that are characteristic of the rostral entorhinal cortex. How-ever, the clusters are more densely aggregated than in Er so that, at low magnification, layer III has a more uniform appearance in Eo. The superficially located cells in layer III are more densely packed and thus appear more darkly stained than the deeper cells. There is no evident

FIGURE 1 Low magnification photomicrographs of coronal sections through the adult rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex. (a), (c), (e), (g) Nissl-stained preparations, arranged from rostral (a) to caudal (g). (b), (d), (f ), (h) SMI-32 immunohistochemistry preparations, arranged from rostral (b) to caudal (h). Eo = olfactory field; Er = rostral field; Elr = lateral rostral field; Elc = lateral caudal field; Ei = intermediate field; Ec = caudal field; and Ecl = caudal limiting field. Scale bar = 1 mm

(8)

layer IV in Eo. The deeper layers V and VI are poorly developed; at caudal levels and at the transition between Eo and Er, layer V appears like a cell-sparse layer, whereas layer VI contains densely packed, darkly stained neurons in Nissl preparations.

In SMI-32 preparations, Eo layer II cells are largely unstained. In layer III, a small number of neurons located in the deeper portion are strongly SMI-32-positive, while individual neurons located superfi-cially are lightly labeled. Layer VI shows a large number of SMI-32-positive neurons; their cell bodies are moderately stained, while a relatively short portion of their dendrites appears heavily stained in coronal sections. A number of SMI-32-positive fibers, probably the dendrites of layer VI neurons, are also visible throughout layer III.

Er: Layer I is thicker in Er than in Eo. Layer II is made up of islands of multipolar cells that are separated by wide, relatively cell-sparse zones. These cell-sparse zones are continuous with similar zones in layer III, as are the cell islands. Layer III is composed of large, irregular patches of neurons that are separated by cell-sparse regions. The more superficial cells in the layer tend to be larger than those at dee-per levels. No distinct layer IV is visible throughout most of this field.

However, near its caudal boundary there are occasional cell-free patches between layers III and V that mark the beginning of the lam-ina dissecans. Layer V is well developed but its sublamlam-inae are not easily distinguished. Layer V neurons are relatively large multipolar neurons, which contrast with the smaller, radially oriented layer VI neurons. Layer VI is as thick as at any caudal level of the entorhinal cortex, but it does not have the laminated appearance that is so char-acteristic of fields Ei and Ec.

In SMI-32 preparations, the cell bodies of layer II neurons are moderately to heavily stained. Their dendrites are strongly labeled but do not extend very far into layer I. SMI-32-positive neurons are found both in the superficial and deep portions of layer III. The cell bodies and dendrites are strongly labeled, whereas the neuropil stains only slightly above background level. SMI-32-positive neurons tend to be located more deeply in layer III at rostral and medial levels of Er, near the border with Eo; whereas they tend to be located more superfi-cially at lateral and caudal levels, near the border with Elr and Ei, respectively. The cell bodies of layers V and VI neurons are moder-ately stained, whereas their dendrites are more darkly stained,

FIGURE 2 Continued on next page

(9)

especially those of layer VI neurons. The neuropil of layers V and VI is moderately stained and stands in contrast with that of the very lightly stained layer III.

El (Elr and Elc): Layer I is generally thicker in Elr and Elc than in the more medial fields. This is likely due to the fact that El is located within the rhinal sulcus. Layer II is formed by large and relatively wide cell islands, so wide that they give the layer an almost continu-ous appearance, especially at caudal levels. In Elr, the cells of layer II merge with those of layer III, but in Elc, there is a narrow cell-free zone separating the two layers. Although the cells of layer III are still clustered, the layer as a whole has a more homogeneous appearance in Elr as compared to Er. The lamina dissecans is not evident in either Elr or Elc; in Elc the corresponding zone appears to be filled with a heterogeneous population of small cells. Layer V is rather narrow; there is a prominent band of darkly stained cells in layer V that appears continuous with layer V of area 35 in Nissl prepara-tions. Layer VI is continuous with the same layer in Er or Ei, but tends to have a less laminated appearance in Elr and Elc, and thus resembles layer VI of area 35.

In SMI-32 preparations, the somas of layer II cells are strongly labeled, and their apical dendrites extend profusely in the deeper half of layer I. In contrast, layer III is largely unstained (except for a small population of superficially located neurons) and appears to extend into area 35. As in medially located areas Er and Ei, layer V neurons are moderately SMI-32-positive, whereas layer VI neurons are more darkly stained. SMI-32-positive dendrites of deep layer neurons are clearly visible and radially oriented in layer III. In contrast, there is no clear organization or orientation of SMI-32-positive processes within layers V and VI.

Ei: There is nothing distinctive about layer I. Layer II is formed by islands of multipolar cells; the mediolateral extent of the islands is highly variable, and some are quite wide. At rostral levels, the cells of layer II tend to merge with those of layer III, whereas at more caudal levels, a thin acellular band (typical of Ec) tends to separate layer II and layer III cells. Layer III is clearly bilaminate; the outer half of layer III has patches of multipolar and pyramidal cells, whereas the deep half of the layer has a more columnar appearance with multipolar and radi-ally oriented fusiform neurons. Layer IV is present and clearly visible

FIGURE 2 High magnification photomicrographs of coronal sections through the adult rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex. (a–c) and (g–j)

Nissl-stained preparations. (d–f) and (k–n) SMI-32 immunohistochemistry preparations. (a) and (d) Eo = olfactory field; (b) and (e) Er = rostral field; (c) and (f) Elr = lateral rostral field. (g) and (k) Ei = intermediate field; (h) and (l) Elc = lateral caudal field; (i) and (m) Ec = caudal field; (j) and (n) Ecl = caudal limiting field. Scale bars in (a) and (g) = 250μm

(10)

TABLE 2 Volume, neuron number, and neuron soma size in the different layers of the seven subdivisions of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex

Volume (mm3) Neuron number Neuron soma size (μm3)

Mean SD % Mean SD % Mean SD

Eo I 0.86 0.25 7.5 – – – – II 0.98 0.17 8.4 40,137 7,108 10.0 1,216 66 III 7.78 1.31 67.1 296,929 41,347 73.7 1,795 95 V-VI 1.97 0.28 17.0 65,969 8,391 16.4 1,696 171 Total 11.59 1.66 100.0 403,035 53,910 100.0 – – Er I 1.50 0.39 9.2 II 1.47 0.28 9.0 50,417 12,078 10.0 1,629 158 III 8.24 1.26 50.9 281,868 48,238 55.7 1,850 99 V 1.83 0.40 11.2 44,363 8,241 8.8 2,370 147 VI 3.20 0.55 19.7 129,493 19,533 25.6 1,783 152 Total 16.24 2.54 100.0 506,140 67,352 100.0 – – Ei I 4.37 0.56 14.5 II 3.56 0.25 11.8 138,243 16,063 12.9 2,637 171 III 11.16 0.57 37.2 445,652 68,989 41.6 2,115 279 IV 1.25 0.23 4.2 – – – – – V 3.92 0.50 13.0 159,237 20,188 14.9 2,492 238 VI 5.82 0.99 19.3 328,132 46,213 30.6 1,755 160 Total 30.08 1.99 100.0 1,071,264 40,889 100.0 Elr I 1.08 0.08 15.0 – – – – – II 0.88 0.16 12.1 37,057 6,695 17.4 1.915 254 III 2.42 0.17 33.4 81,430 8,653 38.2 1,906 316 V 1.08 0.09 14.9 28,949 3,068 13.6 2,134 219 VI 1.78 0.09 24.6 65,686 6,530 30.8 1,642 122 Total 7.24 0.53 100.0 213,123 8,081 100.0 Elc I 1.25 0.23 18.0 – – – – – II 0.89 0.07 13.0 38,090 593 17.4 2,280 283 III 2.27 0.26 33.0 83,439 12,414 38.1 1,721 230 V 0.96 0.14 13.9 34,604 3,957 15.8 2,089 181 VI 1.52 0.19 22.0 62,761 7,761 28.7 1,518 87 Total 6.89 0.70 100.0 218,894 22,439 100.0 Ec I 2.75 0.58 15.3 – – – – II 2.18 0.36 12.2 85,037 13,609 14.7 2,710 67 III 6.61 1.44 36.7 220,281 29,678 38.0 1,945 218 V 2.75 0.53 15.3 88,204 12,656 15.2 2,145 293 VI 3.68 0.81 20.4 185,487 27,211 32.0 1,503 138 Total 17.98 3.46 100.0 579,009 79,525 100.0 Ecl I 3.13 0.28 18.6 – – – – II 2.83 0.44 16.7 110,802 17,631 20.5 2,672 206 III 6.19 1.03 36.6 211,414 26,276 39.1 2,150 139 V 2.01 0.34 11.8 70,664 8,806 13.1 2,188 125 VI 2.76 0.59 16.2 148,066 18,292 27.4 1,424 139 Total 16.91 2.58 100.0 540,946 67,937 100.0 All subdivisions I 14.94 1.81 14.0 II 12.79 1.55 12.0 499,784 64,505 14.1 III 44.66 4.17 41.8 1,621,012 102,823 45.9 IV 1.25 0.23 1.2 – – – V 14.51 1.43 13.6 491,990 45,907 13.9 VI 18.76 2.77 17.5 919,625 105,028 26.0 Total 106.92 10.77 100.0 3,532,411 252,997 100.0

http://doc.rero.ch

(11)

throughout the entire subdivision. Layer V is well developed, but its lamination varies rostrocaudally and mediolaterally. At rostromedial levels, layer V cannot easily be distinguished from layer VI due to the lack of a clearly defined layer Vc. The cell-free layer Vc becomes increasingly more prominent at lateral and caudal levels. Layer VI is strongly laminated, especially at mid mediolateral and lateral levels, and the cell strands have a distinct“coiled” appearance. Packing den-sity increases from lateral to medial, especially for neurons of the dee-pest portion of layer VI.

In SMI-32 preparations, cell bodies and apical dendrites of layer II neurons are heavily stained. In layer III, a number of large neurons located in the superficial half of the layer are heavily stained. The cell bodies are moderately to heavily stained and their basal and apical dendrites are heavily stained. There is also a large population of layer III neurons distributed throughout the layer, whose cell bodies and dendrites are only lightly stained. In addition, lightly to moderately labeled dendrites of deep layer neurons cross layer III, so that the neu-ropil of layer III appears moderately stained at low magnification. Layer IV is largely unstained, except for the dendrites from layer V and VI neurons, and rare SMI-32 labeled fibers running parallel to layer IV. The cell bodies and dendrites of layer V neurons are moder-ately stained, so that layer V stands in sharp contrast with the lightly stained neuropil of layers III and IV. The cell bodies of layer VI neurons are only slightly more heavily stained than those of layer V neurons, but the proximal dendrites of layer VI neurons appear clearly much thicker and more darkly stained than those of layer V neurons. This characteristic helps to distinguish layer V from layer VI in SMI-32 preparations. The white matter below layer VI is largely unstained, except for a small number of moderately stained neurons of various shapes. Short, heavily stained processes are also found throughout the white matter below layer VI.

Ec: Layer I is similar in thickness to that in Ei. Layer II is made up of much wider islands of cells and at caudal levels these islands are almost continuous. Beneath layer II, there is a thin acellular band sepa-rating it from layer III. Although still detectable, the lamination of layer

III is not as obvious as in Ei, and decreases from rostral to caudal. There are no clear isolated patches of cells within layer III. Layer IV is no longer detectable except in the medial half of its more rostral por-tion. In contrast to the cynomolgus monkey, layer V does not appear clearly laminated in Ec of the rhesus monkey. Layer VI is well devel-oped in Ec, as is observed in Ei; three to four distinct bands of cells are aligned parallel to one another.

In SMI-32 preparations, the cell bodies of layer II neurons are moderately stained and their dendrites are heavily stained. Cell den-sity appears lower than in more rostral subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex and the dendritic arborization gives layer II a bushy appearance. A small number of darkly stained neurons are distributed in the super-ficial one-third of layer III; moderately stained neurons are found throughout the superficial half of layer III; the rest of the neuropil of layer III is moderately to lightly stained. The layer V neuropil is moder-ately stained, and contains a small number of modermoder-ately stained neu-rons with darkly stained dendrites, as well as the darkly stained dendrites of superficially located layer VI neurons. Few stained neu-rons are located in the deepest laminae of layer VI.

Ecl: There is nothing distinctive about layer I. Layer II is thicker in Ecl than in Ec, especially near its border with the parasubiculum. Layer II is more or less continuous and the acellular zone subjacent to it is not as prominent as in Ec. Layer III cells tend to be rounder and more darkly stained than in Ec. Layer IV is not discernible in Nissl prepara-tions. The sublaminae of layer V are even less distinct than in Ec, and it is particularly difficult to see a clear layer Vc throughout much of the subdivision. Layer VI is less laminar than in Ec, perhaps because coronal sections through caudal portions of Ecl are cut obliquely.

In SMI-32 preparations, cell bodies of layer II neurons are moder-ately to heavily labeled and the highly stained dendrites of layer II neurons give this layer a bushy appearance as is observed in Ec. The darkly stained neurons observed in layer III are distributed throughout the superficial half of the layer. The density of moderately stained neurons with highly stained dendrites increases in layer V of Ecl, as compared to Ec. Layer V seems to be continuous with the darkly

FIGURE 3 (a) Unfolded map of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex illustrating the relative position of its seven subdivisions. Black arrows indicate the approximate rostrocaudal locations of the coronal sections illustrated in Figures 1 and 2. (b) Volumes of the different layers of seven subdvisions of the adult rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex, measured on Nissl-stained sections cut at 60μm on a freezing sliding microtome

(12)

stained, deep layers of the parasubiculum and presubiculum. Layer VI contains a moderate density of moderately stained neurons with darkly stained dendrites.

3.4 | Stereological analyses

3.4.1 | Volumes of different layers and subdivisions

The volumes of the different layers of the seven subdivisions of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex are presented in Figure 3 and Table 2. Area Eo represents 11% of the entire volume of the entorhi-nal cortex, Er 15%, Elr 7%, Ei 28%, Elc 6%, Ec 17%, and Ecl 16%. Alto-gether, areas Eo, Er, and Elr constitute the rostral third of the volume of the entorhinal cortex; areas Ei + Elc constitute the intermediate third; and areas Ec and Ecl constitute the caudal third.

As one can observe on coronal sections, the relative volumetric development of individual layers varies between different subdivi-sions. Layer I of area Eo is very thin and represents only 7.5% of the volume of Eo. Layer I accounts for 9.2% of the volume of Er, but between 14.5% and 18.6% of the volume of all other subdivisions.

Layer III represents the largest layer across the entorhinal cortex, but this is especially true in the most rostral subdivisions, Eo and Er, where the deep layers are significantly less developed. Layer III accounts for 67.1% of the volume of Eo, and 50.9% of the volume of Er, but between 33.0% and 37.2% of the volume of all other subdivisions. The relative volume of the deep layers, V and VI, is relatively stable across all the other subdivisions.

3.4.2 | Neuron numbers in different layers and

subdivisions

The numbers of neurons in the different layers of the seven subdivi-sions of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex are presented in Figure 4 and Table 2. Consistent with the volumetric estimates, about 11% of all entorhinal neurons are located in area Eo, 14% in Er, 6% in Elr, 30% in Ei, 6% in Elc, 16% in Ec, and 15% in Elc. Neurons located in layer III, which contribute the direct entorhinal cortex projections to CA1 and the subiculum, represent almost half (46%) of all principal neurons found in the monkey entorhinal cortex. This percentage is significantly higher in the rostral areas Eo (74%) and Er (56%), but is

FIGURE 4 (a) Numbers of neurons in the different layers of the seven subdivisions of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex. (b) Percentage of neurons located in each layer, per subdivision. See also Table 2

FIGURE 5 (a) Ratio of the number of neurons in the superficial layers (II and III) and the number of neurons in the deep layers (V and VI) in the seven subdivisions of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex. (b) Ratio of the number of neurons contained in layer III (projecting to CA1 and the subiculum) and the number of neurons contained in layer II (projecting to the dentate gyrus and CA3). Ratios in rat LEC and MEC calculated from the averages of the number of neurons reported in (Merrill, Chiba, & Tuszynski, 2001; Mulders, West, & Slomianka, 1997) for young adult rats

(13)

relatively stable and represents between 38% and 42% of the number of neurons in the remaining subdivisions. Neurons located in layer II, which contribute the entorhinal cortex projections to the dentate gyrus and CA3, represent only 10% of neurons in areas Eo and Er. This percentage varies between 13 and 21% in the other subdivisions.

In light of the variations in the number of neurons located in the different layers of the seven subdivisions of the rhesus monkey ento-rhinal cortex, it is interesting to consider the ratio between the num-ber of neurons located in the superficial layers II and III, which originate the main feedforward projections toward the dentate gyrus and the hippocampus, and the number of neurons located in the deep layers V and VI, which represent the main recipient of the feedback projections originating in the hippocampus and the subiculum (Figure 5a). This ratio was greater in the rostral subdivisions (Eo and Er) than in all the other subdivisions (F(6,18)= 301.761, p < .001,

η2

p= .990). There were no differences between Ei, Elr, and Elc.

Inter-estingly, the ratio in Ecl was lower than in Eo and Er, but overall higher than in the other subdivisions, in particular area Ec with which it is often associated.

Consistent with the poor development of the deep layers of area Eo, there are about five times more neurons in the superficial layers than in the deep layers in this subdivision. In area Er, there are about two times more neurons in the superficial layers than in the deep layers. This suggests that these rostral areas play a much larger role in relaying information to the dentate gyrus and the hippocampus than they do in receiving information that has been processed by the hip-pocampus. Despite some subtle variations between subdivisions, there are on average only about 25% more neurons in the superficial layers than in the deep layers in the other subdivisions of the entorhi-nal cortex. Interestingly, the ratios between the number of neurons in the superficial versus deep layers of the monkey entorhinal cortex are significantly higher than in the two main subdivisions of the rat ento-rhinal cortex (LEC-homologous to the primate rostral entoento-rhinal cor-tex: 1.14; MEC-homologous to the primate caudal entorhinal corcor-tex: 1.10; Table 3). These findings suggest differences in the degree of

reciprocity of the connections between different subdivisions of the monkey entorhinal cortex and the rest of the hippocampal formation.

It is also interesting to consider the ratio between the number of neurons located in layer III, which originate the projections to CA1 and the subiculum, and the number of neurons located in layer II, which originate the projections to the dentate gyrus and CA3, in order to assess the relative importance of the direct and indirect projections to the hippocampus originating from different subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex (Figure 5b). This ratio was greater in the rostral sub-divisions (Eo and Er) than in all the other subsub-divisions (F(6,18)= 29.042,

p < .001,η2

p= .906, Fisher Least Significant Difference, all p < .05).

Note, moreover, that the difference between Er and Ei just failed to reach significance, whereas this ratio was higher in Ec than Ecl. In area Eo, there are about 7.5 times more neurons in layer III than in layer II. In area Er, there are about six times more neurons in layer III than in layer II. In area Ei, there are about three times more neurons in layer III than in layer II. Finally, there are between 2 and 2.5 times more neurons in layer III than in layer II in the remaining subdivisions of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex. These estimates reveal a clear ros-trocaudal gradient, with a relatively greater development of layer III, as compared to layer II, in the rostral entorhinal cortex. Although less prominent, there is a similar pattern in the rat entorhinal cortex, with 2.75 times more neurons in layer III than in layer II in LEC, and only 2.23 times more neurons in layer III than in layer II in MEC.

3.4.3 | Neuronal soma size

In addition to the distinct patterns of connectivity described previ-ously, and the differences in the relative number of neurons in the dif-ferent layers of the seven subdivisions of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex shown here, we also found differences in the vol-ume of neuronal somas between subdivisions (Table 2).

Layer II: The average soma volume of layer II neurons differed between subdivisions (F(6,18)= 43.751, p < .001,η2p= .936). Layer II

neurons were smaller in area Eo than in all other subdivisions. Layer II neurons were also smaller in area Er than in more caudal subdivisions, but not area Elr (Eo < all other fields; Er < Ei, Elc, Ec, Ecl: all p < .05).

TABLE 3 Number of neurons in the different layers of the entorhinal cortex in rats and humans

Layer II Layer III Layer V + VI All layers

Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

Rat MEC 62,924 140,541 184,625 388,090 Mulders et al. (1997) 66,400 10,383 128,200 16,423 185,400 24,419 380,000 36,871 Merrill et al. (2001) 59,448 8,575 152,881 15,122 183,849 23,490 396,178 29,223 Rat LEC 40,896 112,335 134,546 287,777 Mulders et al. (1997) 45,600 13,278 119,400 15,502 143,800 29,575 308,800 44,673 Merrill et al. (2001) 36,192 6,133 105,269 18,538 125,291 24,120 266,752 31,034 Rat EC 103,820 252,875 319,170 675,865 Mulders et al. (1997) 112,000 22,327 247,600 22,843 329,200 53,844 688,800 76,594 Merrill et al. (2001) 95,640 10,543 258,150 23,294 309,140 33,668 663,000 46,000 Monkey EC 499,784 64,505 1,621,012 102,823 1,411,615 145,832 3,532,411 252,997 Human EC 652,500 3,590,500 3,247,500 7,490,500 Gomez-Isla et al. (1996) 647,000 143,066 3,525,000 623,810 2,711,000 619,757 6,883,000 1,136,076

West and Slomianka (1998) 658,000 107,098 3,656,000 570,640 3,784,000 288,929 8,098,000 875,825

(14)

Layer III: Although the average soma volume of layer III neurons also differed between subdivisions (F(6,18)= 3.083, p = .030,

η2

p= .507), these differences were not as pronounced as for layer II

neurons. Layer III neurons were smaller in Eo than in Ecl; they were larger in Ei than in Elc.

Layer V: The average soma volume of layer V neurons also dif-fered between subdivisions (F(6,18)= 5.822, p = .002, η2p= .660).

Layer V neurons were smaller in area Eo, as compared to all other areas, except for Ecl. Layer V neurons were larger in area Er than in areas Eo, Elr, Elc. Similarly, layer V neurons were larger in area Ei than in areas Eo and Ec.

Layer VI: The average volume of layer VI neurons also differed between subdivisions (F(6,18)= 4.256, p = .008,η2p= .587). Layer VI

neurons were larger in Ei than in Elc, Ec, and Ecl. They were also larger in area Er than in area Elc. No other differences were statistically significant.

4 | D I S C U S S I O N

4.1 | Comparison with previous studies in monkeys

The current study provides normative data on the volume, neuron number, and neuronal soma size in the different layers of the seven subdivisions of the adult rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex. To our knowledge, there are only two previous studies that provided partial analyses of neuron number and/or neuronal soma size in the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex.

Gazzaley, Thakker, Hof, and Morrison (1997) reported a preserved number of entorhinal cortex layer II neurons in aged (24– 29-year-old) macaque monkeys, as compared to young adult (7.5 12-year-old) and juvenile (1.2–2-year-old) monkeys. Gazzaley et al. subdivided the entorhinal cortex following the description by Van Hoesen and Pandya (1975). Their counts included the entire ros-tral and intermediate subdivisions, as well as the caudal subdivision that contained a visible lamina dissecans. As we have reported in our detailed description of the cytoarchitectonic organization of the rhe-sus monkey entorhinal cortex, the presence of a visible lamina disse-cans by itself does not constitute a reliable criterion by which to delineate its different subdivisions. Moreover, in their study, Gazzaley and colleagues first cut the brains into 5–6-mm-thick blocks, prior to cutting coronal sections at 40μm with a sliding microtome. This pro-cedure necessarily leads to tissue loss with a resulting underestima-tion of total neuron numbers. Finally, the stereological parameters used to calculate neuron numbers, in particular the thickness of the processed sections and the height of the disector were not reported adequately, and detailed records of the study are no longer available (A. H. Gazzaley, J. H. Morrison and P. R. Hof, personal communication, May 31 and June 6, 2018). Given these uncertainties, it is difficult to compare the results in Gazzaley et al. (1997) with the current findings. Merrill, Roberts, and Tuszynski (2000) also reported the conserva-tion of neuron number and neuronal soma size in layers II, III, and V/VI in the intermediate division of the aged rhesus monkey entorhi-nal cortex. Their estimates correspond to about half the values we found for the intermediate division Ei in the current study.

Inter-laboratory differences may be related to the calibration of the computer-aided analysis systems or other methodological differences that are difficult to identify (Altemus, Lavenex, Ishizuka, & Amaral, 2005). In their study, Merrill et al. cut the brains on a freezing micro-tome set at 40μm and reported an average thickness of the Nissl-stained histological sections of about 21μm. In our study, we cut the brains on a freezing microtome at 60μm for the Nissl series, and mea-sured an average thickness of Nissl-stained sections of 13.32μm across all regions/layers. However, since the optical fractionator pro-vides estimates of neuron numbers that are independent of volume measurements, and that the formula used to calculate neuron num-bers includes the thickness sampling fraction, it is unclear how such differences in tissue processing may lead to differences in neuron numbers. We are confident regarding the measurement of the thick-ness of the sections used in the current study, since our computer-aided analysis system is equipped with a Focus Encoder providing 0.1μm resolution measurements of the actual position of the micro-scope stage in the z axis, and does not rely on the predefined settings of the motorized stage. The average section thickness measured in the current study is similar to what we previously found during the completion of stereological studies of the rat and monkey amygdala, which have supported the reliability and generalizability of our norma-tive data (Chareyron et al., 2011) and that were very close to those reported by other laboratories (Berdel, Morys, & Maciejewska, 1997; Carlo, Stefanacci, Semendeferi, & Stevens, 2010; Cooke, Stokas, & Woolley, 2007; Rubinow & Juraska, 2009). In our study, the disector height (5 μm) represented 37.5% of the averaged section thickness, the counting frame was 40× 40 μm and we used different scan grids for individual layers (Table 1). In their study, Merrill and colleagues reported counting neurons in the middle 75% of total tissue thickness for each section, with the optical disector dimensions set at 50μm2

(which may have been 50× 50 μm, M. Tuszynski, personal communi-cation, June 1, 2018). However, there was no information about the scan grid size or the total number of neurons counted, which would enable us to recalculate the estimates of neuron numbers, and detailed records of the study are no longer available (D. A. Merrill and M. H. Tuszynski, personal communication, May 31 and June 1, 2018). Given these uncertainties, it is difficult to compare the results in Mer-rill et al. (2000) with the current findings.

4.2 | Interspecies comparisons

Previous comparisons of the structure of the entorhinal cortex in dif-ferent species have emphasized either the conservation of the general functional organization of the entorhinal cortex across species (Insausti et al., 1997; Naumann et al., 2016; Witter et al., 2017), or the notable differences in the number, relative development and struc-tural characteristics of different subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex between rats, monkeys, and humans (Amaral et al., 1987; Amaral & Lavenex, 2007; Insausti et al., 1995; Insausti et al., 1997). Here, we compare the number of neurons in the different layers of the rat, monkey, and human entorhinal cortex (Tables 3 and 4). As was the case for the comparison of our current findings with those of previous studies carried out in monkeys, it was difficult to find studies in rats and humans that used reliable, design-based stereological techniques

(15)

combined with well-accepted delineations of layers and subdivisions of the entorhinal cortex. We considered two studies in rats, our cur-rent findings in monkeys, and two studies in humans, in order to com-pare the relative development and quantitative structural characteristics of the entorhinal cortex in different species.

4.2.1 | Rats

Mulders et al. (1997) estimated the number of neurons in the different layers of the lateral and medial entorhinal cortex of 30-day-old female Wistar rats. Merrill et al. (2001) estimated the number of neurons of the lateral and medial entorhinal cortex in 2-month-old female Fischer 344 rats (they also reported data on 21-month-old rats, which we did not include in Table 3). The results of these two studies, carried out in two independent laboratories, provide consistent estimates.

4.2.2 | Humans

Gomez-Isla et al. (1996) estimated the number of neurons in the entire entorhinal cortex of nondemented men and women between 60 and 89 years of age. Note that the definitions of the layers reported in their study differed from that used in other studies, so we adapted the presentation of their results to match the definitions used in the other studies. West and Slomianka (1998) estimated the num-ber of neurons in the entire entorhinal cortex of 19–58-year-old men. The results of these two studies, carried out in two independent labo-ratories, provided consistent estimates of the number of neurons in layers II and III. In contrast, there was a relatively larger difference between studies in their estimates of the number of neurons in layers V and VI. Nevertheless, these estimates can be considered sufficiently reliable to perform interspecies comparisons of the relative number of neurons in different layers of the entorhinal cortex.

Since the previous studies in rats or humans did not report the number of neurons in the different subdivisions of the entorhinal cor-tex based on the nomenclature defined by Amaral and colleagues for monkeys (Amaral et al., 1987), Insausti and colleagues for humans (Insausti et al., 1995) and rats (Insausti et al., 1997), we limit our spe-cies comparisons to the number of neurons in the entire entorhinal cortex (Tables 3 and 4). We consider layer II neurons as the origin of the entorhinal cortex projections to the dentate gyrus and CA3; layer III neurons as the origin of the entorhinal cortex projections to CA1 and the subiculum; and layer V and VI neurons as the main layers of the entorhinal cortex receiving the hippocampal output projections originating in CA1 and the subiculum.

As compared to rats, the total number of neurons in the entorhi-nal cortex is about 5 times greater in monkeys, and 11 times greater in humans (and thus about 2 times greater in humans than in mon-keys). In layer II, there are 4.8 times more neurons in monkeys than in rats, 6.3 times more neurons in humans than in rats, and 1.3 times more neurons in humans than in monkeys. In layer III, there are 6.4 times more neurons in monkeys than in rats, 14.2 times more neurons in humans than in rats, and 2.2 times more neurons in humans than in monkeys. In layer V and VI, there are 4.4 times more neurons in mon-keys than in rats, 10.2 times more neurons in humans than in rats, and 2.3 times more neurons in humans than in monkeys. These findings suggest that the relative importance of the different inputs to the hip-pocampal formation (via entorhinal cortex layer II and layer III neu-rons, respectively) may vary between species. Specifically, the ratio between the number of neurons in layer III and the number of neu-rons in layer II is 2.4 in rats, 3.2 in monkeys, and 5.5 in humans. Thus, the direct entorhinal cortex projection to CA1 appears greater in pri-mates than in rats, and appears further developed in humans as com-pared to monkeys. Interestingly, the ratio between the number of neurons in the superficial layers and the number of neurons in the deep layers appears greater in monkeys and humans than in rats (Table 4). This pattern may be related to the greater development of the neocortical areas projecting to the superficial layers of the ento-rhinal cortex in primates. This finding is similar to what we observed previously for different amygdala nuclei (Chareyron et al., 2011), and consistent with the theory that brain structures with major anatomical and functional links evolve together independently of evolutionary changes in other unrelated structures (Barton & Harvey, 2000).

In sum, our detailed analysis of neuron numbers and neuronal soma size in the different layers of distinct subdivisions of the monkey entorhinal cortex confirms that the entorhinal cortex is a very hetero-geneous structure and that interspecies comparisons should take into account these important regional differences. Our data further sug-gest that, despite being consistent with functional studies in rodents and connectional studies in monkeys, a simple parcellation of the pri-mate entorhinal cortex into two major functional subregions, homolo-gous to the rodent LEC and MEC (Maass et al., 2015; Reagh et al., 2018; Schroder et al., 2015), may be too simplistic to capture the full complexity of information processing carried out by the human ento-rhinal cortex. The development of comprehensive high-resolution atlases of the human brain based on the microscopic evaluation of his-tological sections (Ding et al., 2017) may contribute to reach that goal. We will now focus on our findings in monkeys, in order to discuss the

TABLE 4 Number of neurons in the different layers of the entorhinal cortex, in rats, monkeys and humans

Rata Monkeyb Humanc M/R H/R H/M

II 103,820 499,784 652,500 4.81 6.28 1.31

III 252,875 1,621,012 3,590,500 6.41 14.20 2.21

V + VI 319,170 1,411,615 3,247,500 4.41 10.17 2.30

Total 675,865 3,532,411 7,490,500 5.23 11.08 2.12

(II + III)/(V + VI) 1.12 1.50 1.31

III/II 2.44 3.24 5.50

a

Average data of studies reported in Table 3.

bData from current study reported in Table 3. c

Average data of studies reported in Table 3.

Figure

FIGURE 1 Low magnification photomicrographs of coronal sections through the adult rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex
TABLE 2 Volume, neuron number, and neuron soma size in the different layers of the seven subdivisions of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex
FIGURE 5 (a) Ratio of the number of neurons in the superficial layers (II and III) and the number of neurons in the deep layers (V and VI) in the seven subdivisions of the rhesus monkey entorhinal cortex
TABLE 3 Number of neurons in the different layers of the entorhinal cortex in rats and humans

Références

Documents relatifs

Si l’on en juge par les résultats de l’évaluation lexicostatistique que l’on expose plus loin, ceci est surtout vrai des seules langues SBB mais l’est beaucoup moins et du

The fundamental outcome of the mission will be the Gaia catalogue produced by the Gaia Data Analysis and Pro- cessing Consortium (DPAC), which will contain a variable

We could spec- ulate that patients with metabolic syndrome and insulin resistance might probably benefit from a stringent protocol of glycemia control initiated in the

[r]

Parmi elles, et dans le cadre des récits de voyage en Perse, nous relèverons Adèle Hommaire de Hell et Carla Serena, auteures de récits qui eurent beaucoup de succès en leur temps?. A

Table Comparative evolution of autoantibodies levels, inflammatory parameters, and clinical features March 2003 July 2003 November 2003 December 2003 Anti c-ANCA

Face à des difficultés et des contraintes pour mettre en place une analyse de l’activité préalable à la formation, méthode classique, nous utilisons la complémentarité

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des