• Aucun résultat trouvé

Investigating the phonological status of the Initial Accent in French: an Event-Related Potentials study

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Investigating the phonological status of the Initial Accent in French: an Event-Related Potentials study"

Copied!
7
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-01577527

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01577527

Submitted on 26 Aug 2017

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

Investigating the phonological status of the Initial Accent in French: an Event-Related Potentials study

Noémie Te Rietmolen, Radouane Yagoubi, Robert Espesser, Cynthia Magnen, Corine Astésano

To cite this version:

Noémie Te Rietmolen, Radouane Yagoubi, Robert Espesser, Cynthia Magnen, Corine Astésano. In- vestigating the phonological status of the Initial Accent in French: an Event-Related Potentials study.

Speech Prosody 2016, May 2016, Boston, United States. �10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016-243�. �hal- 01577527�

(2)

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/303983721

Investigating the phonological status of the Initial Accent in French: an Event-Related Potentials study

Conference Paper · June 2016

DOI: 10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016-243

CITATIONS

2

READS

63

5 authors, including:

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

COGNIPROS. Linguistic and cognitive evaluation of prosodic production and perception in deviant speech: stress phenomena.View project

PhonIACog - The role of the Initial Accent in prosodic structuring in French – From phonology to speech processing -View project

Radouane El Yagoubi

University of Toulouse II - Le Mirail 20PUBLICATIONS 244CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Robert Espesser

French National Centre for Scientific Research 105PUBLICATIONS 1,019CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Corine Astésano

University of Toulouse, France 53PUBLICATIONS 450CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

(3)

Investigating the phonological status of the Initial Accent in French:

an Event-Related Potentials study

No´emie te Rietmolen1, Radouane El Yagoubi2, Robert Espesser3, Cynthia Magnen4, Corine Ast´esano1

1

U.R.I Octogone-Lordat (E.A. 4156), Universit´e de Toulouse, UTM, Toulouse, France

2

Laboratoire CLLE-LTC (UMR 5263), Universit´e de Toulouse, UTM, Toulouse, France

3

Laboratoire Parole & Langage (UMR 7309), Aix-Marseille Universit´e, Aix-en-Provence, France

4

MSHS-T (USR 3414), Universit´e de Toulouse, UTM, Toulouse, France

noemie.te-rietmolen@univ-tlse2.fr, yagoubi@univ-tlse2.fr, Robert.Espesser@lpl-aix.fr, cynthia.magnen@univ-tlse2.fr, astesano@univ-tlse2.fr

Abstract

This Event-Related Potentials (ERP) study investigates the use of prosodic information in the process of lexical access in French. In French, accentuation is said to be post-lexical, with a primary final accent (FA) and secondary initial accent (IA) marking the edges of the phrase. Results from previous studies, however, suggest IA may hold a demarcative function close to the level of the word. Still, the contribution of IA in word pro- cessing has not yet been empirically tested. In this study, par- ticipants listened to trisyllabic French nouns and pseudowords, with (+IA) or without (IA) initial accent while completing a lexical decision task. We were mainly interested in modula- tions of the N325, a component assumed to reflect difficulties in the extraction of lexical stress patterns. ERP results show a larger N325 when stimuli were presentedIA, revealing both the automaticity of stress extraction and a preference for stress templates with initial accent.

Index Terms: stress, French, Initial Accent, lexical decision, Event-Related Potentials, N325

1. Introduction

The ability to understand spoken language is a fundamental and intriguing human skill. Considering speech is formed out of connected and co-articulated units with no spaces or other breaks, the manner in which we translate its signal into a se- quence of words is far from obvious. One source that may help speech segmentation comes from the metrical structure of the signal. According to Metrical Segmentation Strategy (MSS), the segmentation of continuous speech is accomplished by re- lying on the dominant metrical pattern of the language [1].

In stress-based languages such as English and Dutch, where the vast majority of lexical words start with a strong syllable [2, 3], listeners are thought to exploit that high prosodic prob- ability and initiate lexical access at each stressed syllable. But, while this may be a successful strategy in languages with lex- ical stress, segmenting on strong onsets is arguably much less efficient in languages in which the domain for metrical rules is not the lexical word.

French is often described as a syllable-based language with fairly homogeneous metrical weight on syllables. Con- sequently, it is held that the French metrical structure is defined by the syllable and that the syllable is used as the basic unit for segmenting speech [4, 5, 6]. This idea is further supported by

the view that, in French, accentuation is post-lexical, demarcat- ing boundaries not at the level of the word but at the level of groups of words. That is, the primary French accent, known as the final accent (FA), is fixed on the last syllable of the phrase, marking its right edge. When necessary (e.g. in case of long stretches of unaccented syllables), FA can be accompanied with a secondary initial accent (IA) that marks the left edge of the phrase [7]. So, French is considered a language without lexical stress, making accents unlikely candidates to cue lexical access.

In contrast with this view, Di Cristo’s metrical model con- siders both FA and IA to be phonologically represented at the level of the prosodic word (i.e. close to the lexical word [8]) despite accentuation not being lexically distinctive in French [9]. According to this model, French accentuation thus pro- vides not one, but two entries; at the left boundary and at the right boundary of the word. A number of studies investigat- ing the use of French prosodic cues in word processing report results in line with Di Cristo’s conjecture of (latent) stress tem- plates underlying the representation of the prosodic word. Both the primary FAandthe secondary IA have been found to guide French listeners in the segmentation of speech (for use of FA see [10, 11, 12]; for use of IA see [13, 14, 15]). These studies challenge the idea that French listeners adopt a syllable-based segmentation strategy (as proposed by [16]). They instead favor a strategy in which listeners rely on metrical stress patterns dur- ing speech comprehension. They, however, do not challenge the view that IA and FA demarcate phrase boundaries, and still con- sider accentuation to apply to the level of the Accentual Phrase (AP; [17]) and not to the level of the prosodic word. Assum- ing Di Cristo’s view gives new perspectives on the speech seg- mentation strategy in French. Indeed, if French accentuation is actually a stress template encoded at the level close to the lex- ical word, IA and FA could readily notify listeners on when to initiate lexical access.

Here, we further investigate the representation of French accentuation and its contribution to word processing. More specifically, the status of the initial accent and its role in lex- ical access is examined. Because this accent is traditionally re- garded as a secondary and optional accent, only complementary to the final accent, up until recently IA has received relatively little scientific attention. However, it been shown that, simi- lar to FA, IA not only directs listeners in the segmentation of speech [13, 14, 15], but also that IA is a more reliable cue in the marking of lexical structure than FA [18]. In the study, IA

Speech Prosody 2016, May 31 - June 3, 2016, Boston, MA, USA

1181

(4)

was shown to mark lower levels of structure, close to the lexical word. A later study on the perception of prominences indicated that IA is perceived as stronger than FA, in a manner indepen- dent from the depth of prosodic structure. This points towards an association between IA and word demarcation [19].

Following up on these results, a recent event-related poten- tials (ERP) study was carried out that lends further support to the notion of a phonological representation of IA [20]. In the study, Aguilera et al. investigated the phonological status of IA, using an oddball paradigm in which the presence of IA was manipulated on trisyllabic words. When presenting the oddball without IA, a clear MisMatch Negativity component (MMN) emerged. But, when the oddball was presented with IA, the resultant MMN was significantly smaller. The authors took this to indicate that IA is represented at the phonological long- term representation of the word and part of the French preferred stress template.

In the present ERP study, we sought to build on the MMN study and manipulated the presence of IA in a lexical deci- sion task. We were particularly interested in modulations of the N325, a component assumed to reflect difficulties in the extrac- tion of lexical stress templates [21]. The component was first encountered in a study in which the authors presented Dutch participants with a stress discrimination task. In the task, se- quences of four bisyllabic words were presented with stress on the first syllable (the dominant metrical pattern in Dutch) or on the second syllable. Results showed that the less frequent stress template elicited a larger frontal negativity (the N325) than did the dominant stress template. This led the authors to conclude that the N325 may reflect the extraction of metrical stress during lexical access. If IA is linked to the phonological representation of prosodic words and is, along with FA, the expected stress template in French, presenting words without IA should elicit a larger N325 than presenting words with IA. To further attest for the pre-lexicality of stress encoding in French and question whether IA is part of the preferred metrical template, we test this metrical pattern on words and pseudowords.

2. Methods

2.1. Speech stimuli

The stimuli consisted of 120 trisyllabic French nouns (e.g.

chocolat) and120trisyllabic pseudowords (e.g. chibute). The stimuli were extracted from sentences spoken by a na¨ıve native speaker of French. In the sentences, the target words (lexical word or pseudoword) were placed at the beginning of a major phrase to increase the probability of clear IA and FA marking [18]. Stimuli with the most natural IA (+IA) were selected by a panel of three experts and re-synthesized without IA (IA) using a customized quadratic algorithm in PRAAT [22].

Using the same algorithm as [20], thef0value of the first vowel (i.e. IA) was lowered near thef0value of the preced- ing (unaccented) determinant, to deaccentuate the first sylla- ble (i.e. remove IA; see Figure 1). The algorithm progres- sively modified thef0values to reach thef0value at the be- ginning of the last (accented) vowel. This quadratic transfor- mation allowed for micro-prosodic variations to be maintained, thus keeping the natural sound of the stimuli. The+IA stim- uli were forward and back transformed to equalize the speech quality between +IA andIA stimuli. The duration of the target words was held constant in both stress conditions (+IA;

IA), since only thef0parameter was manipulated (lexical wordsm= 813, sd= 81; pseudowordsm= 844, sd= 83).

et les chi bu te et les chi bu te

(a)−IA (b)+IA

Figure 1: Example off0resynthesis (a)IA and (b)+IA on

’[et les] chibute’, with quadratic interpolation from thef0value of the preceding determinant to thef0value at the beginning of the last stressed syllable forIA targets.

2.2. Participants

26 French native speakers, aged1931(mean age25.4; 20 females), took part in the study. All subjects were right-handed, with normal hearing abilities and no reported history of neuro- logical or language-related problems. Due to excessive artifacts in the EEG signal, 3 participants were excluded from analyses.

2.3. Procedure

Each participant was comfortably seated in an electrically shielded and sound attenuated room. Stimuli were presented through headphones and participants were allowed to adjust the volume to their individual preferences.

Participants were instructed to judge as quickly and accu- rately as possible whether a word was a real word or a pseu- doword by pressing the left or right button on a button-box (but- ton assignment was counter-balanced across participants). To ensure participants understood the task requirements, the exper- iment began with a short practice phase. This phase consisted of 12 trials that were very similar to the experimental trials, but were not included in the analyses.

Each participant listened to all 240 stimuli. Using Latin square designs, the four conditions (word +IA, wordIA, pseudoword+IA, pseudowordIA) were evenly distributed over blocks, and block order was balanced between participants.

In order to better control for eye-related EEG activity, each trial started with a400ms presentation of a white fixation cross at the center of a computer screen. The stimulus was presented immediately after the offset of the fixation cross. Participants were given a maximum of 2000 ms to give their answer. The intertrial interval (ITI) followed the participants’ response and lasted until 2500 ms post stimulus onset. As a result, the dura- tion of ITI varied, while trial duration was fixed at 2900 ms. To- tal duration of the experiment, including the set-up of the EEG electrodes, was approximately2h.

2.4. EEG recording and preprocessing

The EEG data were recorded with 32 Ag/AgCl-sintered elec- trodes mounted on an elastic cap and located at standard left and right hemisphere positions over frontal, central, parietal, oc- cipital and temporal areas (International 10/20 System; Jasper, 1958) at Fz, Cz, Pz, Oz, Fp1, Fp2, AF3, F3, AF4, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, PO3, PO4, P5, P6, O1, O2, F7, F8, T3, T4, T5, T6, FC5, FC6, CP1, CP2, CP5 and CP6. To detect blinks and eye- movements, 4 additional electrodes were placed around the eyes (HEOG: bipolar channel placed lateral to the outer corner of both eyes; VEOG: bipolar channel placed above and below the left eye). The EEG and EOG signals were amplified by BioSemi amplifiers (ActiveTwo System) and digitized at 512 Hz.

The data were preprocessed using the EEGLAB package [23] in Matlab [24]. Each electrode was re-referenced offline to

(5)

64 2 0 -2 -4 -6

-100 0 200 400 600 800

6 4 2 0 -2 -4 -6

-100 0 200 400 600 800

6 4 2 0 -2 -4 -6

-100 0 200 400 600 800

wordpseudoword

P200

N100 N325

P200

N100 N325

P200

N100 N325

µV

ms

FC2 (a)

IA

(b)

+IA

(c)

main effect

Figure 2:Grand average P200 in the lexical condition (word, pseudoword), recorded at the FC2 (frontocentral) electrode for: (a)IA, (b)+IA, (c) main effect. The vertical gray bars indicate the selected P200 time window (151251ms). For ease of presentation, ERP waveforms are cut off at 800 ms. Negativity is plotted as an upward deflection.

the algebraic average of the left and right mastoids. The data were band-pass filtered between 0.01-30 Hz and epoched from -0.2 to 2 seconds surrounding the onset of the speech signal.

Following a visual inspection, epochs containing EMG or other artifacts not related to eye-movements or blinks were manually removed. Independent Components Analysis (ICA) was per- formed on the remaining epochs in order to identify and sub- tract components containing oculomotor artifacts from the data.

Finally, data were averaged within and across participants to obtain the grand-averages for each of the 4 conditions.

2.5. EEG analysis

With its high temporal resolution, EEG provides a rich database to determine the exact latency of an effect. However, testing at all data points independently quickly leads to a multiple com- parison problem where the risk of making Type I errors in- creases considerably. As EEG measures are not independent, but in fact temporally and spatially correlated, we used a non- parametrictmaxpermutation test to analyze the data [25, 26].

Intmaxpermutation testing, the null distribution is estimated by repeatedly resampling the obtained data and calculatingt- scores for each sample. The most extremet-scores (tmax) are selected for the null distribution. Finally, the t-scores of the observed data are computed and compared to the simulatedtmax

distribution, just as in parametric hypothesis testing.

As with each permutation the chance of obtaining a large tmax increases, the test automatically becomes more conserva- tive when making more comparisons. Also, since the actual, ob- tained data is used to estimate the null distribution, the test does not assume test independence, allowing for stringent control of Type I error without considerable decrease in sensitivity. To fur- ther maximize power and reduce the number of comparisons, the data were down-sampled to 125 Hz and time-windows were estimated following the method used in [21]. We were mainly interested in modulations of the P200 (151251ms) and the N325 (201431ms), as these two components reflect auditory processes in the pre-lexical stage of word processing (acoustical processing and stress extraction, respectively [27] [21]).

Each comparison of interest was analyzed with a separate repeated measures, two-tailedt-tests, using the original data and 2500 random permutations to approximate the null distribution for the customary family-wise alpha (α) level of 0.051.

1In fact we used more than twice the number of permutations Manly suggested for an alpha at5%.[28]

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral results

Behavioral data (error rates and reaction times) were analyzed with paired two-tailedt-tests in R [29]. Overall, performance on the lexical decision task revealed high accuracy (<5%errors) with no differences between conditions. Reaction times showed a main effect of lexicality (t = 16.85, p < 0.001); words were responded to faster than pseudowords. Presence of IA had no effect on response latencies (p= 0.7,ns).

3.2. ERP results

In the P200 time-window (Figure 2), there was a main effect of lexicality (critical t-score: ±3.5589, p < 0.05). Pseu- dowords elicited a larger P200 than words in the frontocentral region (FC2) peaking182ms after stimulus presentation. The difference between words and pseudowords was also signifi- cant within the condition without IA (criticalt-score:±3.575, p < 0.05). Within the condition with IA this effect was not significant (p= 0.4,ns).

In the N325 time-window (Figure 3), there was a main ef- fect of presence of IA (criticalt-score: ±3.6887,p < 0.05).

Compared to stimuli+IA, stimuliIA elicited a larger nega- tivity in the frontocentral region (FC2 and Cz) from318358 ms after stimulus presentation. The difference in ERP ampli- tude is small, but robust and comparable to the amplitude differ- ence reported in B¨ocker et al (12.5µV). The effect was also significant within the lexical words condition (criticalt-score:

±3.8546,p <0.05); wordsIA resulted in a larger negativity than words+IA. There was a similar trend in the pseudowords condition, although it did not reach significance (p= 0.09,ns).

A visual inspection of the ERPs, however, suggests similar pro- cesses between words and pseudowords.

4. Discussion

In the present study, we examined the interplay between ac- centuation and lexical access in French. We were particularly interested in the status and possible roles of the initial accent.

Our results show that IA is represented in the French preferred and expected stress template. As pre-lexical language-specific stress templates are suggested to serve as gateways to the men- tal lexicon, IA could thus play an important role in the process of speech segmentation in French.

We used a lexical decision task in which we manipulated the presence of IA. The manipulation modulated the resultant frontocentral N325; a larger N325 emerged when IA had been

1183

(6)

6 4 2 0 -2 -4 -6

-100 0 200 400 600 800

6 4 2 0 -2 -4 -6

-100 0 200 400 600 800

6 4 2 0 -2 -4 -6

-100 0 200 400 600 800

+IA

IA

P200 N100 N325

P200 N100 N325

P200 N100 N325 µV

ms

FC2 (a)

lexical word

(b)

pseudoword

(c)

main effect

Figure 3: Grand average N325 in the±IA condition, recorded at the FC2 (frontocentral) electrode for: (a) lexical words, (b) pseu- dowords, (c) main effect.The vertical gray bars indicate the selected time window (201431ms). For ease of presentation, ERP waveforms are cut off at 800 ms. Negativity is plotted as an upward deflection.

omitted. As the N325 is assumed to reflect difficulties in the extraction of lexical stress, this result indicates a stress pro- cessing cost when stimuli are presented without IA. Recall that B¨ocker et al. report similar findings after manipulating stress in Dutch, a language with lexical stress [21]. In their study, lis- teners were asked to discriminate between the Dutch dominant stress template and a less frequent stress template. Words pre- sented without the dominant stress pattern elicited a more ample N325. In our study, words presented without IA resulted in the larger N325 suggesting that, even though stress is not lexically distinctive in French, IA is part of the French expected stress pattern (cf[20]). In addition, while in the study of B¨ocker et al.

participants were asked to explicitly attend the metrical struc- ture of the stimuli, in the present study, attention was diverted from the stress manipulation using a lexical decision task. Still finding a robust modulation of the N325, further demonstrates that word processing naturally engaged the pre-lexical extrac- tion of IA. That is, we show that lexical access is facilitated when words are presented with the French preferred stress tem- plate, i.e. with the initial accent.

The amplitude modulation of the N325 was small (between 12.5µV), but robust as revealed by our conservative non- parametric statistics (see Methods section). In fact, finding a relatively small difference in amplitude was expected and com- parable to the amplitude difference in B¨ocker et al. [21]. Similar to B¨ocker et al., we did not manipulate the legality, but rather the probability of the presented stress templates. That is, while in French there is a preference for words marked with IA, words without IA are not illegal. Indeed, in continuous speech IA is not always realized and may be suppressed to serve for instance a more rhythmically balancing function. So, while French lis- teners may expect and prefer words to be marked with IA, words without IA do not exceedingly hamper word processing.

Our manipulation of IA did not modulate the P200, a com- ponent thought to reflect the bottom-up extraction of purely physical/acoustical parameters [27]. This indicates that our re- sults reveal a more controlled process in which stress is ex- tracted in a top-down fashion. We did find lexicality to affect the P200 when stimuli were presented without IA; pseudowords elicited a more ample P200 than did words when presented without IA. This effect is surprising since the latency range of the P200 precedes lexical processing [30]. Considering the lo- cation of the P200 (frontocentral; similar to the location of the N325) the effect appears to be the product of a temporal overlap between the P200 and the N325. The N325 was more negative forIA stimuli than+IA stimuli and this difference was larger in the words condition than in the pseudowords condition. This

means that in theIA condition the overlap between the P200 and N325 will be more evident for words than for pseudowords, while in the+IA condition the overlap will be smaller (as+IA stimuli elicited a smaller N325). In fact, B¨ocker et al. report a similar overlap between the N325 and the P200 at the fron- tocentral electrodes. Finding an overlap between the N325 and the P200 implies that the process of stress extraction starts be- fore our predefined N325 time-window (201431ms) and during the P200 time-window (151251ms). Such an early latency confirms that lexical access crucially involved an auto- matic, pre-lexical extraction of the French initial accent.

A visual inspection of the ERP components suggests±IA also affected the later integration stages of word processing, as there seems to be an amplitude difference in the latency range typically associated with the N400 [31]. It is however unlikely that these late amplitude modulations really reflect difficulties in the post-lexical process of semantic integration, as word were presented in isolation. Moreover, the N400 is typically maximal over centroparietal sites [31, 32], while the reported ERPs in the current study have a frontocentral distribution. A more probable explanation is suggested by B¨ocker et al., who encountered a similar late frontocentral amplitude difference and interpret it to reflect N325 residue. A study to determine if±IA also affects the later stages of speech processing, is currently in progress.

In the study,±IA words are embedded within a congruent or incongruent semantic context, and as such, the study is better adapted to give insight into whether IA also affects the later stages of speech processing.

5. Conclusions

In this study, we investigated the status of the French initial accent. Our ERP results demonstrate that IA is linked to the phonological representation of words. Words presented without initial accent elicited a more ample N325, a component that in- dexes difficulties in pre-lexical stress extraction. Moreover, as we diverted attention away from our stress manipulation with a lexical decision task, the extraction of IA seems to be an au- tomatic step during the early stages of word processing. This indicates that the initial accent is part of the French preferred stress template and as such, contrary to popular belief, plays a valuable role in French speech comprehension.

6. Acknowledgments

This study is supported by the Agence Nationale de la Recherche grant ANR-12-BSH2-0001 (PI: Corine Ast´esano)

(7)

7. References

[1] A. Cutler and D. Norris, “The role of strong syllables in segmenta- tion for lexical access.”J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., vol. 14, no. 1, pp. 113–121, 1988.

[2] A. Cutler and D. M. Carter, “The predominance of strong ini- tial syllables in the English vocabulary,”Comput. Speech Lang., vol. 2, no. 3-4, pp. 133–142, 1987.

[3] J. Vroomen and B. De Gelder, “Metrical segmentation and lexical inhibition in spoken word recognition.”Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, vol. 21, no. 1, p. 98, 1995.

[4] J. Mehler and R. Hayes, “The role of syllables in speech process- ing: Infant and adult data [and discussion],”Philosophical Trans- actions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol. 295, no.

1077, pp. 333–352, 1981.

[5] J. Mehler, J. Y. Dommergues, U. Frauenfelder, and J. Segui, “The syllable’s role in speech segmentation,”Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, vol. 20, no. 3, pp. 298–305, 1981.

[6] A. Cutler, J. Mehler, D. Norris, and J. Segui, “The syllable’s dif- fering role in the segmentation of french and english,”Journal of memory and language, vol. 25, no. 4, pp. 385–400, 1986.

[7] M. Rossi, L’intonation: le syst`eme du franc¸ais: description et mod´elisation. Editions Ophrys, 1999.

[8] E. Selkirk, “The prosodic structure of function words,”Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisi- tion, vol. 187, p. 214, 1996.

[9] A. Di Cristo, “Vers une mod´elisation de l’accentuation du franc¸ais (seconde partie),”Journal of French Language Studies, vol. 10, no. 01, pp. 27–44, 2000.

[10] M.-H. Banel and N. Bacri, “On metrical patterns and lexical pars- ing in french,”Speech Communication, vol. 15, no. 1, pp. 115–

126, 1994.

[11] O. Bagou, C. Fougeron, and U. H. Frauenfelder, “Contribution of prosody to the segmentation and storage of” words” in the acqui- sition of a new mini-language,” inSpeech Prosody 2002, Interna- tional Conference, 2002.

[12] A. Christophe, S. Peperkamp, C. Pallier, E. Block, and J. Mehler,

“Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access i. adult data,”Journal of Memory and Language, vol. 51, no. 4, pp. 523–

547, 2004.

[13] P. Welby, “The role of early fundamental frequency rises and elbows in french word segmentation,”Speech Communication, vol. 49, no. 1, pp. 28–48, 2007.

[14] E. Spinelli, P. Welby, and A.-L. Schaegis, “Fine-grained access to targets and competitors in phonemically identical spoken se- quences: the case of french elision,”Language and cognitive pro- cesses, vol. 22, no. 6, pp. 828–859, 2007.

[15] E. Spinelli, N. Grimault, F. Meunier, and P. Welby, “An into- national cue to word segmentation in phonemically identical se- quences,”Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, vol. 72, no. 3, pp. 775–787, 2010.

[16] A. Content, R. K. Kearns, and U. H. Frauenfelder, “Boundaries versus onsets in syllabic segmentation,”Journal of Memory and Language, vol. 45, no. 2, pp. 177–199, 2001.

[17] S.-A. Jun and C. Fougeron, “A phonological model of french in- tonation,” inIntonation. Springer, 2000, pp. 209–242.

[18] C. Ast´esano, E. G. Bard, and A. Turk, “Structural influences on initial accent placement in French.”Lang. Speech, vol. 50, no. Pt 3, pp. 423–446, 2007.

[19] C. Ast´esano, R. Bertrand, R. Espesser, and N. Nguyen, “Percep- tion des fronti`eres et des pro´eminences en franc¸ais,”JEP-TALN- RECITAL, 2012.

[20] M. Aguilera, R. El Yagoubi, R. Espesser, and C. Ast´esano,

“Event-Related Potential of Initial Accent Processing in French,”

Speech Prosody 2014, no. Umr 5263, pp. 3–7, 2014.

[21] K. B. B¨ocker, M. Bastiaansen, J. Vroomen, C. H. Brunia, and B. Gelder, “An erp correlate of metrical stress in spoken word recognition,” Psychophysiology, vol. 36, no. 6, pp. 706–720, 1999.

[22] P. Boersma and D. Weenink, “{P}raat: doing phonetics by com- puter,” 2010.

[23] A. Delorme and S. Makeig, “Eeglab: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial eeg dynamics including independent component analysis,”Journal of neuroscience methods, vol. 134, no. 1, pp. 9–21, 2004.

[24] T. MathWorks, “Image processing toolboxtm user’s guide, version r2014a,”The MathWorks, 2014.

[25] D. M. Groppe, T. P. Urbach, and M. Kutas, “Mass univariate anal- ysis of event-related brain potentials/fields i: A critical tutorial review,”Psychophysiology, vol. 48, no. 12, pp. 1711–1725, 2011.

[26] S. J. Luck,An introduction to the event-related potential tech- nique. MIT press, 2014.

[27] S. A. Hillyard and T. W. Picton, “Electrophysiology of cognition,”

Comprehensive Physiology, 1987.

[28] B. Manly, “Randomization, bootstrap, and monte carlo methods in biologychapman and hall,”New York, 1997.

[29] R. C. Team, “The r project for statistical computing,”R Foun- dation for Statistical Computing web-site. www. R-project. org.

Accessed June, vol. 9, 2014.

[30] F. Grosjean, “Spoken word recognition processes and the gating paradigm,”Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 28, no. 4, pp. 267–

283, 1980.

[31] C. Brown and P. Hagoort, “The processing nature of the n400: Ev- idence from masked priming,”Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol. 5, no. 1, pp. 34–44, 1993.

[32] M. Kutas and K. D. Federmeier, “Electrophysiology reveals se- mantic memory use in language comprehension,”Trends in cog- nitive sciences, vol. 4, no. 12, pp. 463–470, 2000.

1185

View publication stats View publication stats

Références

Documents relatifs

However, recent studies suggest that verbal working memory is not always involved in syntactic and semantic processing: Some patients with severely reduced verbal span have shown

Afin de mettre en évidence que l’erreur de transmission est un indicateur de la géométrie et de l’excitation, nous avons établi un programme de simulation de l’influence

In Experiment 1, in which primes were presented without the assimilating context, a significantly larger priming effect was obtained for the unassimilated

Finally, while the main effect of ± IA did not reach signi- ficance (p = 0.24, ns; Figure 3a), we did find a significant interaction effect between semantic congruity and ±IA in

Es ist vielleicht kein Zufall, daß eine neue, differen- ziertere Sicht auf Schnabel von einem profilierten Historiker der politischen Theorien der frühen Neuzeit kommt, der nicht

Among the syllable- related features are shwa epenthesis, Umlaut, resyllabification, the n-rule, voice assimilation and the presence of shwa in stressed syllables (in

GHARIB Noureddine Chirurgie plastique et

which dimensions are recruited (among all the potential dimensions) and 2. how segments spread along these dimensions. Our approach rests on this hypothesis of variable phonetic