• Aucun résultat trouvé

Mundurucu Number Words as a Window on Short-Term Memory

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Mundurucu Number Words as a Window on Short-Term Memory"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: halshs-01497577

https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-01497577

Submitted on 28 Dec 2019

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access

archive for the deposit and dissemination of

sci-entific research documents, whether they are

pub-lished or not. The documents may come from

teaching and research institutions in France or

abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est

destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents

scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non,

émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de

recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires

publics ou privés.

Mundurucu Number Words as a Window on Short-Term

Memory

Johan Rooryck, Alberto Tonda, Jairo Saw, Pierre Pica

To cite this version:

Johan Rooryck, Alberto Tonda, Jairo Saw, Pierre Pica. Mundurucu Number Words as a Window on

Short-Term Memory. The origins of numerical abilities: the future, Feb 2017, Chicheley Hall, United

Kingdom. 2017. �halshs-01497577�

(2)

1. Properties of Mundurucu number words

2. Classifier words and STM collections

3. Evidence

1

Leiden University,

2

Université Paris-Saclay,

3

Sawre Aboy,

4

CNRS & Instituto do Cerébro, Natal

For further information: pierrepica@gmail.com http://www.pierrepica.com

4. Conclusions

Figure 1 from Pica et al., 2004

Word Gloss Reference Syllables

Pûg “One” One One

Xepxep “Fat–redup” Two Two

Ebapûg “Your arm(s) and one” About three Three

Ebadipdip “Your parent(s)–redup” About four Four Table 1

We observe that Mundurucu classifiers express iconic properties of working memory, such as grouping and serial ordering.

Classifiers can also combine to increase the size

of the magnitude they express (e.g. badip = ba + dip).

If Mundurucu number words reflect the organization of working memory, and they show a specific ordering, this perceived ordering can also be a property of working memory, as some works on subitizing seem to suggest (see for example von Szeliski, 1924 and Garner, 1948).

In Mundurucu, each number word is one syllable longer than the previous one. Moreover, they seem articulated in two distinct parts, expressing a set and the successor

function, respectively (Pica, 2016).

Figure 1, Give-a-Number task (after Wynn, 1990)

« Give me three seeds! »

Stimuli: pûg ,xep xep, ebapûg,

ebadipdip, pûg pôgbi, etc (from Izard et al., 2008)

Short-term memory (STM) is organized in collections of concepts that have strong association with one another and much weaker associations to chunks already in use. There is strong evidence that STM has a limit of 4 slots

that can store information (Cowan, 2001). STM slots

2-slot collection

3-slot collection Interestingly, Mundurucu number words are made out of

classifiers, which are traditionally viewed in linguistics as

elements making reference to specific geometrical features of an object.

ako-ba = “Banana fruit”

ako-dip = “Field of banana trees” waje-ba = “Cocoa fruit”

waje-dip = “Field of cocoa trees”

In general, it seems that

xep < ba <dip < badip

Could

characteristics

of Mundurucu

number words

be related to

properties of

working

memory?

Mundurucu speakers are limited in the expression of numbers because they somehow only use working memory.

The remaining question is: how does the representation of working memory properties relate to the properties of the Approximate Number System?

The Mundurucu linguistic system seems to indicate that objects are perceived mainly with respect to their spatiotemporal characteristics, somewhat ignoring the full identity of the object. This is coherent with observations on STM and its relation to object file, from which it emerges that object file representation can only make reference to spatiotemporal features.

Even though Mundurucu number words do not possess an exact cardinality, they still feature a number of syllables that corresponds to the number of STM slots used to store that many objects.

Figure

Figure 1 from Pica et al., 2004

Références

Documents relatifs

Original measures of WMC, such as reading span and operation span (also known as complex span tasks, see the measurement section below), were shown to be more strongly correlated

Central executive, phonological loop, phonological awareness, native and foreign vocabulary knowledge, language comprehension, and reading were investigated.. longitudinally in

Recent functional neuroimaging techniques permit investigation of the information represented within particular brain regions, rather than whether or not they.. Such

These findings suggest that complex WM span task tap into a fundamental aspect of cognition that is shared with measures of fluid intelligence and that might represent the ability

Visual array comparison tasks have not been used as often as memory span tasks to investigate individual differences in cognitive ability.. However, recent

We first sought to identify brain regions differentially involved in processing reference (RM) or working memory (WM) with and without interference (involving or not forgetting) using

Our modeling study suggests that selective persistent activity could indeed emerge from astrocyte-mediated short-term synaptic facilitation triggered by the brief presentation of

(2) It is also possible that children suffer from a restricted capacity to switch attention between processing and maintenance activities.. It would be less easy for children