• Aucun résultat trouvé

Males' calls carry information about individual identity and morphological characteristics of the caller in burrowing petrels

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Males' calls carry information about individual identity and morphological characteristics of the caller in burrowing petrels"

Copied!
29
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-02371209

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02371209

Submitted on 17 Nov 2020

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci-entific research documents, whether they are pub-lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

Males’ calls carry information about individual identity

and morphological characteristics of the caller in

burrowing petrels

Charlène Gémard, Thierry Aubin, Francesco Bonadonna

To cite this version:

Charlène Gémard, Thierry Aubin, Francesco Bonadonna. Males’ calls carry information about indi-vidual identity and morphological characteristics of the caller in burrowing petrels. Journal of Avian Biology, Wiley, 2019, 50 (12), pp.e02270, doi:10.1111/jav.02270. �10.1111/jav.02270�. �hal-02371209�

(2)

Call rate, fundamental frequency and syntax determine

male-call attractiveness in blue petrels Halobaena caerulea

Charlène Gémard

1,2

, Thierry Aubin

2

, Eliette L. Reboud

1

, Francesco Bonadonna

1

1 CEFE, Univ Montpellier, CNRS, Univ Paul Valéry Montpellier 3, EPHE, IRD, Montpellier, France

2 Equipe Communications Acoustiques, UMR 9197, Neuro-PSI-CNRS, Université Paris-Sud, Bat.446, 91405

Orsay, France.

(3)

Acknowledgments

This work was financially and logistically supported by the French polar institute Paul Emile Victor (IPEV,

program no. 354 ETHOTAAF). We are very grateful to Jessica Graham who improved the English of this

manuscript, and to Guilhem Battistella and Jean-Yves Barnagaud for their precious help on the field.

10  11 

Abstract

12 

In blue petrels (Halobaena caerulea), females are supposed to be particularly choosy and mate choice 13 

can take a couple of years. In these lifelong monogamous seabirds, choosing a good mate is crucial and 14 

has a strong influence on their fitness. Due to their nocturnal habits, the absence of sexual dimorphism 15 

and the physical barrier between males calling from their burrow and females flying above the colony, 16 

vocal signals seem to be one of the main channels for males to communicate with potential mates. 17 

However vocal communication is also costly as it attracts predators. In a previous study, we investigated 18 

whether acoustic traits of male calls carry information about morphological characteristics that might 19 

be indicators of males’ qualities. Here, we test whether these acoustic traits linked to male characteristics 20 

are actually attractive to females. To do so, we played-back modified calls of males to females in a 21 

colony of blue petrels of the Kerguelen archipelago. We found that flying females were more attracted 22 

by high-pitched calls and by calls broadcasted at a high call rate. Previous studies showed a relationship 23 

between pitch and bill depth and length. In filter-feeding birds, such as blue petrels, bill morphology 24 

influences feeding efficiency. A high call rate is an indicator of sexual motivation and makes the caller 25 

easier to locate by potential mates and predators in the hubbub of the colony. We thus hypothesized that 26 

producing frequent high-pitched calls may be the result of a trade-off between predation avoidance and 27 

conspicuous sexual signalling. 28 

Keywords

29 

Vocal communication, sexual signal, attractiveness, mate choice, seabird, blue petrel 30 

(4)

Introduction

Knowing how animal communication systems work is crucial to understanding social 2 

behaviours. Indeed, social interactions are mediated by signals in one or several modalities, such as 3 

acoustic, olfactory, or visual (Bradbury and Vehrencamp 2011). Among these, vocal signals provide 4 

information mainly to conspecifics, but not exclusively (see Magrath et al. 2015 for a review). Signals

carry different kinds of information, stable information on long-term (e.g. species, sex, social status, 6 

group membership, phenotypic characteristics: Searcy and Nowicki 2005) and temporary information 7 

through acoustic modulations (e.g. motivation: Morton 1977, emotion: Briefer 2018). Vocalizations may 8 

also bear stable individual signatures to allow efficient discrimination among individuals (Beecher 1989, 9 

Tibbets and Dale 2007), particularly in social species. For instance, it has been shown in rodents that 10 

alarm calls contain more individual information in species living in large groups than species living in 11 

small ones (Pollard and Blumstein 2011). 12 

Informative content of vocal signals has been particularly well-studied in birds in the framework 13 

of sexual selection likely because the “dual function hypothesis” states that birdsongs serve two main 14 

purposes: attract mates and deter rivals (Kroodsma and Byers 1991, Catchpole and Slater 2008). Due to 15 

high costs (predation, social aggression, etc) and physiological constraints associated with their 16 

production, vocalizations are considered as honest and reliable signals of male phenotypic qualities, 17 

such as body size and body condition (Gil and Gahr 2002). Individual differences in phenotypic qualities 18 

among males are translated by differences in structure of vocal signals (Rowe and Houlde 1996). 19 

Several acoustic parameters have been shown to reflect a singer’s characteristics linked to 20 

overall male quality, and there is evidence that females are attracted by specific acoustic parameters 21 

related to male qualities (see Nowicki and Searcy 2004 for a review). A well-known example is the 22 

fundamental frequency (or pitch) which negatively correlates with body size (e.g. Galeotti et al. 1997, 23 

Mager et al. 2007, Favaro et al. 2017, Kriesell et al. 2018). In many species, larger males have a higher 24 

breeding success because body mass is an indicator of physical strength and/or foraging success (Chastel 25 

et al. 1995, Salton et al. 2015) and females rely on fundamental frequency as a sexual signal (review in 26 

Cardoso 2012). Besides frequency parameters, females rely on song output structure, complexity, and 27 

(5)

vocal performance (Nowicki and Searcy 2004). For instance, they are attracted by songs constituted of 28 

many and/or complex elements (Martín-Vivaldi et al. 2000, Martín-Vivaldi 2004) which may indicate 29 

a male in good body condition (Martín-Vivaldi et al. 1998). Females are also attracted by males that 30 

perform challenging songs (i.e. with a high rate and/or a large frequency bandwidth), that indicates body 31 

size, age, and endurance (Ballentine et al. 2004, Ballentine 2009, Byers et al. 2016). 32 

In many bird species, among the functions that calls and songs fulfil, there is the transmission 33 

of individual signature information (Falls 1982, Lambrechts and Dhont 1995). Vocal individual 34 

signature is a crucial piece of information as it provides the basis for individual recognition required for 35 

almost all aspects of social life (Tibbetts and Dale 2007). Individual recognition is possible when an 36 

individual identifies another according to the easily-distinctive characteristics of its signal, for example 37 

acoustic parameters of a call (Falls 1982, Dale et al. 2001). Vocal signature has been assessed in many 38 

non-songbird species, especially seabirds (e.g. Robisson et al. 1993, Charrier et al. 2001, Aubin and 39 

Jouventin 2002, Aubin et al. 2007, Favaro et al. 2017). In many seabirds, adults usually breed in dense 40 

colonies (Croxall and Prince 1980, Wittenberg and Hunt 1985), form lifelong monogamous bonds 41 

(Warham 1996), and forage far from the colony. Individual recognition would be advantageous when 42 

returning to the nest/mate/chick (Warham 1990, Mathevon et al. 2003, Jouventin et al. 1999, Curé et al. 43 

2011), especially for species that have no nest site to meet at (Robisson et al. 1993, Aubin and Jouventin 44 

2002). 45 

Among Procellariidae, burrowing petrels occupy deep burrows and are active, at the colony, at 46 

night during the breeding period (Warham 1990). In this context, visual communication seems strongly 47 

restricted whereas olfactory and vocal signals may be used for long-range communication. The role of 48 

olfaction has been investigated in petrel social interactions. Studies suggest that the chemical signature 49 

of nests is used by mates to find their own burrow (Bonadonna et al. 2001, Bonadonna et al. 2004), and 50 

that individual body’s odour carries individuality signals and some genetic information that may 51 

influence mate choice (Bonadonna and Nevitt 2004; Bonadonna and Sanz-Aguilar 2012; Leclaire et al 52 

2017). Comparatively, acoustic communication has been poorly studied so far. For some burrowing-53 

petrel species, vocal communication is extremely costly because it exposes callers to an increased risk 54 

of predation (Mougeot and Bretagnolle 2000). However, during pair formation, bachelor males looking

(6)

for a mate intensely vocalize at night from their burrow, while females fly and call over the colony 56 

(Bretagnolle 1990, Warham 1996). These costly calls are likely to be reliable sexual signals that attract 57 

and/or stimulate suitable mates (Bretagnolle 1990), but we still ignore which one, between male or 58 

female, stimulates the other. Petrels also vocalize after being vocally challenged by a conspecific close 59 

to the entrance of their burrow. These vocalizations might be aggressive signals to defend the own 60 

burrow (occupied year after year) from intruders (Warham 1990, Warham 1996). According to 61 

Bretagnolle (1996), burrowing petrels have a small vocal repertoire. For instance, genera Halobaena 62 

and Pachyptila produce a single major call produce in both sexual and agonistic interactions. 63 

As previously mentioned, calling in the colony increases the predation risks (Mougeot and

64 

Bretagnolle 2000) and should thus provide important benefits to balance these costs. Following the 65 

suggestions of the wide literature on other bird species, we can hypothesize that calls play a role in mate 66 

choice and/or male competition for mate and burrow. However, informative content of most burrowing-67 

petrel species’ calls have been neglected so far. Nevertheless, previous studies have shown that heavy 68 

males call with a higher rhythm than light males in blue petrels (Genevois and Bretagnolle 1994) and 69 

that large heavy males produce low-pitched calls in snow petrels (Barbraud et al. 2000), suggesting 70 

several ways of body size signalling. Here, we investigated the informative content of calls in two 71 

burrowing petrels: the blue petrel (Halobaena caerulea) and the Antarctic prion (Pachyptila desolata) 72 

(Procellariidae, Gmelin 1789). These two petrel species, with similar breeding and feeding ecology 73 

(Warham 1990, Warham 1996), are highly vocal and suffer from a high predation pressure by the brown 74 

skua (Mougeot and Bretagnolle 1998). We tested the hypotheses that acoustic parameters in both 75 

temporal and spectral domains of a petrel calls (i) reflect morphological characteristics, and (ii) bear an 76 

individual vocal signature. 77 

Material and Methods

78 

Studied species

79 

Blue petrels (H. caerulea) and Antarctic prions (P. desolata) are monogamous medium-sized 80 

(about 190 and 160 g, respectively) seabirds belonging to the Procellariidae. They spend most of their 81 

life at sea but they breed in dense colonies on coastal grass slops. This long-lived seabirds do not reach 82 

(7)

sexual maturity for five years. A couple of years is needed for establishing stable pair-bonds. They show 83 

a high partner fidelity, low extra-pair paternities, and divorces are rare. After pairing, pairs produce a 84 

single egg per year. From the incubation (50 days in blue petrels, 45 in Antarctic prions) to the fledging 85 

(50 and 53 days, respectively), they exhibit bi-parental cares. Partners regularly alternate fasting period 86 

in the burrow and feeding trips at sea (Warham 1990, Warham 1996, Brooke 2004). 87 

Callers are mainly non-breeder males and females looking for a mate but they are tricky to catch, 88 

as they occupy inaccessible burrows and fly over the colony, respectively. On the contrary, breeders 89 

usually have high mate and burrow fidelity over years (Warham 1996, Brooke 2004). Occupied burrows 90 

can be equipped with an artificial access to the incubating chamber, by digging a tunnel and closing it 91 

with a stone. From this artificial door, birds become easily accessible. For this reason, we worked only 92 

on breeding birds. After some infructuous attempts to obtain females’ replies to playback, we 93 

constrained our study to only on males’ calls. According to the literature, very few breeding females

94 

react to the playback of bachelor males’ calls (Brooke 1978, Bretagnolle and Lequette 1990, Bretagnolle 95 

1996). A possible explanation is that, in natural conditions, females call only in flight, whereas males 96 

call from their burrow. The study was thus restricted on breeding males of the two petrel species: 31 97 

blue petrels (16 in 2017 and 15 in 2018) and 24 Antarctic prions (16 in 2017 and 8 in 2018). Mates take 98 

a shift every eight to ten days incubating the eggs (Warham 1990), and occasions when both mates are 99 

present in the nest at the same time are quite rare. This enabled us to record the male alone. Petrels well 100 

tolerate weak human disturbance (Bergès et al. 2019), allowing us to conduct several recording sessions 101 

on the same individuals to assess the individual signature. All the birds were metal ringed and 102 

individually identifiable. 103 

Study location

104 

Fieldwork was performed in a small sub-Antarctic island (Ile Verte, 49°51′ S, 70°05′ E) of the Kerguelen 105 

Archipelago, in the southern Indian Ocean, where blue petrels and the Antarctic prions gather in stable 106 

colonies during the breeding season. We conducted the study during the 2017 and 2018 birds’ incubation 107 

period (November 25th 2017 to December 12th 2017 and November 27th 2018 to December 20th 2018 for 108 

(8)

blue petrels; December 23rd 2017 to January 16th 2018 and December 25th 2018 to January 13th 2019 for 109 

Antarctic prions). 110 

Playback procedure and recording of provoked calls

111 

During incubation, breeding petrels are silent in the colony, rarely calling spontaneously, 112 

seemingly to avoid predation (Mougeot and Bretagnolle 2000). However, it is possible to elicit vocal 113 

responses by broadcasting calls of same-gender conspecifics (Taoka et al. 1989, Bretagnolle & Lequette 114 

1990, Curé et al. 2011). Petrel calls consist of a repetition of distinct sections called “phrases”, 115 

themselves composed of indivisible elements called syllables (see Catchpole and Slater 2008 for 116 

definition, see Fig. 2a and Bretagnolle 1996 for sonograms). Signals to broadcast were graphically 117 

synthesized from spontaneous calls of nine bachelor male blue petrels and five bachelor male Antarctic 118 

prions recorded in the same colonies in 2013 and 2017 (i.e. series of phrases, themselves constituted of 119 

syllables). We used the signal processing software Avisoft–SASLab Pro v 5.2.11 (Specht 2017). Each 120 

synthesized call consisted in a two-phrase call of a single blue petrel or Antarctic prion male, separated 121 

by a silence of 200 ± 60 ms (5.5 ± 1.6 s and 3.43 ± 0.85 s, respectively; mean ± SD). To avoid 122 

pseudoreplication (McGregor et al. 1992), nine blue petrel call series and five Antarctic prion call series 123 

were played-back. 124 

At the beginning of the 2017 and 2018 breeding seasons, we checked each burrow of the colony to 125 

detect occupied burrows with the presence of an egg. We then choose distant burrows (i.e. separated by 126 

a minimum of five meters) for each species. Several recording sessions were performed on each male, 127 

with an interval of two to five day between sessions to avoid habituation. In total, we performed a mean 128 

of 3 ± 1 recording sessions on 31 blue petrel males and we recorded 172 calls (6 ± 4 calls per male; 129 

mean ± SD). We performed 4 ± 2 recording sessions on 24 Antarctic prion males and we recorded 224 130 

calls (5 ± 3 calls per male; mean ± SD). Playbacks were carried out at night during the period of maximal 131 

vocal activity of the colony (i.e. between 22:00 and 02:00, unpublished data) in dark nights (Mougeot 132 

and Bretagnolle 2000), and in calm weather (i.e. wind speed < 4 km/h and no rain) to limit background 133 

noises. 134 

(9)

The recording equipment was composed of an omnidirectional Sennheiser K6-ME62 microphone 135 

(frequency response: 20-20 000 Hz ± 2.5 dB) connected to a Marantz PMD 660 digital recorder 136 

(sampling frequency: 44.1 kHz, dynamic: 16 bits). The microphone was positioned on the ground, at the 137 

burrow entrance, and one randomly selected synthesized call was broadcast at a natural sound pressure 138 

level (± 67 dB, measured on 111 calls from 54 males with a sound level meter) using a TASCAM DR-139 

07MKII digital recorder (sampling frequency: 44.1 kHz, dynamic:16 bits). Calls emitted in reply by the 140 

burrow owner were recorded during the playback and two minutes after the end of the playback stimulus. 141 

If the focal individual did not reply, the playback was repeated every 30 seconds, up to three times. 142 

Morphometric measurements

143 

Soon after recording, the identity of the bird was checked by capturing the birds and reading the 144 

metal ring. The day after, birds were caught again in order to take a blood sample from the brachial vein 145 

(0.2 mL in Queen lysis buffer, Seutin et al. 1991) and the following seven morphometricmeasurements 146 

(some shown in Fig. 1, Cramp and Simmons 1977): bill length (CL), from edge of implantation of 147 

feathers to the bill tip; bill depth (BD), from angle of gonys to dorsal surface of hook; head length (HL), 148 

from supraoccital to front edge of bill; tarsus length (TL), from middle of mid-tarsal joint to distal end 149 

of tarso-metatarsus; wing chord length (WL), maximum flattened chord, from carpal joint to tip of 150 

longest primary; mass (MS); wing area (WA). 151 

Head and tarsus measurements (CL, BD, HL, and TL) were taken using a calliper with an 152 

accuracy of ± 0.1 mm. Wing length was measured with a stainless-steel rule with an accuracy of ± 1.0 153 

mm. To limit measurement biases, a single person (CG) took each measurement three times and 154 

calculated the mean. We also took standardized pictures of both sides of the right wing for each tested 155 

individual, using a digital camera (Olympus TG-610). Wing area was then measured by a single person 156 

(CG) by counting the number of calibrated pixels using ImageJ image analysis software version 1.52a 157 

(National Institute of Health, USA). Correlation scores between morphometric measurements and 158 

summary are given in Appendices I and II, respectively. 159 

We chose morphometric measurements likely to be indicators of body size and/or to have an 160 

effect on call production based on the literature, and/or may be important in the biology of the species. 161 

(10)

Head size has been used as an indicator of body size in swallows (Winkler and Allen 1996, Patel et al. 162 

2010) and nestling growth in blue tits (Plummer et al. 2013). Similarly, tarsus and wing lengths are good 163 

indicators of body size in passerines (Senar and Pascual 1997, Gosler et al. 1998), although it has not 164 

been documented if these indicators are suitable in seabirds. Bill is considered as the end of the vocal 165 

tract and its morphology (length and depth) may influence sound production. Bill is also the small 166 

petrels’ feeding means (Warham 1996), as they filter small crustaceans at the surface of the water. Wing 167 

area seems a relevant morphological trait because petrels are a soaring species and travel great distances 168 

at sea to feed (Cherel et al. 2002). 169 

Body mass is highly variable in petrels due to their feeding habits. Every ten days or so, mates 170 

alternate fasting incubation shifts in the burrow and foraging trips where they restore their energy 171 

reserves (Warham 1990). The longer they stay in the burrow incubating, the lighter they get. For 172 

instance, in blue petrels, breeders loose about 45 ± 6 g in 10 ± 2 days (mean ± SD) during the incubating 173 

shift (Chaurand and Weimerskirch 1994a, 1994b). Therefore, we did not include mass in the following 174 

statistical analyses. 175 

Birds were genetically sexed following methods for non-ratite birds (Fridolfsson and Ellegren 176 

1999). 177 

Acoustic analysis

178 

Calls recorded were first down sampled at 11.025 kHz to increase the precision of frequency 179 

measurements and high-pass filtered (Cutting frequency: 0.10 kHz, FFT filter) to remove the 180 

background noise. We described calls at a phrase level by measuring 11 acoustic parameters in the 181 

temporal and frequency domains (see Table 1 for abbreviations and description), on oscillograms and 182 

spectra of the signal-processing software Avisoft–SASLab Pro v 5.2.11, respectively (Specht 2017). 183 

Temporal variables were automatically extracted on the amplitude envelopes using the “Pulse Train 184 

Analysis” function, with a resolution of 0.09 ms (hysteresis: +30 dB; start/end threshold: -30 dB). 185 

Settings were not manipulated during measurements, except the threshold which was manually adjusted 186 

to detect all syllables. Mean fundamental frequencies and variables describing energy spectral 187 

distribution were automatically extracted on linear amplitude spectra with a resolution of 22 Hz (FFT 188 

(11)

length: 512, Blackman window) (Fig. 2). Automatic extractions are based on clear criteria preliminarily 189 

set, insuring objectivity in element demarcation and thus replicable measurements (Fischer et al. 2013). 190 

Syllables and phrases were counted on sonograms (Fig. 2). Correlation scores between acoustic-191 

parameter measures and summary are given in Appendices III and IV, respectively. 192 

Statistical Analysis

193 

We implemented all analysis results under the R software environment version 3.4.4 (R Core 194 

Team 2018) with the ade4 package (Dray and Dufour 2007). 195 

Relating acoustic parameters and morphological traits

196 

Body mass has been shown to be related to vocal performance in birds, i.e. heavy males have a 197 

higher calling activity than light males (e.g. Berg et al. 2005, Barnett and Briskie 2011, Yamada and 198 

Soma 2016). As previously mentioned, mass is highly variable in petrels and decreased during the 199 

fasting periods in the burrow (Chaurand and Weimerskirch 1994a, 1994b), and consequently between 200 

successive playback sessions. To avoid potential bias related to the high variation of mass between 201 

playback sessions, we considered only the first session of each individual and we omitted mass in our 202 

statistical analyses. 203 

We investigated covariance among morphometric measurements and acoustic parameters with 204 

a Co-Inertia Analysis (CIA), an ordination method designed to reveal the co-structures among two data 205 

tables (Dolédec and Chessel 1994, Dray et al. 2003). A summary table of acoustic and morphometric 206 

data are given in Appendices I and II, respectively. We first summarized the morphometric measurement 207 

table with the two first axes of a normed Principal Component Analysis (PCA1), which explained 208 

together 55.6 % for blue petrel (32.2 and 23.3 % for PC11 and PC12, respectively) and 60.6% of total 209 

inertia for Antarctic prion (34.6 and 26.0 % for PC11 and PC12, respectively). We ordinated the acoustic 210 

parameters table with a Between-Class Analysis (PCA2), a PCA which accounts for the clustering of 211 

object (here phrases) by a grouping factor (here individuals), similarly as in a redundancy analysis 212 

(Dolédec and Chessel 1987). We retained the two first axes of the PCA2 in both species (28.2% and 213 

24.7% of variance explained by PC21 and PC22, respectively, in blue petrel; 34.6% and 24.9% in 214 

Antarctic prion). We built the CIA as an ordination of PCA1 and PCA2. Overall similarity between the 215 

(12)

two ordination tables was assessed by the RV coefficient, a correlation coefficient between two sets of 216 

variables recorded from the same sample (0: no similarity, 1: equal tables). 217 

Individual signature

218 

We assessed whether males can be discriminated based on their calls using a Between-Class 219 

Analysis (BCA, Dolédec and Chessel 1987). Then, we compared the intra and inter-individual variations 220 

for each acoustic parameter to highlight the most discriminant parameters. As a single recording session 221 

is not sufficient to highlight a vocal signature (Průchová et al. 2017), we considered only males that 222 

have been recorded at least three times to run the BCA. In total, we had 127 blue petrel calls from 21 223 

males (6 ± 4 calls per male), and 224 Antarctic prion calls from 16 males (5 ± 3 calls per male). 224 

We calculated the Potential of Individuality Coding (PIC) of each call acoustic parameter. PIC 225 

is defined as the ratio between intra-individual variation (CVw) and inter-individual variation (CVb) of 226 

a given acoustic parameter. For each parameter and each individual, we calculated the coefficient of 227 

intra-individual variation CVw corrected for small samples (Scherrer 1984): CV 100 1 228 

with X the mean of the parameter measured, and n is the number of measures. We calculated the inter-229 

individual variation coefficient as CV 100 where std and X arethe standard deviation and the 230 

mean calculated with all measures of a given parameters, respectively, from all individuals. PIC values 231 

have been calculated as PIC . If PIC>1, intra-individual variability is smaller than inter-individual 232 

variability for a given parameter, suggesting that this parameter may potentially carry individual 233 

information. PIC values are considered high (i.e. likely to carry individual information), when they are 234 

superior to 2 (Robisson et al. 1993, Lengagne et al. 1997). 235 

Results

236 

Relation between acoustic parameters and morphological traits

237 

In blue petrels, the RV coefficient of the CIA between morphometric measurements and acoustic 238 

parameters is 0.34. The first two axes of the CIA explained 51.0% and 22.8% of total variance, 239 

respectively. The first CIA axis shows the duration and the syllable number of the phrase are mainly 240 

related to head and bill length (HL, CL). More precisely, males with a small head (HL) and a short bill 241 

(13)

(CL) produce short calls with few syllables. The second CIA axis shows rhythm and syllable rate are 242 

related to both wing measurements (WL, WA). More precisely, males with short, large wings produce 243 

slow (i.e. low phrase rate) calls with long syllables (i.e. high rhythm) and short interphrase silences. 244 

Frequency parameters are weakly related to the chosen morphometric measurements (Fig. 3). 245 

In Antarctic prions, the RV coefficient of the CIA between morphometric measurements and 246 

acoustic parameters is 0.40. The first two axes of the CIA explained 52.9% and 25.6% of total variance, 247 

respectively. The first CIA axis of shows head-bill length (HL) and wing area (WA) are related to energy 248 

quartiles. More precisely, males with a short head (HL) and slim wings (WA) produce high-pitched 249 

calls. The second CIA axis shows temporal parameters (duration, interphrase silences and phrase rate) 250 

are mainly related to wing length (WL) and bill length (CL). In other words, males with long wings and 251 

a short bill produce short, fast (i.e. high phrase rate, and short interphrase silences) calls (Fig. 4). 252 

Tables of CIA scores for both species are given in Appendix V. 253 

Individual signature

254 

We focused here on the individual information carried in males’ calls. We did not investigate 255 

the sexual signature or species signature, both already described in the literature (Bretagnolle 1996, 256 

Bretagnolle and Genevois 1997). 257 

The total inertia explained by the differences between males in blue petrels and Antarctic prions 258 

was 74% and 65%, respectively, showing that the chosen acoustic parameters allow a significant vocal 259 

discrimination of individuals (p <10-5 in both species). Composite plots of BCA analyses are given in 260 

Appendix VI. 261 

The PIC values of each of the 11 acoustic parameters used to describe blue petrel and Antarctic 262 

prion calls are presented in Fig. 4. All values in both species are higher than one, suggesting that all 263 

acoustic parameters considered here may bear the vocal signature (Robisson et al. 1993). Eight out of 264 

11 parameters are higher than two, suggesting they potentially code more than others for individual 265 

information (Robisson et al. 1993). In both species, acoustic parameters with highest PIC values are 266 

upper energy quartiles in spectral domain, duration, and phrase rate in time domain (Table 2). BCA and 267 

PIC methods gave similar results. 268 

(14)

Discussion

269 

We aimed to investigate the informative content of calls in two species of burrowing petrels, the 270 

blue petrel and the Antarctic prion. Results show acoustic parameters carry information about the caller 271 

individuality and morphological traits, for males of both species. 272 

Relationship between acoustic parameters and morphological traits

273 

In both species, results obtained by CIA show a clear relationship between acoustic parameters 274 

of males’ calls and some morphological traits which may constraint call productions (bill morphology, 275 

body size indicators), and/or may be relevant considering the feeding ecology (bill morphology, wing 276 

morphology) of these species. 277 

Specifically, frequency parameters (i.e. fundamental frequency and energy quartiles) are related 278 

to bill morphology, especially head-bill length, in both species even though the relationship is weak in 279 

blue petrels. Males with a short head and bill produce low-pitched calls in blue petrels whereas they 280 

produce high-pitched calls in Antarctic prions. Relationship between bill morphology and frequency is 281 

unclear in birds and both opposite patterns have been observed in many species (e.g. Palacios and Tubaro 282 

2000, Laiolo and Rolando 2003, Christensen et al. 2006). Other studies have found no relationships 283 

between frequencies and bill morphology (e.g. Garcia and Tubaro 2018). 284 

Head-bill length is also a body size indicators, suggesting frequencies are related to body size. 285 

This frequency-body size relationship has been illustrated in many species at the light of the source-286 

filter theory (e.g. Taylor & Reby 2010). This theory states that vocal signals result from a two-stage 287 

production: the source signal is generated by membrane vibrations in the vocal organ (larynx in 288 

mammals or syrinx in birds), and subsequently filtered by the vocal tract and the mouth/nostrils (Fant 289 

1960). Consequently, the anatomy of the “source”, particularly the length and mass of vibrating 290 

membranes, shapes the fundamental frequency. Muscular interactions changes the airflow in the 291 

“source” and thus influence temporal parameters such as rhythm, duration and amplitude. The “filter” 292 

influences the formants, i.e. the resonant frequencies (Titze 1994, Taylor and Reby 2010). Although this 293 

theory is valid in mammals, the relationships between frequencies and body size remains unclear in 294 

(15)

birds (Galeotti et al. 1997, Mager et al. 2007, Favaro et al. 2017, Kriesell et al. 2018 but see Patel et al.

295 

2010). 296 

Our results show some temporal parameters are related to body size in both species, suggesting 297 

big males produce more challenging songs than small males. More precisely, in blue petrels, males with 298 

a long head, wings and tarsus produce long, fast calls with many syllables. In Antarctic prions, males 299 

with long wings produce short, fast calls. In many species, females are more attracted by males 300 

performing physically challenging songs/calls (e.g. high rate, long duration), likely because they reflect 301 

male characteristics linked to qualities such as endurance (Ballentine et al. 2004), survival (Byers et al. 302 

2016), age, and size (Ballentine 2009). 303 

Results show a strong relationship between acoustic parameters and wing morphology, 304 

especially wing length in both species, and wing area even though this relationship is weaker in blue 305 

petrels. We thus suggest that for soaring seabirds, which travel great distances at sea to feed, wing 306 

morphology is a relevant parameter to be considered (Cherel et al. 2002). We can hypothesize that cues 307 

that may inform females on this male characteristic might be important as wing morphology is correlated 308 

with foraging behaviour (Hertel and Ballance 1999) and reproductive costs (Mauck and Grubb 1995). 309 

In other Procellariidae, a reduction of parents’ flying abilities (e.g. clipping feather tips, removing flight 310 

feathers, adding extra weight) affects the incubation routine and results in a decrease of their body 311 

condition and/or a deterioration of chick condition (Saether et al. 1993, Weimerskirch et al. 1995, 312 

Navarro and Gonzáles-Solís 2007). In many species, females are more attracted by males performing 313 

physically challenging songs/calls (e.g. high rate, long duration), likely because they reflect male 314 

characteristics linked to qualities such as endurance (Ballentine et al. 2004), survival (Byers et al. 2016), 315 

age, and size (Ballentine 2009). 316 

Blue petrels and Antarctic prions form lifelong monogamous bonds. Pair bonding takes up to 317 

two years and partners equally share parental care during the incubation period (Chaurand and 318 

Weimerskirch 1994a, Warham 1990, Warham 1996). Sexual maturity is reached tardily (around seven 319 

years), they show low fecundity (one single egg per year without a replacement clutch), and extra-pair 320 

paternity is extremely rare (Jouventin and Mougin 1981, Warham 1996). Consequently, choosing the 321 

“wrong” mate affects fitness more than in species that change mates at each breeding. Females should 322 

(16)

rely on attractive and reliable cues to evaluate the quality of a potential partner (Darwin 1871, Smith 323 

1991). As in many bird species, males’ calls are likely to be sexual signals that attract and/or stimulate 324 

a potential mate (Bretagnolle 1990). Especially as they are highly vocal, despite the predation cost, and 325 

as living in burrows prevents visual communication on long-range. We hypothesize, thus, that the 326 

information contained in male calls about the caller body size may have a significant role in sexual 327 

selection.In blue petrels, bigger males at the beginning of the breeding season have a higher breeding 328 

success (Chastel et al. 1995). Moreover, calls contain information about wing morphology, which may 329 

be linked to reproductive success in these soaring birds (Saether et al. 1993, Weimerskirch et al. 1995, 330 

Navarro and Gonzáles-Solís 2007). 331 

Individual signature

332 

Our results suggest that individuality in calls of males of both species is coded by several 333 

parameters in spectral (energy quartiles) and temporal domain (phrase rate and duration). Individual 334 

identity coded by frequency parameters has been shown in numerous species (e.g. Robisson et al. 1993, 335 

Jouventin et al. 1999, Charrier et al. 2001, Charrier et al. 2004, Favaro et al. 2017). Among these 336 

parameters, the fundamental frequency often codes for individual signature in many birds, likely because 337 

it is linked to anatomical structure of the vocal tract (Fletcher and Tarnopolsky 1999). Nevertheless, our 338 

results show that fundamental frequency has the lowest PIC value and that its correlation with 339 

morphological traits is weak. This is consistent with previous studies on blue petrels (Genevois and 340 

Bretagnolle 1994), although a negative correlation between mass and fundamental frequency has been 341 

highlighted in the snow petrel (Guillotin and Jouventin 1980). 342 

In many seabirds, vocal identity is crucial in social interactions, such as mate reunion or kin 343 

recognition (e.g. Spheniscidae: Robisson et al. 1993, Jouventin et al. 1999, Aubin and Jouventin 2002, 344 

Procellariidae: Barbraud et al. 2000, Curé et al. 2011, Stercorariidae: Charrier et al. 2001, Laridae 345 

Mathevon et al. 2003). However, in burrowing petrels, finding relatives and nest sites seems not based 346 

on vocal signals. As they return from their foraging trip at sea, petrels rely on olfactory signals to find 347 

their burrow (Bonadonna et al. 2001, Bonadonna et al. 2004) and take the place of their mate incubating 348 

in the burrow, without emitting any call (FB and CG personal observation). Therefore, vocal signature 349 

(17)

does not seem to be involved in mate reunion. Vocal signatures do not systematically imply recognition 350 

(Townsend et al. 2010). So far, there is no clear evidence that burrowing petrels vocally recognize 351 

conspecifics as shown in other seabird families (e.g. Spheniscidae: Robisson et al. 1993, Jouventin et al. 352 

1999, Aubin and Jouventin 2002, Procellariidae: Barbraud et al. 2000, Curé et al. 2011, Stercorariidae: 353 

Charrier et al. 2001, Laridae: Mathevon et al. 2003). Nonetheless, vocal signature could be used in 354 

neighbour/stranger discrimination processes. Indeed, blue petrel and Antarctic prion males call from 355 

their burrow in crowded colonies (respectively up to 0.7 and 1.4 burrows/m² according to Croxall and 356 

Prince 1980). The significant costs of calling behaviour (Mougeot and Bretagnolle 2000) suggests that 357 

males should call only when necessary, for instance in the presence of a stranger male not belonging to 358 

the neighbouring community, to avoid higher predation risks and useless energy expenditures. This 359 

“dear-enemy phenomenon” (Fisher 1954) has been documented in many territorial songbirds where 360 

males are less aggressive with neighbours than with strangers when defending their breeding territory 361 

(Temeles 1994). One hypothesis is that territory owners adjust their aggressive behaviour according to 362 

the familiarity and/or threat degrees and thus minimizes fighting costs (Temeles 1994). 363 

Knowing which information is passed through vocal signals is crucial to understanding many 364 

social behaviours, such as female mate choice or male-male competition. Here, we investigated the 365 

informative content of males’ calls in blue petrels and Antarctic prions, two under-studied species that 366 

use vocal signals in their social interactions. Results highlight similarities on informative content of 367 

males’ calls between the two study species, such as the relationship between body size - element rates, 368 

and individual-identity coding strategy. However, results also highlight dissimilarities. Although these 369 

species are phylogenetically and ecologically close (Warham 1990, Warham 1996), they exhibit calls 370 

that differ by their structure, with two different strategies of body-size vocal signalling. In Antarctic 371 

prions, frequencies bear information on the caller body size whereas this information is only coded by 372 

temporal parameters in blue petrels. These results are consistent with previous studies on blue petrels 373 

(Genevois and Bretagnolle 1994) and snow petrels (Barbraud et al. 2000), suggesting the existence of 374 

different coding strategies in Procellariidae. Syntactic differences between blue petrel and Antarctic 375 

prions calls (Bretagnolle 1996) or differences of the intensity of predation pressure (Mougeot et al. 1998) 376 

might be possible explanations for these different coding strategies. 377 

(18)

Globally, our results showing that several call parameters correlate with male body size, suggest 378 

that they might be potential indicators of male quality for females. However, so far, there is no evidence 379 

showing that females are more attracted to males with particular morphological traits. Several call 380 

parameters also bear an individual signature. Nevertheless, individual recognition has not been explored 381 

in these species. Our study is the first step to understand the importance of vocal signals in burrowing 382 

petrels’ social lives. The direction for future studies will be to assess how the informative content of 383 

males' calls influence conspecific behaviours, for instance, by using playback experiments to test the 384 

existence of individual vocal recognition and whether call characteristics could be good predictors of 385 

female preference. 386 

387 

Acknowledgments – We thank all people who kindly helped us in the field, especially Jean-Yves Barnagaud,

388 

Guilhem Battistella, Matthieu Bergès, Samuel Perret and the French polar institute IPEV which provided logistic

389 

support in the field. We are very grateful to Jean-Yves Barnagaud and Pierre Legendre for their sound advices and

390 

guidance on statistical analyses. We are also grateful to Jessica Graham for her comments and English

391 

improvement of the paper.

392 

Conflict of interest – The authors declare no conflicts of interest associated with this study.

393 

Funding – This work was supported by the French Polar Institute Paul Emile Victor (IPEV, program no. 354).

394 

Permits – This study was performed according to guidelines established by both IPEV and CNRS for the Ethical

395 

Treatment of Animals. All experiments comply with the current laws of the country where they were performed.

396  397 

References

398 

Aubin, T. and Jouventin, P. 2002. How to vocally identify kin in a crowd: the penguin model. - Adv. 399 

Study Behav. 31: 243–277. 400 

Aubin, T., Mathevon, N., Staszewski, V. and Boulinier, T. 2007. Acoustic communication in the 401 

kittiwake Rissa tridactyla: potential cues for sexual and individual signatures in long calls. - 402 

Polar Biol. 30: 1027–1033. 403 

Ballentine, B. 2009. The ability to perform physically challenging songs predicts age and size in male 404 

swamp sparrows, Melospiza georgiana. - Anim. Behav. 77: 973–978. 405 

(19)

Ballentine, B., Hyman, J. and Nowicki, S. 2004. Vocal performance influences female response to 406 

male bird song: an experimental test. - Behav. Ecol. 15: 163–168. 407 

Barbraud, C., Mariani, A. and Jouventin, P. 2000. Variation in call properties of the snow petrel, 408 

Pagodroma nivea, in relation to sex and body size. - Aust. J. Zool. 48: 421–430.

409 

Barnett, C. A. and Briskie, J. V. 2011. Strategic regulation of body mass and singing behaviour in 410 

New Zealand robins. - Ethology 117: 28–36. 411 

Beecher, M. 1989. Signalling system for individual recognition: an information theory approach. - 412 

Anim. Behav. 38: 248–261. 413 

Berg, M. L., Beintema, N. H., Welbergen, J. A. and Komdeur, J. 2005. Singing as a handicap: the 414 

effects of food availability and weather on song output in the Australian reed warbler 415 

Acrocephalus australis. - J. Avian Biol. 36: 102–109.

416 

Bergès, M., Choquet, R. and Bonadonna, F. 2019. Impact of long-term behavioural studies in the wild: 417 

the blue petrel, Halobaena caerulea, case at Kerguelen. - Anim. Behav. 151: 53–65. 418 

Bonadonna, F., and Nevitt, G. A. 2004. Partnerspecific odour recognition in an Antarctic seabird. -419 

Science 306: 835. 420 

Bonadonna, F., and Sanz-Aguilar, A. 2012. Kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance in wild birds: 421 

the first evidence for individual kin-related odour recognition. - Animal Behav. 84: 509-513. 422 

Bonadonna, F., Spaggiari, J. and Weimerskirch, H. 2001. Could osmotaxis explain the ability of blue 423 

petrels to return to their burrows at night? - J. Exp. Biol. 204: 1485–1489. 424 

Bonadonna, F., Villafane, M., Bajzak, C. and Jouventin, P. 2004. Recognition of burrow’s olfactory 425 

signature in blue petrels, Halobaena caerulea: an efficient discrimination mechanism in the dark. 426 

- Anim. Behav. 67: 893–898. 427 

Bradbury, J. W. and Vehrencamp, S. L. 1998. Principles of animal communication. 428 

Bretagnolle, V. 1990. Behavioural affinities of the blue petrel Halobaena caerulea. - Ibis (Lond. 429 

1859). 132: 102–105. 430 

Bretagnolle, V. 1996. Acoustic communication in a group of nonpasserine birds, the petrels. - In: 431 

Kroodsma, D. E. and Miller, E. H. (eds), Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in 432 

Birds. Cornell Un. pp. 160–177. 433 

(20)

Bretagnolle, V. and Lequette, B. 1990. Structural Variation in the Call of the Cory’s Shearwater 434 

(Calonectris diomedea, Aves, Procellariidae). - Ethology 85: 313–323. 435 

Bretagnolle, V. and Genevois, F. 1997. Geographic variation in the call of the blue petrel: effects of 436 

sex and geographical scale. - Condor 99: 985–989. 437 

Bretagnolle, V., Genevois, F. and Mougeot, F. 1998. Intra- and intersexual functions in the call of a 438 

non-passerine bird. - Behaviour 135: 1161–1184. 439 

Briefer, E. F. 2018. Vocal contagion of emotions in non-human animals. - Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 440 

285: 20172783. 441 

Brooke, M. L. 1978. Sexual differences in the voice and individual vocal recognition in the Manx 442 

shearwater (Puffinus puffinus). - Anim. Behav. 26: 622–629. 443 

Brooke, M. 2004. Albatrosses and petrels across the world. 444 

Byers, B. E., Akresh, M. E. and King, D. I. 2016. Song and male quality in prairie warblers. - 445 

Ethology 122: 660–670. 446 

Candolin, U. and Voigt, H. R. 2001. Correlation between male size and territory quality: consequence 447 

of male competition or predation susceptibility? - Oikos 95: 225–230. 448 

Cardoso, G. C. 2012. Paradoxical calls: the opposite signaling role of sound frequency across bird 449 

species. - Behav. Ecol. 23: 237–241. 450 

Catchpole, C. K. and Slater, P. J. B. 2008. Bird song: biological themes and variations. 451 

Charrier, I., Jouventin, P., Mathevon, N. and Aubin, T. 2001. Individual identity coding depends on 452 

call type in the south polar skua Catharacta maccormicki. - Polar Biol. 24: 378–382. 453 

Charrier, I., Bloomfield, L. L. and Sturdy, C. B. 2004. Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. I: 454 

The chick-a-dee call of the black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). - Can. J. Zool. 82: 455 

769–779. 456 

Chastel, O., Weimerskirch, H. and Jouventin, P. 1995. Body condition and seabird reproductive 457 

performance: a study of three petrel species. - Ecology 76: 2240–2246. 458 

Chaurand, T. and Weimerskirch, H. 1994a. Incubation routine, body mass regulation and egg neglect 459 

in the blue petrel Halobaena caerulea. - Ibis (Lond. 1859). 136: 285–290. 460 

Chaurand, T. and Weimerskirch, H. 1994b. The regular alternation of short and long foraging trips in 461 

(21)

the blue petrel Halobaena caerulea: a previously undescribed strategy of food provisioning in a 462 

pelagic seabird. - J. Anim. Ecol. 63: 275. 463 

Cherel, Y., Bocher, P., Trouvé, C. and Weimerskirch, H. 2002. Diet and feeding ecology of blue 464 

petrels Halobaena caerulea at Iles Kerguelen, Southern Indian Ocean. - Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 465 

228: 283–299. 466 

Christensen, R., Kleindorfer, S. and Robertson, J. 2006. Song is a reliable signal of bill morphology in 467 

Darwin’s small tree finch Camarhynchus parvulus, and vocal performance predicts male pairing 468 

success. - J. Avian Biol. 37: 617–624. 469 

Cramp, S. and Simmons, K. E. L. 1977. The birds of the Western Palearctic. Vol. 1. Ostrich to ducks. - 470 

In: Handbook of the Birds of Europe. Oxford University Press, pp. 722. 471 

Croxall, J. P. and Prince, P. A. 1980. Food, feeding ecology and ecological segregation. - 472 

Environment: 103–131. 473 

Curé, C., Aubin, T. and Mathevon, N. 2011. Sex discrimination and mate recognition by voice in the 474 

yelkouan shearwater Puffinus yelkouan. - Bioacoustics 20: 235–250. 475 

Dale, Lank and Reeve 2001. Signaling individual identity versus quality: a model and case studies 476 

with ruffs, queleas, and house finches. - Am. Nat. 158: 75. 477 

Darwin, C. 1871. The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex (Murray, Ed.). 478 

Dolédec, S. and Chessel, D. 1994. Co-inertia analysis: an alternative method for studying species-479 

environment relationships. - Freshw. Biol. 31: 277–294. 480 

Dolédec, S., and D. C. 1987. Rythmes saisonniers et composantes stationnelles en milieu aquatique I- 481 

Description d’un plan d’observations complet par projection de variables. - Acta Oecologica. 482 

Oecologia Gen. 8: 403–426. 483 

Dray, S. and Dufour, A.-B. 2015. The ade4 package: implementing the duality diagram for ecologists. 484 

- J. Stat. Softw 22: 1-20. 485 

Dray, S., Chessel, D. and Thioulouse, J. 2003. Co-inertia analysis and the linking of ecological data 486 

tables. - Ecology 84: 3078–3089. 487 

Falls, J. B. 1982. Individual recognition by sounds in birds. - In: Kroodsma, D. E. and Miller, E. H. 488 

(eds), Acoustic Communication in Birds. Academic Press, pp. 237–278. 489 

(22)

Fant, G. 1960. Acoustic theory of speech production. 490 

Favaro, L., Gamba, M., Gili, C. and Pessani, D. 2017. Acoustic correlates of body size and individual 491 

identity in banded penguins. - PLoS One 12: e0170001. 492 

Fisher, J. 1954. Evolution and bird sociality. - Evol. as a Process: 71–83. 493 

Fletcher, N. H. and Tarnopolsky, A. 1999. Acoustics of the avian vocal tract. - J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 494 

105: 35–49. 495 

Fridolfsson, A.-K. and Ellegren, H. 1999. A simple and universal method for molecular sexing of non-496 

ratite birds. - J. Avian Biol. 30: 116–121. 497 

Funghi, C., Leitão, A. V., Ferreira, A. C., Mota, P. G. and Cardoso, G. C. 2015. Social dominance in a 498 

gregarious bird is related to body size but not to standard personality assays. - Ethology 121: 84– 499 

93. 500 

Galeotti, P., Saino, N., Sacchi, R. and Møller, A. P. 1997. Song correlates with social context, 501 

testosterone and body condition in male barn swallows. - Anim. Behav. 53: 687–700. 502 

García, N. C. and Tubaro, P. L. 2018. Dissecting the roles of body size and beak morphology in song 503 

evolution in the “blue” cardinalids (Passeriformes: Cardinalidae). - Auk 135: 262–275. 504 

Genevois, F. and Bretagnolle, V. 1994. Male blue petrels reveal their body mass when calling. - Ethol. 505 

Ecol. Evol. 6: 377–383. 506 

Gil, D. and Gahr, M. 2002. The honesty of bird song: multiple constraints for multiple traits. - Trends 507 

Ecol. Evol. 17: 133–141. 508 

Gosler, A. G., Greenwood, J. J. D., Baker, J. K. and Davidson, N. C. 1998. The field determination of 509 

body size and condition in passerines: a report to the British Ringing Committee. - Bird Study 510 

45: 92–103. 511 

Guillotin, M. and Jouventin, P. 1980. Le pétrel des neiges à Pointe Géologie. - Gerfaut 70: 51–72. 512 

Hertel, F. and Ballance, L. T. 1999. Wing ecomorphology of seabirds from Johnston Atoll. - Condor 513 

101: 549–556. 514 

Jouventin, P. and Mougin, J.-L. 1981. Les strategies adaptatives des oiseaux de mer. - Rev. Ecol. 515 

(Terre vie) 35: 217–249. 516 

Jouventin, P., Aubin, T. and Lengagne, T. 1999. Finding a parent in a king penguin colony: the 517 

(23)

acoustic system of individual recognition. - Anim. Behav. 57: 1175–1183. 518 

Kriesell, H. J., Aubin, T., Planas-Bielsa, V., Benoiste, M., Bonadonna, F., Gachot-Neveu, H., Le 519 

Maho, Y., Schull, Q., Vallas, B., Zahn, S. and Le Bohec, C. 2018. Sex identification in king 520 

penguins Aptenodytes patagonicus through morphological and acoustic cues. - Ibis (Lond. 1859). 521 

160: 755–768. 522 

Kroodsma, D. E. and Byers, B. E. 1991. The function(s) of bird song. - Am. Zool. 31: 318–328. 523 

Laiolo, P. and Rolando, A. 2003. Comparative analysis of the rattle calls in Corvus and Nucifraga: the 524 

effect of body size, bill size and phylogeny. - Condor 105: 139-144. 525 

Lambrechts, M. M. and Dhondt, A. A. 1995. Individual voice discrimination in birds. - In: Powers, D. 526 

M. (ed), Current Ornithology. Springer US, pp. 115–139. 527 

Leclaire, S., Strandh, M., Mardon, J., Westerdahl, H. and Bonadonna, F. 2017. Odour-based 528 

discrimination of MHC-similarity in birds. - Proc. R. Soc. B 284: 2016-2466. 529 

Lengagne, T., Lauga, J. and Jouventin, P. 1997. A method of independent time and frequency 530 

decomposition of bioacoustic signals: inter-individual recognition in four species of penguins. - 531 

Comptes Rendus l’Académie des Sci. - Ser. III - Sci. la Vie 320: 885–891. 532 

Mager, J. N., Walcott, C. and Piper, W. H. 2007. Male common loons, Gavia immer, communicate 533 

body mass and condition through dominant frequencies of territorial yodels. - Anim. Behav. 73: 534 

683–690. 535 

Magrath, R. D., Haff, T. M., Fallow, P. M. and Radford, A. N. 2015. Eavesdropping on heterospecific 536 

alarm calls: From mechanisms to consequences. - Biol. Rev. 90: 560–586. 537 

Martín-Vivaldi, M., Palomino, J. J. and Soler, M. 1998. Song structure in the hoopoe (Upupa epops) - 538 

strophe length reflects male condition. - J. fur Ornithol. 139: 287–296. 539 

Martín-Vivaldi, M., Palomino, J. J. and Soler, M. 2000. Attraction of hoopoe Upupa epops females 540 

and males by means of song playback in the field: influence of strophe length. - J. Avian Biol. 541 

31: 351–359. 542 

Martín-Vivaldi, M., Palomino, J. J. and Soler, M. 2004. Strophe length in spontaneous songs predicts 543 

male response to playback in the hoopoe Upupa epops. - Ethology 110: 351–362. 544 

Mathevon, N., Charrier, I. and Jouventin, P. 2003. Potential for individual recognition in acoustic 545 

(24)

signals: A comparative study of two gulls with different nesting patterns. - Comptes Rendus - 546 

Biol. 326: 329–337. 547 

Mauck, R. A. and Grubb, T. C. 1995. Petrel parents shunt all experimentally increased reproductive 548 

costs to their offspring. - Anim. Behav. 49: 999–1008. 549 

McGregor, P. K. 1992. Quantifying responses to playback: one, many, or composite multivariate 550 

measures? - In: Playback and Studies of Animal Communication. Springer US, pp. 79–96. 551 

Morton, E. S. 1977. On the occurrence and significance of Motivation-Structural Rules in some bird 552 

and mammal sounds. - Am. Nat. 111: 855–869. 553 

Mougeot, F. and Bretagnolle, V. 2000. Predation as a cost of sexual communication in nocturnal 554 

seabirds: an experimental approach using acoustic signals. - Anim. Behav. 60: 647–656. 555 

Mougeot, F., Genevois, F. and Bretagnolle, V. 1998. Predation on burrowing petrels by the brown 556 

skua (Catharacta skua lönnbergi) at Mayes Island, Kerguelen. - J. Zool. 244: 429–438. 557 

Navarro, J. and González-Solís, J. 2007. Experimental increase of flying costs in a pelagic seabird: 558 

effects on foraging strategies, nutritional state and chick condition. - Oecologia 151: 150–160. 559 

Nowicki, S. and Searcy, W. A. 2004. Song function and the evolution of female preferences: why 560 

birds sing, why brains matter. - Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1016: 704–723. 561 

Palacios, M. G. and Tubaro, P. L. 2000. Does beak size affect acoustic frequencies in woodcreepers? - 562 

Condor 102: 553-560. 563 

Patel, R., Mulder, R. A. and Cardoso, G. C. 2010. What makes vocalisation frequency an unreliable 564 

signal of body size in birds? A study on black swans. - Ethology 116: 554–563. 565 

Plummer, K. E., Bearhop, S., Leech, D. I., Chamberlain, D. E. and Blount, J. D. 2013. Winter food 566 

provisioning reduces future breeding performance in a wild bird. - Sci. Rep. in press. 567 

Podos, J. 2001. Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal structure in Darwin’s finches. - 568 

Nature 409: 185–188. 569 

Pollard, K. A. and Blumstein, D. T. 2011. Social group size predicts the evolution of individuality. - 570 

Curr. Biol. 21: 413–417. 571 

Průchová, A., Jaška, P. and Linhart, P. 2017. Cues to individual identity in songs of songbirds: testing 572 

general song characteristics in chiffchaffs Phylloscopus collybita. - J. Ornithol. 158: 911–924. 573 

(25)

Robisson, P., Aubin, T. and Bremond, J. 1993. Individuality in the voice of the emperor penguin 574 

Aptenodytes forsteri: adaptation to a noisy environment. - Ethology 94: 279–290.

575 

Rowe, L. and Houlde, D. 1996. The lek paradox and the capture of genetic variance. - Proc. Biol. Sci. 576 

263: 1415–1421. 577 

Sæther, B. E., Andersen, R. and Pedersen, H. C. 1993. Regulation of parental effort in a long-lived 578 

seabird an experimental manipulation of the cost of reproduction in the Antarctic petrel, 579 

Thalassoica antarctica. - Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 33: 147–150.

580 

Salton, M., Saraux, C., Dann, P. and Chiaradia, A. 2015. Carry-over body mass effect from winter to 581 

breeding in a resident seabird, the little penguin. - R. Soc. Open Sci. 2: 140390–140390. 582 

Scherrer, B. 1984. Biostatistique. - Gaetan Morin Editeur. 583 

Searcy, W. A. and Nowicki, S. 2005. The evolution of animal communication: reliability and 584 

deception in signaling systems. 585 

Senar, J. C. and Pascual, J. 1997. Keel and tarsus length may provide a good predictor of avian body 586 

size. - Ardea 85: 269–274. 587 

Seutin, G., White, B. N. and Boag, P. T. 1991. Preservation of avian blood and tissue samples for 588 

DNA analyses. - Can. J. Zool. 69: 82–90. 589 

Smith, J. M. 1991. Theories of sexual selection. - Trends Ecol. Evol. 6: 146–151. 590 

Taylor, A. M. and Reby, D. 2010. The contribution of source-filter theory to mammal vocal 591 

communication research. - J. Zool. 280: 221–236. 592 

Temeles, E. J. 1994. The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when are they “dear enemies”? - 593 

Anim. Behav. 47: 339–350. 594 

Tibbetts, E. A. and Dale, J. 2007. Individual recognition: it is good to be different. - Trends Ecol. 595 

Evol. 22: 529–537. 596 

Titze, I. R., & Martin, D. W. (1998). Principles of voice production. 597 

Townsend, S. W., Hollén, L. I. and Manser, M. B. 2010. Meerkat close calls encode group-specific 598 

signatures, but receivers fail to discriminate. - Anim. Behav. 80: 133–138. 599 

Warham, J. 1990. The petrels: their ecology and breeding systems (A Press, Ed.). 600 

Warham, J. 1996. The behaviour, population biology and physiology of the petrels. - In: Elsevier, in 601 

(26)

press. 602 

Weimerskirch, H., Chastel, O. and Ackermann, L. 1995. Adjustment of parental effort to manipulated 603 

foraging ability in a pelagic seabird, the thin-billed prion Pachyptila belcheri. - Behav. Ecol. 604 

Sociobiol. 36: 11–16. 605 

Winkler, D. W. and Allen, P. E. 1996. The seasonal decline in tree swallow clutch size: physiological 606 

constraint or strategic adjustment? - Ecology 77: 922–932. 607 

Wittenberger, J. F. and Hunt, G. L. 1985. The adaptive significance of coloniality in birds. - In: Avian 608 

Biology. Academic P. xx, pp. 1–78. 609 

Yamada, K. and Soma, M. 2016. Diet and birdsong: short-term nutritional enrichment improves songs 610 

of adult Bengalese finch males. - J. Avian Biol. 47: 865–870. 611 

(27)

Table 1. Abbreviations and descriptions for the acoustic parameters measured on each phrase of blue petrel and 613 

Antarctic prion provoked calls.

614 

Acoustic parameters Description

NbPh Number of phrases in a call NbSy Number of syllables in a phrase

Temporal analysis

Duration Phrase duration (s)

Interphrase Duration of silence between two phrases (s)

Rhythm Phrase rhythm (i.e. ratio between syllable and silence duration for each phrase) SyllRate Syllable rate (i.e. number of syllables per second for each phrase)

PhRate Phrase rate (i.e. number of phrases per second for each call)

Frequency analysis

q25 Upper quartile (frequency in Hz at the upper limit of the 25% of phrase energy) q50 Medium quartile (frequency in Hz at the upper limit of the 50% of phrase energy) q75 Lower quartile (frequency in Hz at the upper limit of the 75% of phrase energy)

F0 Mean fundamental frequency of the phrase (Hz)

(28)

Table 2. Potential of Individual Coding (PIC) for the acoustic parameters measured on blue petrel (top) and 616 

Antarctic prion (bottom) calls. Abbreviations of acoustic parameters as in Table 1. PIC values above 2.0 are in

617 

bold.

618 

Acoustic features PIC

Blue petrel Antarctic prion

NbPh 2.24 2.52 NbSy 2.05 2.24 Duration 2.44 2.27 Interphrase 2.17 2.10 Rhythm 1.63 1.65 SyllRate 1.79 2.07 PhRate 2.32 3.97 q25 2.01 1.95 q50 2.33 2.76 q75 3.15 3.22 F0 1.56 1.28 619 

(29)

Figure captions

620 

Figure 1. Tarsus and head measurements from Cramp and Simmons (1977), taken on blue petrels and Antarctic prions: head

621 

length (HL), bill depth (BD), bill length (CL) and tarsus length (TL).

622 

Figure 2. (a) Sonogram (top) and oscillogram (bottom) of a male blue petrel call constituted of 2 phrases. (b) Linear amplitude

623 

spectrum of a call phrase. Red lines indicate energy quartiles and blue line indicates the mean frequency of the phrase.

624 

Figure 3. In blue petrels, correlation circles of CIA showing relationships between the two first components of the 625 

CIA and (a) the morphometric measurements, and (b) the acoustic parameters of male calls.

626 

Figure 4. In Antarctic prions, correlation circles of CIA showing relationships between the two first components 627 

of the CIA and (a) the morphometric measurements, and (b) the acoustic parameters of male calls.

Figure

Table 1. Abbreviations and descriptions for the acoustic parameters measured on each phrase of blue petrel and 613 
Table  2.  Potential  of  Individual  Coding  (PIC)  for  the  acoustic  parameters  measured  on  blue  petrel  (top)  and 616 

Références

Documents relatifs

Cumsum for salinity and Shannon index (night and day) calculated on large copepods (BIONESS net data) during Dynaproc 2 cruise... Raybaud

ABSTRACT: This research aimed to apply the near infrared spectroscopy technique (NIRS) for fast prediction of basic density and morphological characteristics of wood fibers

The fundamental outcome of the mission will be the Gaia catalogue produced by the Gaia Data Analysis and Pro- cessing Consortium (DPAC), which will contain a variable

However, islet enlargement, attributed to increased beta cell proliferation and decreased beta cell apoptosis, has been reported in a limited number of human samples [ 15 ],

In both men and women, age correlates negatively and sta- tistically significantly above the p = 0.05 level with physical fitness and level of education. The negative correlation of

For many years, in any follow-up consultation of patients who take adjuvant endocrine treatment, the dialog with the patient has included targeted questions regarding

rus (SRP), the two major dissolved N and P species avail- able to aquatic biota, respond differently to varying water discharge rates (Q) in agricultural drainage pipes and rivers

After establishing an experimental mouse model of intratracheal adiaspiromycosis infection with the causative agent Emmonsia crescens, we observed adiaspore development.. The