• Aucun résultat trouvé

Information structure and choice of perspective in Hungarian narrative discourse: a developmental study

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Information structure and choice of perspective in Hungarian narrative discourse: a developmental study"

Copied!
53
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Information structure and choice of perspective in Hungarian narrative

discourse: a developmental study

Gabriella Fekete

Dynamique Du Langage

(UMR 5596 CNRS & Université Lyon 2) gabriella.fekete@univ-lyon2.fr

Syntax of the World’s Languages lll, Free University of Berlin, September 25-28, 2008

(2)

Narrative production

• Organization of events by linguistic expressions

• Multi-propositional structure

• Coherence

• Guide of attention flow in the story

(3)

Mastery of many linguistic tools BUT

Difficulties in the construction of a narration

(4)

Several linguistic options for the organization of the information flow (Jisa et al. 2002)

Constructions in competition for the same function

(5)

Berman & Slobin (1994)

Dimensions of event construal:

(a) selection of topic ;

(b) selection of loci of control and effect ; (c) selection of event view ;

(d) selection of degree of agency.

(6)

Distribution of information:

- Choice of elements - Attribution of salience

- Selection of foreground or background

(7)

Foundations of a basis of reference

order of access important

• “Privilege” of the initial element

(Gernsbacher & Hargreaves 1992, Croft 1994)

• Initial focus of attention

(Langacker 1998)

• “Starting point”

(MacWhinney 1977)

(8)

Problem with the terminology „starting point”

Languages with fixed word order (English, French):

First element = subject/agent = topic = starting point

Equivalents

(9)

Languages with flexible word order (Hebrew, Spanish, Turkish, Hungarian):

First element = subject/agent / direct objet / indirect object

In Hungarian:

• Not obligatory topic → clauses beginning with the verb

• Pro drop+object marking in the verb → clauses containing a verbal form

S/A, starting point, topic = not equivalents

Use of the term « perspective »

(10)

Several devices for the manipulation of perspective (Berman & Slobin 1994):

Transitivity

(1) a.The boy was frightened because an owl came out.

b. The boy was afraid of the owl.

c. The owl frightened the boy.

(11)

Reference form

(2) The boy hung on to the antlers of a deer.

The deer/he/which/this one ran away.

(12)

Voice

(3) a. The bees chased the dog.

b. The dog was chased (by the bees).

c. (fr.) Le chien s’enfuit. = The dog ran away.

(13)

Topicalization, Word order

(4) a. As for the frog, the boy saw it.

b. (hu) A békát nézte a fiú. = ‘The frog(acc.) saw the boy.’

(14)

How do Hungarian children and adults

organize the components of information?

Which participant do they prefer to take as the perspective?

(15)

Methodology

Subjects

• 5 age groups : 3, 5, 7/8, and 11/12 years of age, and adults

• 15 subjects in each group

• Monolingual Hungarian speakers from middle class backgrounds

(16)

Task

A series of pictures with no text

Elicitation of the narrative

(17)

4 episodes treated here

(18)

Table 1. Number of subjects, number of clauses encoding the 4 targeted events, mean and range of clauses coded per subject.

3-year-

olds 5-year-

olds 7-8-

year- olds

11-12- year-

olds

adults Total

n 15 15 15 15 15 75

Total number of

clauses 43 63 68 76 78 328

Mean clauses per

subject 2.87 4.2 4.53 5.07 5.2

Range 1.6 2.7 2.8 1.9 3.9

Mean of episodes not mentioned per subject

1.6

0.87

0.53

0.53 0.33 Number of subjects

who do not mention

all episodes 12 8 7 3 3

Range 1.3 1.2 1.2 2.3 1.2

(19)

Characteristics of the Hungarian Language

• Agglutinative language of the Finno-Ugrian language family

• Pro-drop

• Case-marked grammatical relation for every argument (17 cases)

• No gender

(20)

• Object marker in transitive verb forms (2 types of conjugation).

(5)

a fiú meg-ijeszt-ett egy bagly-ot def boy prev-to frighten-past.3S indef owl-ACC the boy frightened an owl

a-ki le-lök-t-e a fá-ról

rel-animate prev-to push-past-3SO def tree-delative which pushed [him] out of the tree

(21)

• No passive construction

• Lexicalized verbal form for the middle voice

• Left-dislocation → another register

(22)

• SVO (subject-verb-object) canonical word order - Very flexible

- Pragmatically determined (topic- focus-comment)

3 syntactic 3 pragmatic positions = functions

• Sentence-initial position → topic

• Immediately preverbal position → focus

• Postverbal position → background information (comment)

(23)

• Topic = definite and/or animate NP

• Focus = the most information-bearing element

Identification : - the strongest accent of the sentence

- pre-verb moved after the verb

• Post-verbal position = backgrounded / defocused NP

(24)

Restrictive hierarchy of the position of the argument in perspective

(25)

Argument in perspective

Initial (topic)

(26)

(6)

a. viszont egy ideges vakond meg-csíp-t-e

but indef nervous mole prev-to bite-past-3SO az orr-á-t

def nose-poss-ACC

but a nervous mole has bitten his nose (19;06.d) b. itt meg a kutyá-t el-kerget-ik

here and def dog-ACC prev-to chase-present-3PO

a legy-ek def fly-pl

and here the dog, the flies are chasing it (5;08.f)

(27)

Argument in perspective

Initial (topic)

Grammatical (subject/agent)

(28)

(7)

a. mert meg-harap-t-a a orr-á-t

because prev-to bite-past-3SO def nose-poss-ACC

because [it] bit his nose (5;07.b)

b. és itt le-dob-ja

and here prev-to throw-present-3SO

and [it] throws him here (3;07.c)

(29)

Argument in perspective

Initial (topic)

Grammatical (subject/agent) Grammatical (object)

(30)

(8)

ugyanis kerget-ik a méh-ek ideed to chase-present-3PO def bee-pl.

indeed, the bees are chasing [it]. (21;07.n)

(31)

Argument in perspective

Initial (topic)

Grammatical (subject/agent) Grammatical (object)

Post-verbal

(32)

(9)

ott le-dob-t-a a szarvas over there prev-to throw-past-3SO def deer

a kis-fiú-t

def little-boy-ACC

over there, the deer has thrown the little boy (8;01.a)

(33)

Argument in perspective

Initial (topic)

Grammatical (subject/agent) Grammatical (object)

Post-verbal

Pre-verbal (focus)

(34)

(10)

mert az odú-ból egy bagoly jött elő because def hole-elatif indef owl to come.past.3S prev because it was an owl that came out o the hole (11;08.f)

(35)

Results

Graph1. Mean (%) of the distribution of intransitive versus transitive clauses in the 4 episodes

• Intransitive constructions decrease (F(4,65)=2.323,p=.0658)

• Transitive options increase (F(4,65)=2.045,p=.0984)

• Intransitive clauses with obliques increase (F(4,65)=.588,p=.6726)

(36)

Only clauses with at least two participants (transitive clauses, intransitive clauses with oblique(s))

One device alternating perspective in Hungarian

Variations in word order

(37)

Graph 2. Mean (%) of the distribution of positions of the actor/agent perspective in the clauses with two participants in the 4 episodes

• 3-year-olds: grammatical forms (F(4,65)=.768,p=.5496)

• 5 and 7/8-year-olds : initial position (F(4,65)=3.022,p=.0238)

• 7/8-year-olds: post-verbal position (F(4,65)=2.075,p=.0942)

• 11/12-year-olds and adults: alternance of initial and grammatical positions

(38)

Graph 3. Mean (%) of the distribution of the position, the characters and the linguistic means used for the actor/agent perspective in the clauses with two participants in the 4 episodes.

• Secondary characters = actor/agent (F(4,65)=5.172,p=.0011)

• 3-year-olds: grammatical options (F(4,65)=.522,p=.7199)

• 5 and 7/8-year-olds: lexical noun phrases (F(4,65)=3.126,p=.0205)

• 11/12-year-olds and adults: alternance of grammatical and lexical devices

• 11/12-year-olds: pronominals in remarkable proportion (F(4,65)=5.409,p=.0008)

(39)

Graph 4. Mean (%) of the distribution of positions of the oblique/patient perspective in the clauses with two participants in the 4 episodes

• 3-year-olds: grammatical forms (F(4,65)=3.187,p=.0188)

• 5 and 7/8-year-olds: initial position (F(4,65)=1.222,p=.3103)

• 11/12 ans and adults: initial position

(40)

Graph 5. Mean (%) of the distribution of the position, the characters and the linguistic means used for the oblique/patient perspective in the clauses with two participants in the 4 episodes.

• Primary characters = oblique/patient (F(4,65)=1.322,p=.2713)

• 3-year-olds: grammatical options (F(4,65)=3.187,p=.0188)

• 5 and 7/8-year-olds: lexical noun phrases (F(4,65)=.685,p=.6050)

• 11/12-year-olds and adults: lexical noun phrases

• Adults : pronominals in significative proportion (F(4,65)=1.700,p=.1607)

(41)

Discussion

• Clauses with two participants: increase with age

• 3 and 5-year-olds: intransitive clauses

• 7/8 and 11/12-year-olds: intransitive and transitive clauses

• Adults: transitive clauses

(42)

• Secondary characters = actor/agent

• Primary characters = oblique/patient

Secondary characters = do the action

Primary characters = affected by the action

• Oblique/patient perspective → increases with age

! 3-year-olds = appearance of word order

which take the oblique/patient in perspective

(43)

• 3-year-olds = grammatical forms for the perspective

• 5 and 7/8-year-olds = lexical noun phrases whatever the perspective

• 11/12-year-olds and adults = alternation of the 2 linguistics tools for the actor/agent,

lexical noun phrases for the oblique/patient

(44)

• Pronominal oblique/patient at the beginning of sentences in 11/12-year-olds and adults = surprising

In Hungarian, personal pronouns used with a tonic function

Synthesis of parallel actions of the two protagonists, thus contrasted

(45)

• Different linguistic tools depending on the age

groups → no mastery of the conventional rules of referential coherence until the age of 11/12 years

• Resort to different strategies :

- thematic subject strategy (pronominal forms to refer to the main character irrespective of the

function),

- nominal strategy (full nominal even for maintaining characters) )

- and anaphoric strategy ( pronominals for

maintaining reference but nominals for switching).

(Karmiloff-Smith 1981, Wigglesworth 1997).

(46)

• Position of the arguments in perspective → link to the strategies mentionned above

3-year-olds = actor/agent or oblique/patient integrated in the verbal form

5 and 7/8-year-olds = actor/agent or oblique/patient in initial position

(47)

• Post-verbal position attested in the 7-8 year olds

Actor/agent taken in background

Strong topicalization is compensated

(48)

11/12-year-olds and adults = initial and grammatical positions for the actor/agent and initial position for the oblique/patient

(49)

Conclusion

• 3-year-olds = attempt to alternate perspectives but exclusively with verbal forms integrating the affected character

• 5-year-olds = mastery already unsteady of the use of the different ways to encode the actions

• 7/8-year-olds = “true” variation of the canonical word order for pragmatic reasons

• From 7/8-year-olds = initial position favoured for the argument in perspective or its integration in the

verbal form → choice depends on the discursive function of the argument.

(50)

• Linguistic means selected to package the information

properly encoded to discursive functions → difficult to control before 11/12-year-olds.

• The establishment of the referential coherence not perfectly mastered by the children

• Use of different strategies (Karmiloff-Smith1981, Wiglesworth, 1997, Fekete 2008)

- toddlers = pronominal forms (thematic strategy) - oldest children = nominals (nominal strategy)

- adults = coordination of these two strategies (anaphoric strategy)

(51)

• 7/8-year-olds = particular concerning the combination of the linguistic means favoured and the position employed for the argument in perspective

Post-verbal position for the lexical AC/AG

At the same time resort to the nominal strategy, and try to compensate the difficulties of the referential task with the help of the pragmatic functions of word order.

Solution for the excessive lexicalization at the beginning of the sentence → manipulation of the referents’ order

This is another solution, which they already master, to put the chosen element in background.

(52)

• Capacity of all the children to put the patients of the action in perspective, using different

linguistic and pragmatic tools

• Most difficulties in the application of the conventional rules of narration

(53)

• Berman, R. Slobin, D. I. (Eds.) (1994) Relating Events in Narrative: A Crosslinguistic Developmental Study. Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum.

• Croft, W. (1994) Voice: beyond control and affectedness. In Hopper, P. & Fox, B.

Voice: Form and Function. pp. 89-117. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

• Fekete, Gabriella (to appear 2008). Referential cohesion in Hungarian: a developmental study.

• GERNSBACHER, M. A., HARGREAVES, D. (1992) The privilege of primacy:

Experimental data and cognitive explanations. In Payne, D. L. Pragmatics of word order flexibility. pp. 83-116. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

• Jisa, H., Reilly, J., Verheoven, L., Baruch, E. & Rosado, E. (2002) "Cross- linguistic perspectives on the use of passive constructions in written texts."

Journal of Written Language and Literacy, 5, 163-81.

• Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1981) The grammatical marking of thematic structure in the development of language production. In Deutsch, W. (Ed.). The child’s

construction of language. New York: Academic Press, 121-147.

• Langacker, R. W. (1998) Conceptualization, Symbolization, and Grammar. In Tomasello, M. The new psychology of language : Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure. pp. 1-39. Mahwah, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum

• MacWhinney, B. (1977) Starting points. In Language, 53. pp. 152-168.

• Mayer, M. (1969) Frog, Where are you? Amsterdam : Dial Press.

• Strömqvist, S., Verhoeven, L. (Eds.) (2003) Relating events in narrative – typological and contextual perspectives. Mahwah, New Jersey : Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

• Wigglesworth, G. (1997) Children’s individual approaches to the organization of narrative. In Journal of Child Language 24: 279-309.

Références

Documents relatifs

В исследовании также может отмечаться, что реклама более здоровых пищевых продуктов способна оказывать некоторое воздействие на улучшение

We observed from tongue contour tracings that the Mid Bunched configuration generally has a lower tongue tip than the Front Bunched one in speakers who present both bunched

In par- ticular, one can examine the continuous correlation be- tween solar wind pressure and the position and motion of the boundary during such a long time interval, rather

Keywords: Scientific journal, Online scientific publication, Fair Open access, Preprints archive, Overlay journal.. 1

Controls automatically follow events in time, and the high res- olution of automatic updating mechanisms results not only in the pro- cessing of the fi rst stimulus when it is

This information will subsequently be combined with data from developmental toxicity studies of NP using other routes of exposure, of ambient air pollu- tion using total

It was predicted that this particular use of and to begin the narration of the last event would be more frequent in simultaneous display (which allowed the speaker to build an

• Discursive local topic: aboutness, perspective + connectivity functions, background, known or deductible, weak degree of the communicative dynamism, place anywhere in the