• Aucun résultat trouvé

Can processing demands explain toddlers’ performance in false-belief tasks?

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Can processing demands explain toddlers’ performance in false-belief tasks?"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Can processing demands explain toddlers’

performance in false-belief tasks?

The MIT Faculty has made this article openly available.

Please share

how this access benefits you. Your story matters.

Citation

Rubio-Fernández, Paula et al. “Can Processing Demands Explain

Toddlers’ Performance in False-Belief Tasks?” Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences 114, 19 (April 2017): E3750–E3750 ©

2017 National Academy of Sciences

As Published

http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.1701286114

Publisher

National Academy of Sciences (U.S.)

Version

Final published version

Citable link

http://hdl.handle.net/1721.1/112168

Terms of Use

Article is made available in accordance with the publisher's

policy and may be subject to US copyright law. Please refer to the

publisher's site for terms of use.

(2)

LETTER

Can processing demands explain toddlers

performance in false-belief tasks?

Paula Rubio-Fern ´andeza,1, Julian Jara-Ettingerb, and Edward Gibsona

Two-and-a-half-year-olds normally fail standard false-belief tasks. In the classic version, children have to say where a protagonist will look for an apple that, unbeknownst to her, was moved to a new location. Children under 4 generally predict that the protago-nist will look for her apple in its current location, rather than where she left it. Setoh, Scott, and Baillargeon (1) argue that young children fail standard false-belief tasks because of their high processing demands, not because young children lack the necessary theory of mind.

This processing-demands account is challenged by “low-inhibition tasks,” in which the apple is removed from the scene altogether (2): rather than succeeding, children under 4 perform at chance in these tasks. Setoh et al. argue that low-inhibition tasks still pose high processing demands for young children, because they must still inhibit a weaker prepotent response (corresponding with the unknown location of the mar-ble) and also generate the correct response. In their study, Setoh et al. show that, indeed, when 2.5-y-olds receive a low-inhibition task with response-generation training (two practice trials with factual“where” ques-tions), they perform significantly above chance.

However, Setoh et al.’s pattern of results is incon-sistent with their account. If both inhibitory control and response generation are critical to passing a standard false-belief task, then reduced demands in either of these two processes (through the low-inhibition modifi-cation or the response-generation practice, respectively) should improve children’s performance. However, this is not what Setoh et al. observed. Experiment 4 shows that,

when 2.5-y-old children receive the same two practice trials in a standard, high-inhibition task, they perform significantly below chance—as they normally do without practice trials.

Setoh et al. claim that these results are “exactly those predicted by [their] account” because the inhibitory-control demands of standard tasks are still too high for young children, even with response-generation practice. However, if children in experi-ment 4 had performed at chance, or succeeded, their improved performance would have also been taken as evidence that response-generation practice is critical. Thus, not only is the prediction inconsistent with the account, but, on this logic, any possible pattern of results in experiment 4 would count as evidence for their account.

Setoh et al.’s account could explain the data if they assumed a linear process in which participants must first inhibit their true belief before they can generate the correct response. This mechanism, however, is in-consistent with looking data from infants, toddlers, and also adults suggesting that they do not first con-sider the true-belief response (3–5).

Finally, low-inhibition versions of the false-belief task are especially prone to solutions that do not require attributing a false belief. In Setoh et al.’s design, child-ren’s training on factual “where” questions might prompt them to point to the last location where the apple was. As such, the results of their study do not speak to the continuity argument, that is, the view that infants, tod-dlers, and older children all reveal the same understand-ing of belief in different types of false-belief tasks.

1 Setoh P, Scott RM, Baillargeon R (2016) Two-and-a-half-year-olds succeed at a traditional false-belief task with reduced processing demands. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:13360–13365.

2 Devine RT, Hughes C (2014) Relations between false belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Dev 85:1777–1794.

3 Clements WA, Perner J (1994) Implicit understanding of belief. Cogn Dev 9:377–395.

4 Southgate V, Senju A, Csibra G (2007) Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychol Sci 18:587–592. 5 Rubio‐Fern ´andez P (2017) Can we forget what we know in a false‐belief task? An investigation of the true‐belief default. Cogn Sci

41:218–241.

a

Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139; andb

Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520

Author contributions: P.R.-F., J.J.-E., and E.G. designed research; and P.R.-F. and J.J.-E. wrote the paper. The authors declare no conflict of interest.

1To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: prubio@mit.edu.

E3750 | PNAS | May 9, 2017 | vol. 114 | no. 19 www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1701286114

LET

T

Références

Documents relatifs

In [3] and [4] the authors present methods of image classification based on data compression techniques, in the first case using a video compressor such as MPEG4

• In a configuration with 32 DSs, Sprint processed 5.3x more transactions per second than a standalone server while running a database 30x bigger than the one fitting in the main

Corollary 2.5 provides a polynomial algorithm to transform S such that it contains at most one task that is not scheduled during its minimum processing time in polynomial time (no

Third, there is no relation between f0 declination and rib cage compression across different speech tasks, even though slope values differ between the two tasks

Considering the well defined structure, it may seem surprising to know that most tasks are written with LibreOffice, with some others written in HTML, with, as the files show, no way

Attempts at efficient parallelization of the breadth-first search algorithm with skewed degree distribution are hampered by the workload imbalance amongst computational threads

(5) Thus [10], limiting the estimate of procedural complexity by TRM method O(nm) does not exceed the complexity of 2-classed polynom for the number of words in the text n and

Основным препятствием для эффективного распараллеливания поиска в ширину на графе являются дисбаланс вычислитель- ной нагрузки внутри