• Aucun résultat trouvé

A short note on number preference

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "A short note on number preference"

Copied!
3
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-00572752

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00572752

Preprint submitted on 2 Mar 2011

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

A short note on number preference

Nicolas Gauvrit, Jean-Paul Delahaye

To cite this version:

Nicolas Gauvrit, Jean-Paul Delahaye. A short note on number preference. 2011. �hal-00572752�

(2)

A SHORT NOTE ON NUMBER PREFERENCE

Nicolas Gauvrit & Jean-Paul Delahaye

Since the early seventies, repeated experiments have shown a general preference, at least in the United States and Europe, for number seven against any other number in the brackets 0-9 (e.g. Simon, 1971; Kubovy and Psotka, 1976). When subjects are asked to choose a number between 0 and 9, 30% of them chose a 7. This

“seven phenomenon” gave rise to two research trends that seem to grow apart, each one ignoring the other.

PURELY COGNITIVE APPROACH

In a cognitive science approach, Shepard and Arabie (1979) suggested that the predominance of seven might be explained by mathematical properties of the numbers involved. This idea have been extended and specified by Griffiths and Tennenbaum (2001). Their model is based on two main ideas.

First, when subjects are asked to produce a random-like response (or to pick the first item that comes to mind), they do not actually try to do so. Instead, they try to maximize the probability that their response be random, in a Bayesian way. More precisely, a response x could a priori be random or based on a simple rule (such as giving the first number in the list). The relevant information when it comes to pick up at random an item x is P(x|random). However, subjects actually try to maximize P(random|x), a quantity that can be computed through Bayesian procedures. In that way, they ascertain that their response is an evidence of randomness.

Second, subjects are sensitive to some properties of numbers: being even, power of 2, or holding special positions such as endpoints (0 or 9), and so on. The less properties a number x has within the list, the greater P(random|x). There comes number 7, which is the only number that, in the bracket 0-9, shows none of the properties Griffiths and Tennenbaum assume subjects spontaneously consider.

CULTURAL APPROACH

On a totally different basis, many researchers suggested that the number 7 preference can be explained by cultural context. The repetition of the symbol 7 in the Bible, for instance, could well explain its predominance.

Arguments in favour of the cultural approach, and against the purely cognitive view, are determining, and can be categorized in two types of evidence:

First, the cultural dependency of numerical choices is well established. For instance, number 9 outdoes number 7 in Nigeria (Vandewiele et al, 1986), whereas 3 is preferred in Turkey (Koloğlu et al., 2002). Other differences have been recently mentioned as well (Strenge et al., 2009). To the best of our knowledge, cultural differences in lotto players’ behaviour have not been mentioned in any publication about number preference, but it provides a confirmation of the cultural approach as well. European players indeed prefer small numbers, and among those number 7 (Roger and Broihanne, 2007), whereas players in Taiwan do show a totally different pattern of choices, preferring 8 over 7, and 36 even more (Hwai-Chung et al., 2006).

Second, the study of psychiatric disorders in relation with numerical choices shows no effect of mental impairment (Koloğlu et al., 2009). A numerical choice based on arithmetical considerations would need healthy executive functions, which does not seem to be the case. Using pseudo-random sequences in a neuropsychological approach, Loetscher and Brugger (2009) even suggest that neglect patients are actually more likely to show enhanced response stereotypy. This result is based on sequential responses properties, but we may assume that stereotypy would mean, in a one-number pick up task, an even greater predominance of seven.

A THIRD WAY?

(3)

Considering the above arguments, opposing a purely cognitive approach and a cultural view gives no chance to cognitive theories of numbers preference. However, the two views are not contradictory, and the cognitive approach outmatches the cultural one in terms of predictive power.

The cognitive approach might be made more flexible to encompass some cultural aspects. For instance, one may imagine that the list of number properties subjects are aware of depends upon cultural context, therefore making a difference in the application of the purely cognitive process involved in the response choice.

One interesting feature in the cognitive approach is that it accounts not only for the predominance of seven (in Europe and the USA), but for the whole distribution of responses (e.g. Griffiths and Tennenbaum, 2001).

Even if the purely cognitive approach obviously cannot account for cultural dependency, the cultural view could also be a dead end if it does not include cognitive aspects, since it would then be bound to remain qualitative.

This is why we advocate “reconciliation” between the two views.

REFERENCES

Griffiths, T. L., and Tennenbaum, J. B. (2001). Randomness and coincidences: Reconciling intuition and probability theory. In J. D. Moore & K. Stenning (Eds.), 23d annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 370-375. Edimbourg, UK.

Hwai-Chung, H., Shih-Chin, L., and Hsiou-Wei, L. (2006). Gambler’s Fallacy in the Taiwan Lotto Market. Taiwan Economic Review, 34(4), 417–444.

Koloğlu, M., Atmaca, M., Tezcan, E., Unal, A., and Gecici, O. (2002). Color and number preferences of patients with psychiatric disorders in eastern Turkey. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 207-213.

Kubovy, M., and Psotka, J. (1976). The predominance of seven and the apparent spontaneity of numerical choices. Journal of experimental psychology: Human perception and performance, 2, 291-294.

Loetscher, T., and Brugger, P. (2009). Random number generation in neglect patients reveals enhanced response stereotypy, but no neglect in number space, Neuropsychologia, 47, 276-279.

Roger, P., and Broihanne, M.-H. (2007). Efficiency of betting markets and the rationality of players: evidence from the french 6/49 lotto, Journal of Applied Statistics, 34, 5.

Shepard, R., and Arabie, P. (1979). Additive cluttering: Representation of similarities as combinations of discrete overlaping properties. Psychological Review, 86, 87-123.

Simon, W. E. (1971). Number and color responses of some college students: preliminary evidence for a “blue and seven” phenomenon. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 33, 373-374.

Strenge, H., Lesmana, C. B. J., and Suryani, L. K. (2009). Random number generation in bilingual Balinese and German students: Preliminary findings from an exploratory cross-cultural study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 109, 61-75.

Vandewiele, M., D’Hondt, W., Didillon, H., Iwawaki, S., and Mwamwendat, T. (1986). Number and color preferences in four countries. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63(2), 945-946.

Références

Documents relatifs

We considered the possibility that there was only very weak molecular correlation along the texture axis (a classical nematic) so that the observed meridional arcs could be

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des

First I propose a statistical examination of what is found in English, depending on number magnitude (from zero to the infinite) and number type (not just cardinal numbers,

Instrumentation 2x Alto Saxophone 2x Tenor Saxophone Baritone Saxophone 4x Trumpet / Flugelhorn 3x Tenor Trombone Bass Trombone Electric Guitar Bass Guitar

Specifically, we show that it is impossible to distinguish - using choice data - if the behavior of an agent (be it an individual or an institution) that satisfies the

In order to simplify the process of converting numbers from the modular representation to the positional representation of numbers, we will consider an approximate method that allows

Furthermore such a maximal lVln must necessarily be irreducible. Otherwise it must contain a ring of three regions.. Thus in this remaining case we are also led

Moreover we make a couple of small changes to their arguments at, we think, clarify the ideas involved (and even improve the bounds little).. If we partition the unit interval for