• Aucun résultat trouvé

Related literature

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Partager "Related literature"

Copied!
1
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Introduction & objectives

Cooperation declines as the game progresses to its final rounds. As a consequence, individual payoffs decrease as well and players receive a lower fraction of the maximum possible

payoff in each round.

Conclusion Analysis

Methodology

Standard PGG

In line with former research, results have verified that individuals, on average, provide levels of contributions halfway the free- riding and the full provision scenario. Regarding the use of incentives, Figure 1 shows how both punishment and reward succeeded to prevent the decline of cooperation observed in the standard game. In this respect, while punishment has been more effective at incentivizing higher contribution levels, in Figure 2 we clearly see how reward has been far more effective at prompting higher payoffs. As shown in Figure 3, this can be the result of the use of punishment, which entails a cost for both the

player that assigns the penalty and the punished one.

ANALYSIS OF

COOPERATION IN THE PUBLIC GOOD SETTING

to free-ride in the framework of the financing of public goods

An experimental study aimed to analyze group behavior under the possibility

Under-investment of public goods is a concerning threat that almost all economies have to face. The same nature of this kind of goods provides the opportunity to free ride and, for such reason, they are often provided by governments.

However, the truth is that most of the time the maintenance of public goods still relies on the willingness of individuals to cooperate. Therefore, the question is: how could cooperation be incentivized and parasitism disincentivized?

To answer this question, a standard "Public Goods Game"

experiment with two of its variations has been conducted. In the first one, punishment opportunities were allowed and, in the second variation, rewarding opportunities were present.

This method has allowed the comparison of both incentives against each other and against the standard PGG, revealing both their weaknesses and advantages and providing some insight regarding which incentive is better overall.

Academic year 2020/2021 Bachelor's Degree in Economics

Arnau Ribot Domènech

Related literature

ANDREONI, J. (1995). Cooperation in public-goods experiments: kindness or confusion? American Economic Review, 85(4), 891-904.

FEHR, E. & GÄCHTER, S. (2000). Cooperation and Punishment in Public Goods Experiments. American Economic Review, 90 (4)(November 1999), 980-994.

RAND, D. G., DREBER, A., ELLINGSEN, T., FUDENBERG, D., & NOWAK, M. A. (2009). Positive interactions promote public cooperation. Science, 325(5945).

SZOLNOKI, A., & PERC, M. (2010). Reward and cooperation in the spatial public goods game. Epl, 92(3), 1-6.

Figure 1 - Mean contribution in each variation of the PGG Figure 2 - Mean payoff in each variation of the PGG Figure 3 - Usage of reward and punishment

This study provides evidence that both punishment and reward can succeed at sustaining cooperation in the PGG setting;

punishment delivers higher levels of cooperation but harms social welfare while reward prompts higher payoffs. Once analyzed strengths and downsides of each incentive, it has became palpable that deciding which incentive is better overall is far from easy.

However, taking as a given that we do not pursue cooperation for the sake of cooperation itself but for the larger payoffs it renders, I have concluded that reward can be the optimal incentive scheme. Therefore, future research should reconsider the

hegmony of punishment and test the validity of rewarding schemes.

Mean contribution Mean payoff Usage of punishment and reward

Mean contribution of individuals is extraordinarily stable throughout the

whole experiment. However, mean payoff and the fraction of maximum payoff decay with repetition due to the

increasing use of punishment.

Cooperation remains stable throughout the course of the game and so does the mean payoff. The usage of reward does not harm payoff and, as a result, players are able to achieve higher fractions of the

maximum possible payoff.

Peer punishment Peer reward

Results of the experiment

A total of 68 individuals participated in this study. They were divided into groups of four and played each variation of the "game" for a number of ten rounds. The data gathered through these experiments is summarized below.

A B C

Références

Documents relatifs

Zaprionus indianus is assigned to the subgenus Zaprionus, species group armatus and species subgroup vittiger (Chassagnard, 1988; Chassagnard and Tsacas, 1993).. We provide here

In addition, because Coxiella-like bacteria are present in tick salivary glands, they may be transmit- ted during blood meals and therefore directly represent an infection risk

Complétez les phrases suivantes avec le présent simple ou le présent

(.tbereallllrltetforldf'-directedieaminginaworid with values largely framed within a formal context? A5educaton,exploringooD- forma1 autonomous leaming, weseek clues 10

using other records in the lake sediment, including pollen (vegetation change), diatoms (lake water pH and chemistry), SOOt (coal combustion), charcoal (wood combustion),

LOADING PEST FROM TAPE 2 LOADING PEST FROM DRUM 4 Legitimate Control Characters 4 Illegitimate Control Characters 4 FILES FROM TAPE 5 THE EDITOR UNDER PEST CONTROL 6 EDITOR

J'ai vécu l'expérience dee run* de té B et 3( H â LVRF avec de* chercheur* qui n'ont par craint d'uiiliter mon matériel et d'accepter ma cet laboration. Tardieu et J.L. Ranck,

We aspire to see family practice operational in the countries of WHO’s Eastern Mediterranean Region, focusing on the needs of the patients and users of health services;