• Aucun résultat trouvé

Chapter 2. Article of thesis

2. Material and methods

4.3. Significance, future directions and limitations

Chronic pain conditions may advance with the age-related cognitive decline. Besides, pain is associated with changes in the brain that may worsen the cognitive decline observed in older adults. For instance, patients with chronic neuropathic or radicular pain show decreased cognitive performance and this decline is particularly observed in older patients (Moriarty et al., 2011, 2017). This suggests that clinical pain can decrease cognitive function and that this effect is moderated by age. Conversely, studies in patients with dementia indicate that cognitive decline is associated with greater amplitude and duration of pain-related activity in regions associated with sensory, affective and cognitive processes (Summers et al., 2016). Based on these interactions between cognition, pain and age, anodal tDCS may be especially useful in older persons affected by cognitive decline, chronic pain or both (Hsu et al., 2015). Besides, It remains to be determined whether anodal tDCS of the DLPFC may be effective at improving pain inhibition by WM in different clinical populations in which an attentional bias to pain was reported (Eccleston et al., 1999b; Torta et al., 2017a). Another limitation that should be mentioned is that tDCS is not a focal method and other regions and their networks may be stimulated in addition to the DLPFC, over which the anode was placed. There- fore, like in other tDCS studies, the effects reported here cannot be attributed to the DLPFC exclusively.

5. Conclusion

The present study shows that WM and pain inhibition is enhanced by anodal tDCS in older persons. This warrants future studies to examine whether multiple tDCS sessions with cognitive training may produce long-lasting changes in pain regulation and pain symptoms in healthy older persons and patients with cognitive decline or chronic pain.

Author’s contributions to the manuscript

1- Zoha Deldar contributed to all aspects of the research.

2- Nabi Rustamov contributed to data acquisition and interpretation.

3- Isabelle Blanchette contributed to experimental design, data interpretation and manuscript preparation.

4- Mathieu Piché contributed to all aspects of the research and obtained funding for the study.

All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflicts of interest

The authors report no financial or other relationship that may lead to any conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (MP - grant number 402176). The contribution of MP was supported by the research Chair in Pain Neurophysiology from Université du Québec à Trois- Rivières and the

Fonds de Recherche du Québec en Santé (FRQS). The contribution of Zoha Deldar was supported by the department of Anatomy from UQTR and the Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition (CERNEC) from Université de Montréal.

6. References

Bopp, K. L., & Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: a meta-analysis. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, 60(5), P223-233.

Borella, E., Carretti, B., & De Beni, R. (2008). Working memory and inhibition across the adult

life-span. Acta Psychologica, 128(1), 33-44.

doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.09.008

Brink, J. M., & McDowd, J. M. (1999). Aging and selective attention: an issue of complexity or multiple mechanisms? J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, 54(1), P30-33.

Darowski, E. S., Helder, E., Zacks, R. T., Hasher, L., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2008). Age-related differences in cognition: the role of distraction control. Neuropsychology, 22(5), 638-644. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.22.5.638

De Beni, R., & Palladino, P. (2004). Decline in working memory updating through ageing:

intrusion error analyses. Memory, 12(1), 75-89. doi:10.1080/09658210244000568 Deldar, Z., Rustamov, N., Bois, S., Blanchette, I., & Piche, M. (2018). Enhancement of pain

inhibition by working memory with anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. J Physiol Sci. doi:10.1007/s12576-018-0598-4 Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation

by attentional control. J Abnorm Psychol, 111(2), 225-236.

Eccleston, C., & Crombez, G. (1999). Pain Demands Attention: A Cognitive-Affective Model of the Interruptive Function of Pain. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 356-366.

doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.356

Erpelding, N., & Davis, K. D. (2013). Neural underpinnings of behavioural strategies that prioritize either cognitive task performance or pain. Pain, 154(10), 2060-2071.

doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.06.030

Fabiani, M. (2012). It was the best of times, it was the worst of times: a psychophysiologist's view of cognitive aging. Psychophysiology, 49(3), 283-304. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01331.x

French DJ, N. l. M., Vigneau F, French JA, Cyr CP, Evans RT. (2005). L’Échelle de dramatisation face à la douleur PCS-CF. Adaptation canadienne en langue française de l’échelle Pain Catastrophizing Scale [French]. Can J Behav Sci(37), 181–192.

Frondini, C., Lanfranchi, G., Minardi, M., & Cucinotta, D. (2007). Affective, behavior and cognitive disorders in the elderly with chronic musculoskelatal pain: the impact on an aging population. Arch Gerontol Geriatr, 44 Suppl 1, 167-171.

doi:10.1016/j.archger.2007.01.024

Gazzaley, A., Rissman, J., Cooney, J. W., & D'Esposito, M. (2005). Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging. Nature Neuroscience, 8(10), 1298-1300. doi:10.1038/nn1543

Hsu, W. Y., Ku, Y., Zanto, T. P., & Gazzaley, A. (2015). Effects of noninvasive brain stimulation on cognitive function in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging, 36(8), 2348-2359.

doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.04.016

Ladouceur, A., Rustamov, N., Dubois, J. D., Tessier, J., Lehmann, A., Descarreaux, M., . . . Piche, M. (2017). Inhibition of pain and pain-related brain activity by heterotopic noxious counter-stimulation and selective attention in chronic non-specific low back pain. Neuroscience. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.09.054

Ladouceur, A., Tessier, J., Provencher, B., Rainville, P., & Piché, M. (2012). Top-down attentional modulation of analgesia induced by heterotopic noxious counterstimulation.

Pain, 153(8), 1755-1762. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2012.05.019

Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?: selective attention under load. Trends Cogn Sci, 9(2), 75-82. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.004

Legrain, V., Crombez, G., & Mouraux, A. (2011). Controlling attention to nociceptive stimuli with working memory. PLoS One, 6(6), e20926. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020926 Legrain, V., Crombez, G., Plaghki, L., & Mouraux, A. (2013). Shielding cognition from

nociception with working memory. Cortex, 49(7), 1922-1934.

doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.014

Legrain, V., Damme, S. V., Eccleston, C., Davis, K. D., Seminowicz, D. A., & Crombez, G.

(2009). A neurocognitive model of attention to pain: behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Pain, 144(3), 230-232. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2009.03.020

Legrain, V., Perchet, C., & Garcia-Larrea, L. (2009). Involuntary orienting of attention to nociceptive events: neural and behavioral signatures. J Neurophysiol, 102(4), 2423-2434. doi:10.1152/jn.00372.2009

Lustig, C., May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (2001). Working memory span and the role of proactive interference. J Exp Psychol Gen, 130(2), 199-207.

Mansfield, K. E., Sim, J., Jordan, J. L., & Jordan, K. P. (2016). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the prevalence of chronic widespread pain in the general population. Pain, 157(1), 55-64. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000314

Marouf, R., Caron, S., Lussier, M., Bherer, L., Piche, M., & Rainville, P. (2014). Reduced pain inhibition is associated with reduced cognitive inhibition in healthy aging. Pain, 155(3), 494-502. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.11.011

Moriarty, O., McGuire, B. E., & Finn, D. P. (2011). The effect of pain on cognitive function: A review of clinical and preclinical research. Prog Neurobiol, 93(3), 385-404.

doi:http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.01.002

Moriarty, O., Ruane, N., O'Gorman, D., Maharaj, C. H., Mitchell, C., Sarma, K. M., . . . McGuire, B. E. (2017). Cognitive Impairment in Patients with Chronic Neuropathic or Radicular Pain: An Interaction of Pain and Age. Front Behav Neurosci, 11(100).

doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00100

Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1995). Frontal lobes, memory and aging. Structure and functions of the frontal lobes (Vol. 169).

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., . . . Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc, 53(4), 695-699.

doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

Oosterman, J. M., Gibson, S. J., Pulles, W. L., & Veldhuijzen, D. S. (2013). On the moderating role of age in the relationship between pain and cognition. Eur J Pain, 17(5), 735-741.

doi:10.1002/j.1532-2149.2012.00235.x

Park, S.-H., Seo, J.-H., Kim, Y.-H., & Ko, M.-H. (2014). Long-term effects of transcranial direct current stimulation combined with computer-assisted cognitive training in healthy older adults. Neuroreport, 25(2), 122-126. doi:10.1097/WNR.0000000000000080

Piche, M., Bouin, M., Arsenault, M., Poitras, P., & Rainville, P. (2011). Decreased pain inhibition in irritable bowel syndrome depends on altered descending modulation and higher-order brain processes. Neuroscience, 195, 166-175.

doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.08.040

Pourtois, G., Schettino, A., & Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Brain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention: what is magic and what is not. Biol Psychol, 92(3), 492-512. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.007

Roe, J. M., Nesheim, M., Mathiesen, N. C., Moberget, T., Alnæs, D., & Sneve, M. H. (2016).

The effects of tDCS upon sustained visual attention are dependent on cognitive load.

Neuropsychologia, 80, 1-8. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.005 Roelofs, J., Peters, M. L., McCracken, L., & Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2003). The pain vigilance and awareness questionnaire (PVAQ): further psychometric evaluation in fibromyalgia and other chronic pain syndromes. Pain, 101(3), 299-306. doi:10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00338-x

Spieler, D. H., Balota, D. A., & Faust, M. E. (1996). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 22(2), 461-479.

Summers, J. J., Kang, N., & Cauraugh, J. H. (2016). Does transcranial direct current stimulation enhance cognitive and motor functions in the ageing brain? A systematic review and meta- analysis. Ageing Res Rev, 25, 42-54. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2015.11.004

Torta, D. M., Legrain, V., Mouraux, A., & Valentini, E. (2017). Attention to pain! A neurocognitive perspective on attentional modulation of pain in neuroimaging studies.

Cortex, 89, 120-134. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.010

Van Damme, S., Legrain, V., Vogt, J., & Crombez, G. (2010). Keeping pain in mind: A motivational account of attention to pain. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 34(2), 204-213.

doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.01.005

Vigneau. (2009). L’inventaire d’anxiété situationnelle et de trait d’anxiété (IASTA-Y) structure factorielle et biais linguistique [French]. Can J Behav Sci, 41(49), 115–120.

Wolkenstein, L., & Plewnia, C. (2013). Amelioration of cognitive control in depression by transcranial direct current stimulation. Biol Psychiatry, 73(7), 646-651.

doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.010

Wu, Y.-J., Tseng, P., Chang, C.-F., Pai, M.-C., Hsu, K.-S., Lin, C.-C., & Juan, C.-H. (2014).

Modulating the interference effect on spatial working memory by applying transcranial

direct current stimulation over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Cogn, 91, 87-94. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2014.09.002

Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., & Leirer, V. O.

(1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17(1), 37-49.

doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4