• Aucun résultat trouvé

Directions pour les études futures

Nos études fournissent aux chercheurs la plus grande base de données d’estimations franco-canadiennes, jusqu’à présent, pour la familiarité conceptuelle et la force perceptuelle visuelle et auditive. Nos notations complètent les notations franco-canadiennes de la fréquence subjective et de l’imageabilité précédemment obtenues pour les mêmes mots (Desrochers & Thompson, 2009). Nous espérons que cette importante base de données s'avérera un outil pratique pour les chercheurs travaillant dans les domaines liés à la psycholinguistique, à l'éducation et à l'acquisition du langage ou aux troubles du langage chez les populations francophones du Canada. Comme directives futures de ce projet, il serait pertinent de regarder si ces valeurs sont représentatives aussi pour les autres variantes du français dans la

147

francophonie, comme la France, la Belgique, la Suisse et les pays francophones du Maghreb et du Moyen-Orient, entre autres. C’est vrai que les variations sociales et géographiques influencent les estimations subjectives des participants. Il est important de voir à quel point ces banques de données seraient utilisables pour la recherche auprès d’autres natifs du français. Cette étude représente une première étape nécessaire pour établir les normes de force perceptuelle en français pour les modalités de perception les plus étudiées (c.-à-d. visuelles et auditives). Nos résultats ont montré le rôle critique de ces variables pour le traitement des mots écrits. Cela met en évidence l'importance de continuer à collecter les normes pour les trois autres modalités de perception (olfactive, gustative et tactile). Les futures études devront aborder cette question.

Des études complémentaires devraient être menées pour tester l'impact spécifique de ces variables sur le traitement des mots dans d’autres tâches, comme la lecture des mots à voix haute. Nous sommes confiants que les nouvelles évaluations de la force perceptuelle visuelle et auditive pour le grand ensemble de noms en français que nous avons présenté ici, permettront aux nouvelles études d’enquêter sur le rôle de l’expérience de perception sur la représentation des concepts.

7. Conclusion

L’étude de la reconnaissance visuelle des mots a produit une quantité considérable d’informations sur les variables orthographiques (p. ex. longueur de mots), lexicales (p. ex. fréquence des mots et similarité orthographique) et sémantiques (p. ex. familiarité, imageabilité, concrétude et âge d’acquisition) qui ont une influence sur l’exécution de tâches langagières, telle que la décision lexicale (Balota et al., 2004; Bonin et al., 2018; Cortese & Khanna, 2007;

148

New et al., 2006). La force perceptuelle est une variable récente qui concerne l’expérience sensorielle/ perceptuelle et a un rôle dans la représentation conceptuelle des mots.

La familiarité conceptuelle et la force perceptuelle sont des variables sémantiques qui jouent un rôle facilitateur dans la décision lexicale, au-delà d’autres variables psycholinguistiques. Nos résultats confirment et développent les découvertes précédentes qui avaient démontré que les évaluations de la force perceptuelle visuelle et auditive, ne peuvent pas être considérées comme une autre forme de concrétude ou d'imageabilité (Connell & Lynott, 2012). Les variables de force perceptuelle visuelle et auditive confèrent une contribution indépendante, à la prédiction des latences dans le traitement des mots écrits en français, spécialement dans une tâche de décision lexicale. Ces résultats sont en ligne avec les modèles cognitifs fondés, indiquant l'importance de l'expérience perceptuelle dans la représentation conceptuelle (Barsalou, 2008). Les normes de force perceptuelle devraient prendre place dans les nouvelles recherches visant l’exploration du système sensoriel/perceptuel.

En conclusion, les chercheurs ont besoin d’avoir des normes pour les caractéristiques de ces mots, afin de contrôler méthodologiquement les différentes variables corrélées avec la ou les variables d’intérêt, afin d’en tirer des conclusions plus robustes et représentatives du traitement visuel de mots.

149

Bibliographie

Alario, F. X., & Ferrand, L. (1999). A set of 400 pictures standardized for French: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, and age of acquisition. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput, 31(3), 531-552.

Andrews, S. (1989). Frequency and Neighborhood Effects on Lexical Access: Activation or Search? (Vol. 15).

Audibert, L. (2007, 2007-06). Désambiguïsation lexicale automatique : sélection automatique d'indices. Paper presented at the Traitement Automatique des Langues Naturelles (TALN-2007), Toulouse, France.

Avdyli, R., Castejon, L., & Cuetos, F. (2014). Lexical effects in word naming in spanish children. Span J Psychol, 17, E23. doi:10.1017/sjp.2014.24

Ayora, P., & Ferrand, L. (2015). Psychologie cognitive de la lecture: Reconnaissance des mots écrits chez l'adulte: De Boeck Supérieur.

Balota, & Chumbley. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 10(3), 340-357.

Balota, Cortese, M. J., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D. H., & Yap, M. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. J Exp Psychol Gen, 133(2), 283-316. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.283

Balota, Pilotti, M., & Cortese, M. J. (2001). Subjective frequency estimates for 2,938 monosyllabic words. Mem Cognit, 29(4), 639-647.

Balota, Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., . . . Treiman, R. (2007). The English Lexicon Project. Behav Res Methods, 39(3), 445-459.

150

Baluch, B., & Besner, D. (2001). Basic processes in reading: semantics affects speeded naming of high-frequency words in an alphabetic script. Can J Exp Psychol, 55(1), 63-69. Barca, L., Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2002). Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms

for Italian nouns. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput, 34(3), 424-434.

Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci, 22(4), 577-609; discussion 610-560.

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu Rev Psychol, 59, 617-645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

Barton, J. J., Hanif, H. M., Eklinder Bjornstrom, L., & Hills, C. (2014). The word-length effect in reading: a review. Cogn Neuropsychol, 31(5-6), 378-412. doi:10.1080/02643294.2014.895314

Bedard, P., Audet, A. M., Drouin, P., Roy, J. P., Rivard, J., & Tremblay, P. (2017). SyllabO+: A new tool to study sublexical phenomena in spoken Quebec French. Behav Res Methods, 49(5), 1852-1863. doi:10.3758/s13428-016-0829-7

Binder, J. R., Westbury, C. F., McKiernan, K. A., Possing, E. T., & Medler, D. A. (2005). Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. J Cogn Neurosci, 17(6), 905-917.

Bonin, Méot, A., Aubert, L., Malardier, N., Niedenthal, P., & Capelle-Toczek, M.-C. (2003). Normes de concrétude, de valeur d'imagerie, de fréquence subjective et de valence émotionnelle pour 866 mots. [Concreteness, imageability, subjective frequency and emotionality ratings for 866 words.]. L'Année Psychologique, 103(4), 655-694. doi:10.3406/psy.2003.29658

151

Bonin, P., Fayol, M., & Chalard, M. (2001). Age of acquisition and word frequency in written picture naming. Q J Exp Psychol A, 54(2), 469-489. doi:10.1080/713755968

Bonin, P., Meot, A., & Bugaiska, A. (2018). Concreteness norms for 1,659 French words: Relationships with other psycholinguistic variables and word recognition times. Behav Res Methods. doi:10.3758/s13428-018-1014-y

Bonin, P., Meot, A., Ferrand, L., & Bugaiska, A. (2015). Sensory experience ratings (SERs) for 1,659 French words: Relationships with other psycholinguistic variables and visual word recognition. Behav Res Methods, 47(3), 813-825. doi:10.3758/s13428-014-0503-x Bonin, P., Peereman, R., Malardier, N., Meot, A., & Chalard, M. (2003). A new set of 299

pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput, 35(1), 158-167. Brysbaert, & Ghyselinck, M. (2006). The effect of age of acquisition: Partly frequency related,

partly frequency independent. Visual Cognition, 13(7-8), 992-1011. doi:10.1080/13506280544000165

Brysbaert, M., & Cortese, M. J. (2011). Do the effects of subjective frequency and age of acquisition survive better word frequency norms? Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 64(3), 545- 559. doi:10.1080/17470218.2010.503374

Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., & New, B. (2011). Assessing the usefulness of google books' word frequencies for psycholinguistic research on word processing. Front Psychol, 2, 27. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00027

152

Chedid, G., Wilson, M. A., Bedetti, C., Rey, A. E., Vallet, G. T., & Brambati, S. M. (2018). Norms of conceptual familiarity for 3,596 French nouns and their contribution in lexical decision. Behav Res Methods. doi:10.3758/s13428-018-1106-8

Coltheart, Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. T., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. The psychology of reading.

Coltheart, Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychol Rev, 108(1), 204-256. Coltheart, & Winograd, E. (1986). Word imagery but not age of acquisition affects episodic

memory. Mem Cognit, 14(2), 174-180.

Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2010). Look but don't touch: Tactile disadvantage in processing modality-specific words. Cognition, 115(1), 1-9. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.005 Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2012). Strength of perceptual experience predicts word processing

performance better than concreteness or imageability. Cognition, 125(3), 452-465. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.010

Connell, L., & Lynott, D. (2014). I see/hear what you mean: semantic activation in visual word recognition depends on perceptual attention. J Exp Psychol Gen, 143(2), 527-533. doi:10.1037/a0034626

Connine, C. M., Mullennix, J., Shernoff, E., & Yelen, J. (1990). Word familiarity and frequency in visual and auditory word recognition. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 16(6), 1084- 1096.

Cortese, M. J., & Fugett, A. (2004). Imageability ratings for 3,000 monosyllabic words. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput, 36(3), 384-387.

153

Cortese, M. J., & Khanna, M. M. (2007). Age of acquisition predicts naming and lexical- decision performance above and beyond 22 other predictor variables: an analysis of 2,342 words. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 60(8), 1072-1082. doi:10.1080/17470210701315467

Cortese, M. J., & Schock, J. (2013). Imageability and age of acquisition effects in disyllabic word recognition. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 66(5), 946-972. doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.722660

Cortese, M. J., Simpson, G. B., & Woolsey, S. (1997). Effects of association and imageability on phonological mapping. Psychon Bull Rev, 4(2), 226-231. doi:10.3758/BF03209397 Crutch, S. J., Connell, S., & Warrington, E. K. (2009). The different representational

frameworks underpinning abstract and concrete knowledge: evidence from odd-one-out judgements. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 62(7), 1377-1388, 1388-1390. doi:10.1080/17470210802483834

Crutch, S. J., & Warrington, E. K. (2005). Abstract and concrete concepts have structurally different representational frameworks. Brain, 128(Pt 3), 615-627. doi:10.1093/brain/awh349

Cuetos, F., & Barbón, A. (2006). Word naming in Spanish. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 415-436. doi:10.1080/13594320500165896

Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1986). The syllable's differing role in the segmentation of French and English (Vol. 25).

Cutler, A., Mehler, J., Norris, D., & Segui, J. (1989). Limits on bilingualism. Nature, 340(6230), 229-230. doi:10.1038/340229a0

154

Davies, R., Wilson, M., Cuetos, F., & Burani, C. (2014). Reading in Spanish and Italian: effects of age of acquisition in transparent orthographies? Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 67(9), 1808- 1825. doi:10.1080/17470218.2013.872155

Davis, C. J., Perea, M., & Acha, J. (2009). Re(de)fining the orthographic neighborhood: the role of addition and deletion neighbors in lexical decision and reading. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 35(5), 1550-1570. doi:10.1037/a0014253

de Groot. (2011). Language and cognition in bilinguals and multilinguals: An introduction. New York, NY, US: Psychology Press.

de Groot, A. M. (1989). Representational aspects of word imageability and word frequency as assessed through word association. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(5), 824-845. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.15.5.824

Della Rosa, P. A., Catricala, E., Vigliocco, G., & Cappa, S. F. (2010). Beyond the abstract- concrete dichotomy: mode of acquisition, concreteness, imageability, familiarity, age of acquisition, context availability, and abstractness norms for a set of 417 Italian words. Behav Res Methods, 42(4), 1042-1048. doi:10.3758/BRM.42.4.1042

Desrochers, A., Liceras, J. M., Fernandez-Fuertes, R., & Thompson, G. L. (2010). Subjective frequency norms for 330 Spanish simple and compound words. Behav Res Methods, 42(1), 109-117. doi:10.3758/BRM.42.1.109

Desrochers, A., & Thompson, G. L. (2009). Subjective frequency and imageability ratings for 3,600 French nouns. Behav Res Methods, 41(2), 546-557. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.2.546 Diependaele, K., Brysbaert, M., & Neri, P. (2012). How Noisy is Lexical Decision? Front

155

Diez-Alamo, A. M., Diez, E., Wojcik, D. Z., Alonso, M. A., & Fernandez, A. (2018). Sensory experience ratings for 5,500 Spanish words. Behav Res Methods. doi:10.3758/s13428- 018-1057-0

Ellis, A. W., & Morrison, C. M. (1998). Real age-of-acquisition effects in lexical retrieval. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 24(2), 515-523.

Eriksen, C. W., Pollack, M. D., & Montague, W. E. (1970). Implicit speech: Mechanism in perceptual encoding? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 84(3), 502-507. doi:10.1037/h0029274

Ernst, M. O., & Bulthoff, H. H. (2004). Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends Cogn Sci, 8(4), 162-169. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.002

Estes, Z., & Adelman, J. S. (2008). Automatic vigilance for negative words in lexical decision and naming: comment on Larsen, Mercer, and Balota (2006). Emotion, 8(4), 441-444; discussion 445-457. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.4.441

Evans, G., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Woollams, A. M. (2017). Seeing the Meaning: Top-Down Effects on Letter Identification. Front Psychol, 8, 322. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00322 Ferrand, L., Bonin, P., Meot, A., Augustinova, M., New, B., Pallier, C., & Brysbaert, M. (2008).

Age-of-acquisition and subjective frequency estimates for all generally known monosyllabic French words and their relation with other psycholinguistic variables. Behav Res Methods, 40(4), 1049-1054. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.4.1049

Ferrand, L., Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., New, B., Bonin, P., Meot, A., . . . Pallier, C. (2011). Comparing word processing times in naming, lexical decision, and progressive demasking: evidence from chronolex. Front Psychol, 2, 306. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00306

156

Ferrand, L., & New, B. (2003). Syllabic length effects in visual word recognition and naming. Acta Psychol (Amst), 113(2), 167-183.

Ferrand, L., New, B., Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., Bonin, P., Meot, A., . . . Pallier, C. (2010). The French Lexicon Project: lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and 38,840 pseudowords. Behav Res Methods, 42(2), 488-496. doi:10.3758/BRM.42.2.488

Ferrand, L., Segui, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1997). The syllable's role in word naming. Mem Cognit, 25(4), 458-470.

Feyereisen, P., Van der Borght, F., & Seron, X. (1988). The operativity effect in naming: a re- analysis. Neuropsychologia, 26(3), 401-415.

Fliessbach, K., Weis, S., Klaver, P., Elger, C. E., & Weber, B. (2006). The effect of word concreteness on recognition memory. Neuroimage, 32(3), 1413-1421. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.007

Forster, K. I., & Chambers, S. M. (1973). Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(6), 627-635. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022- 5371(73)80042-8

Fraisse, P. (1963). La perception des mots. ProblPmesde PsJ’rholirryuistique.

François, J., & Cordier, F. (2006). Psycholinguistique vs psychologie cognitive du langage : une simple variante terminologique ? Syntaxe et sémantique, 7(1), 57-78. doi:10.3917/ss.007.0057

Funnell, E., & Davies, P. D. M. (1996). JBR: A reassessment of concept familiarity and a category-specific disorder for living things. Neurocase, 2(6), 461-474. doi:10.1080/13554799608402422

157

Gala, N., François, T., Bernhard, D., & Fairon, C. (2014, 2014-07-01). A model to predict lexical complexity and to grade words. Paper presented at the TALN'2014, Marseille, France. Gardner, E., & Martin, J. (2019). Coding of Sensory Information.

Garrod, S. (2006). Psycholinguistic Research Methods. In (pp. 251-257).

Gerhand, S., & Barry, C. (1999). Age of acquisition, word frequency, and the role of phonology in the lexical decision task. Mem Cognit, 27(4), 592-602.

Ghyselinck, M., Lewis, M. B., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). Age of acquisition and the cumulative- frequency hypothesis: a review of the literature and a new multi-task investigation. Acta Psychol (Amst), 115(1), 43-67.

Gilhooly, K. J., & Logie, R. H. (1980). Age-of-acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity, and ambiguity measures for 1,944 words. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 12(4), 395-427. doi:10.3758/bf03201693

Glenberg, A. M., Witt, J. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2013). From the Revolution to Embodiment: 25 Years of Cognitive Psychology. Perspect Psychol Sci, 8(5), 573-585. doi:10.1177/1745691613498098

Goldstein, E. B. (2011). Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research, and Everyday Experience (Third). Belmpont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning.

Grainger, J., O'Regan, J. K., Jacobs, A. M., & Segui, J. (1989). On the role of competing word units in visual word recognition: the neighborhood frequency effect. Percept Psychophys, 45(3), 189-195.

Holcomb, P. J., Kounios, J., Anderson, J. E., & West, W. C. (1999). Dual-coding, context- availability, and concreteness effects in sentence comprehension: an electrophysiological investigation. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 25(3), 721-742.

158

Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2(2), 127-160. doi:10.1007/bf00401799

Hudson, P. T. W., & Bergman, M. W. (1985). Lexical knowledge in word recognition: Word length and word frequency in naming and lexical decision tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 24(1), 46-58. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(85)90015-4

Izura, C., Hernandez-Munoz, N., & Ellis, A. W. (2005). Category norms for 500 Spanish words in five semantic categories. Behav Res Methods, 37(3), 385-397.

Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the computational mind. Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press.

James, C. T. (1975). The role of semantic information in lexical decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1(2), 130-136. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.1.2.130

Jessen, F., Heun, R., Erb, M., Granath, D. O., Klose, U., Papassotiropoulos, A., & Grodd, W. (2000). The concreteness effect: evidence for dual coding and context availability. Brain Lang, 74(1), 103-112. doi:10.1006/brln.2000.2340

Juhasz, B. J. (2005). Age-of-acquisition effects in word and picture identification. Psychol Bull, 131(5), 684-712. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.684

Juhasz, B. J., & Yap, M. J. (2013). Sensory experience ratings for over 5,000 mono- and disyllabic words. Behav Res Methods, 45(1), 160-168. doi:10.3758/s13428-012-0242-9 Juhasz, B. J., Yap, M. J., Dicke, J., Taylor, S. C., & Gullick, M. M. (2011). Tangible words are recognized faster: the grounding of meaning in sensory and perceptual systems. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 64(9), 1683-1691. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.605150

159

Juphard, A., Carbonnel, S., & Valdois, S. (2004). Length effect in reading and lexical decision: evidence from skilled readers and a developmental dyslexic participant. Brain Cogn, 55(2), 332-340. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.035

Kaschak, M. P., Zwaan, R. A., Aveyard, M., & Yaxley, R. H. (2006). Perception of auditory motion affects language processing. Cogn Sci, 30(4), 733-744. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_54

Keetels, M., & Vroomen, J. (2012). Perception of Synchrony between the Senses. In M. M. Murray & M. T. Wallace (Eds.), The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes. Boca Raton (FL).

Kiefer, M., & Pulvermuller, F. (2012). Conceptual representations in mind and brain: theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Cortex, 48(7), 805-825. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.006

Kroll, J. F., & Merves, J. S. (1986). Lexical access for concrete and abstract words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12(1), 92-107. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.12.1.92

Kuperman, V., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words. Behav Res Methods, 44(4), 978-990. doi:10.3758/s13428- 012-0210-4

Lambon Ralph, M. A., Graham, K. S., Ellis, A. W., & Hodges, J. R. (1998). Naming in semantic dementia--what matters? Neuropsychologia, 36(8), 775-784.

Lara, B., Astorga, D., Mendoza-Bock, E., Pardo, M., Escobar, E., & Ciria, A. (2018). Embodied Cognitive Robotics and the learning of sensorimotor schemes. Adaptive Behavior, 26(5), 225-238. doi:10.1177/1059712318780679

160

Laxon, V., Gallagher, A., & Masterson, J. (2002). The effects of familiarity, orthographic neighbourhood density, letter-length and graphemic complexity on children's reading accuracy. Br J Psychol, 93(Pt 2), 269-287.

Lima, S., & Pollatsek, A. (1983). Lexical access via an orthographic code? The Basic Orthographic Syllabic Structure (BOSS) reconsidered (Vol. 22).

Lynott, D., & Connell, L. (2009). Modality exclusivity norms for 423 object properties. Behav Res Methods, 41(2), 558-564. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.2.558

Lynott, D., & Connell, L. (2013). Modality exclusivity norms for 400 nouns: the relationship between perceptual experience and surface word form. Behav Res Methods, 45(2), 516- 526. doi:10.3758/s13428-012-0267-0

Marin, B., & Legros, D. (2008). Chapitre 2. Psycholinguistique cognitive de la lecture. In Psycholinguistique cognitive (pp. 27-46). Louvain-la-Neuve: De Boeck Supérieur. Mason, J. M. (1978). Role of strategy in reading by mentally retarded persons. Am J Ment Defic,

82(5), 467-473.

Mehler, J., Dupoux, E., Segui, J., & Altmann, d. G. T. (1990). Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives.

Mehler, J., Hayes, R. W., Longuet-Higgins, H. C., Lyons, J., & Broadbent, D. E. (1981). The role of syllables in speech processing: infant and adult data. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences, 295(1077), 333-352. doi:doi:10.1098/rstb.1981.0144

Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). Coming of age: a review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48(7), 788-804. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.002

161

Miklashevsky, A. (2018). Perceptual Experience Norms for 506 Russian Nouns: Modality Rating, Spatial Localization, Manipulability, Imageability and Other Variables. J Psycholinguist Res, 47(3), 641-661. doi:10.1007/s10936-017-9548-1

Miller, G. A. (1965). The Psycholinguists. On The New Scientists of Language.

Moreno-Martinez, F. J., Montoro, P. R., & Rodriguez-Rojo, I. C. (2014). Spanish norms for age of acquisition, concept familiarity, lexical frequency, manipulability, typicality, and other variables for 820 words from 14 living/nonliving concepts. Behav Res Methods, 46(4), 1088-1097. doi:10.3758/s13428-013-0435-x

New, B., Ferrand, L., Pallier, C., & Brysbaert, M. (2006). Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: new evidence from the English Lexicon Project. Psychon Bull Rev, 13(1), 45-52.

New, B., Pallier, C., Brysbaert, M., & Ferrand, L. (2004). Lexique 2: a new French lexical database. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput, 36(3), 516-524.

Newcombe, P. I., Campbell, C., Siakaluk, P. D., & Pexman, P. M. (2012). Effects of emotional and sensorimotor knowledge in semantic processing of concrete and abstract nouns. Front Hum Neurosci, 6, 275. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00275

Nickels, L., & Howard, D. (2004). Dissociating Effects of Number of Phonemes, Number of Syllables, and Syllabic Complexity on Word Production in Aphasia: It's the Number of Phonemes that Counts. Cogn Neuropsychol, 21(1), 57-78. doi:10.1080/02643290342000122

Oleron, P. (1966). Estimation de mots Français sur des échelles de fréquence et d'abstraction. [Estimation of French words on a scale of frequency and abstraction.]. Bulletin de Psychologie, 19(8-12), 603-610.

162

Osgood, C. E., & Sebeok, T. A. (1967). Psycholinguistics: A Survey of Theory and Research Problems. Edited by Charles E. Osgood and Thomas A. Sebeok, Wit A Survey of Psycholinguistic Research, 1954-1964, by A. Richard Diebold and The Psycholinguists, by George A. Miller [3rd Printing]: London.

Paivio, A. (2013). Dual coding theory, word abstractness, and emotion: a critical review of Kousta et al. (2011). J Exp Psychol Gen, 142(1), 282-287. doi:10.1037/a0027004 Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Madigan, S. A. (1968). Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness

values for 925 nouns. J Exp Psychol, 76(1), Suppl:1-25.

Paivio, A., Yuille, J. C., & Smythe, P. C. (1966). Stimulus and response abstractness, imagery, and meaningfulness, and reported mediators in paired-associate learning. Can J Psychol, 20(4), 362-377.

Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci, 8(12), 976- 987. doi:10.1038/nrn2277

Pind, J., Jonsdottir, H., Tryggvadottir, H. B., & Jonsson, F. (2000). Icelandic norms for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) pictures: name and image agreement, familiarity, and age of acquisition. Scand J Psychol, 41(1), 41-48.

Pinker, S. (1979). Formal models of language learning. Cognition, 7(3), 217-283.

Plaut, D. C., & Shallice, T. (1993). Perseverative and semantic influences on visual object naming errors in optic aphasia: a connectionist account. J Cogn Neurosci, 5(1), 89-117. doi:10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.89

Raman, I., Raman, E., & Mertan, B. (2014). A standardized set of 260 pictures for Turkish: norms of name and image agreement, age of acquisition, visual complexity, and

163

conceptual familiarity. Behav Res Methods, 46(2), 588-595. doi:10.3758/s13428-013- 0376-4

Rogers, T. T., & Cox, C. R. (2015). The Neural Bases of Conceptual Knowledge. The Wiley Handbook on the Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory. doi:doi:10.1002/9781118332634.ch4

10.1002/9781118332634.ch4

Romani, C., McAlpine, S., & Martin, R. C. (2008). Concreteness effects in different tasks: implications for models of short-term memory. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove), 61(2), 292-323. doi:10.1080/17470210601147747

Sabsevitz, D. S., Medler, D. A., Seidenberg, M., & Binder, J. R. (2005). Modulation of the semantic system by word imageability. Neuroimage, 27(1), 188-200. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.012

Sanfeliu, M. C., & Fernandez, A. (1996). A set of 254 Snodgrass-Vanderwart pictures standardized for Spanish: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28(4), 537- 555. doi:10.3758/bf03200541

Schroder, A., Gemballa, T., Ruppin, S., & Wartenburger, I. (2012). German norms for semantic typicality, age of acquisition, and concept familiarity. Behav Res Methods, 44(2), 380- 394. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0164-y

Sebastián Gallés, N. (2000). Lexesp lÈxico informatizado del espaÒol, programa CORCO. Shelton, J. R., & Caramazza, A. (1999). Deficits in lexical and semantic processing:

164

Sirois, M., Kremin, H., & Cohen, H. (2006). Picture-naming norms for Canadian French: name agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, and age of acquisition. Behav Res Methods, 38(2), 300-306.

Smith, E. E. (1989). Concepts and induction. In Foundations of cognitive science. (pp. 501- 526). Cambridge, MA, US: The MIT Press.

Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn, 6(2), 174-215.

Soares, A. P., Costa, A. S., Machado, J., Comesana, M., & Oliveira, H. M. (2017). The Minho Word Pool: Norms for imageability, concreteness, and subjective frequency for 3,800 Portuguese words. Behav Res Methods, 49(3), 1065-1081. doi:10.3758/s13428-016- 0767-4

Soares, A. P., Pureza, R., & Comesana, M. (2018). Portuguese Norms of Name Agreement, Concept Familiarity, Subjective Frequency and Visual Complexity for 150 Colored and

Documents relatifs