• Aucun résultat trouvé

Cortical and Thalamic Excitation Mediate the Multiphasic Responses of Striatal Cholinergic Interneurons to Motivationally Salient Stimuli

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Cortical and Thalamic Excitation Mediate the Multiphasic Responses of Striatal Cholinergic Interneurons to Motivationally Salient Stimuli"

Copied!
18
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: hal-02329140

https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02329140

Submitted on 23 Oct 2019

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

Multiphasic Responses of Striatal Cholinergic Interneurons to Motivationally Salient Stimuli

Natalie Doig, Peter Magill, Paul Apicella, J. Bolam, Andrew Sharott

To cite this version:

Natalie Doig, Peter Magill, Paul Apicella, J. Bolam, Andrew Sharott. Cortical and Thalamic Ex- citation Mediate the Multiphasic Responses of Striatal Cholinergic Interneurons to Motivationally Salient Stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, Society for Neuroscience, 2014, 34 (8), pp.3101-3117.

�10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4627-13.2014�. �hal-02329140�

(2)

Systems/Circuits

Cortical and Thalamic Excitation Mediate the Multiphasic Responses of Striatal Cholinergic Interneurons to

Motivationally Salient Stimuli

Natalie M. Doig,1Peter J. Magill,1Paul Apicella,2J. Paul Bolam,1and Andrew Sharott1

1Medical Research Council Anatomical Neuropharmacology Unit, Department of Pharmacology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3TH, United Kingdom;

and2Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique-Aix-Marseille Universite´, 13005 Marseille, France

Cholinergic interneurons are key components of striatal microcircuits. In primates, tonically active neurons (putative cholinergic in- terneurons) exhibit multiphasic responses to motivationally salient stimuli that mirror those of midbrain dopamine neurons and together these two systems mediate reward-related learning in basal ganglia circuits. Here, we addressed the potential contribution of cortical and thalamic excitatory inputs to the characteristic multiphasic responses of cholinergic interneuronsin vivo. We first recorded and labeled individual cholinergic interneurons in anesthetized rats. Electron microscopic analyses of these labeled neurons demon- strated that an individual interneuron could form synapses with cortical and, more commonly, thalamic afferents. Single-pulse electrical stimulation of ipsilateral frontal cortex led to robust short-latency (20 ms) interneuron spiking, indicating monosynaptic connectivity, but firing probability progressively decreased during high-frequency pulse trains. In contrast, single-pulse thalamic stimulation led to weak short-latency spiking, but firing probability increased during pulse trains. After initial excitation from cortex or thalamus, interneu- rons displayed a “pause” in firing, followed by a “rebound” increase in firing rate. Across all stimulation protocols, the number of spikes in the initial excitation correlated positively with pause duration and negatively with rebound magnitude. The magnitude of the initial excitation, therefore, partly determined the profile of later components of multiphasic responses. Upon examining the responses of tonically active neurons in behaving primates, we found that these correlations held true for unit responses to a reward-predicting stimulus, but not to the reward alone, delivered outside of any task. We conclude that excitatory inputs determine, at least in part, the multiphasic responses of cholinergic interneurons under specific behavioral conditions.

Key words: basal ganglia; corticostriatal; parafascicular nucleus; thalamostriatal; tonically active neuron

Introduction

The striatum is the principal site of integration of cortical and thalamic information in the basal ganglia. Although medium- sized spiny projection neurons (MSNs) are the main target of both cortical and thalamic projections (Somogyi et al., 1981;

Dube´ et al., 1988;Doig et al., 2010), certain types of interneurons are selectively targeted by cortical and/or thalamic afferents, so these glutamatergic inputs can powerfully influence the output of the striatum (Mallet et al., 2005;Ding et al., 2010). Cholinergic

interneurons are prime candidates for mediating the effects of excitatory inputs to striatum and have been studied extensively in relation to reinforcement learning through their putative identifica- tion as “tonically active neurons” (TANs) in behaving animals.

TANs are particularly sensitive to motivationally salient stimuli, usu- ally responding with a pause in their tonic firing that can be preceded by short-latency firing and/or followed by a “rebound” increase in spiking (Morris et al., 2004;Joshua et al., 2008). The timing of these responses is entwined with those of midbrain dopamine neurons to the same stimuli that encode reward value (Morris et al., 2004).

These coordinated systems modulate the timing of reinforcement learning in the striatum and thus shape the behavioral output of the basal ganglia (Apicella, 2007).

The mechanisms underlying the responses of cholinergic interneurons to motivationally salient stimuli are unclear.

GABAergic inputs from MSNs (Gonzales et al., 2013) could initiate the pause, but physiological evidence for such a connec- tion is lacking. Therefore, GABAergic interneurons have also been implicated (Sullivan et al., 2008). Furthermore, the intrinsic membrane properties and autonomous firing of cholinergic neu- rons mean that a brief barrage of synaptic excitation can produce complex changes in their firing patterns, including pauses in con- tinuous firing (Oswald et al., 2009;Ding et al., 2010). Both ana-

Received Oct. 28, 2013; revised Jan. 17, 2014; accepted Jan. 23, 2014.

Author contributions: N.M.D., P.J.M., J.P.B., and A.S. designed research; N.M.D., P.A., and A.S. performed re- search; A.S. analyzed data; N.M.D. and A.S. wrote the paper.

This work was supported by the Medical Research Council UK (Grants U138197109 and U138164490), Parkinson’s UK (Grant G-0806), a Marie Curie European Re-integration Grant (SNAP-PD) awarded by the European Union, and an International Joint Project Grant (Grant JP090457) awarded by The Royal Society. N.M.D. was in receipt of a Medical Research Council studentship. We thank J. Morgan for the generous gift of antibodies against Cerebellin-1; G. Hazell, B. Micklem, L. Norman, K. Whitworth, and C. Johnson for technical support; I. Huerta-Ocampo and K. Nakamura for advice and assistance with immunohistochemistry; F. Vinciati for contributing to electrophysiological recordings;

members of the Apicella laboratory for help with TAN data collection; and P. Henny for assistance with EM analysis and helpful comments on the manuscript.

Correspondence should be addressed to Andrew Sharott, MRC Anatomical Neuropharmacology Unit, Department of Pharmacology, Mansfield Road, Oxford OX1 3TH, United Kingdom. E-mail: andrew.sharott@pharm.ox.ac.uk.

DOI:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4627-13.2014

Copyright © 2014 the authors 0270-6474/14/343101-17$15.00/0

(3)

tomical (Lapper and Bolam, 1992) and physiological (Ding et al., 2010;Threlfell et al., 2012) evidence suggests that a major source of this synaptic excitation are the projections from the intralami- nar thalamic nuclei (ITN). Indeed, thalamic input appears nec- essary for the pause response of TANs in nonhuman primates (Matsumoto et al., 2001).In vivo, electrical stimulation of cortex has also been shown to excite cholinergic interneurons at short latencies, suggesting a monosynaptic connection (Reynolds and Wickens, 2004;Sharott et al., 2012), but there is little anatomical evidence to suggest direct cortical innervation (Lapper and Bo- lam, 1992;Dimova et al., 1993).

In this study, we demonstrate that the same cholinergic interneu- ron can form synapses with both cortical and thalamic afferents and that driving these excitatory synapses can lead to distinctive effects on interneuron firing properties. The magnitude of the initial exci- tation in these interneurons predicts the temporal profile of the sub- sequent response components. Finally, we show that this correlation between initial excitation and the profile of the multiphasic response is present in TANs responding to specific motivationally relevant stimuli in behaving primates.

Materials and Methods

Electrophysiological recordings in rats

Experimental procedures were performed on adult male Sprague Dawley rats (Charles River Laboratories) and were conducted in accordance with the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act of 1986 (United Kingdom) and with Society for Neuroscience Policies on the Use of Animals in Neuro- science Research.

Recording and labeling experiments were performed in 27 anesthe- tized rats (280 –340 g). Briefly, anesthesia was induced with 4% v/v iso- flurane in O2,and maintained with urethane (1.3 g/kg ethyl carbamate, i.p.; Sigma-Aldrich) and supplemental doses of ketamine (30 mg/kg Ke- taset, i.p.; Willows Francis) and xylazine (3 mg/kg Rompun, i.p.; Bayer), as described previously (Magill et al., 2006;Sharott et al., 2012). Wound margins were infiltrated with local anesthetic (0.5% w/v bupivacaine;

AstraZeneca). Animals were then placed in a stereotaxic frame (Kopf).

Body temperature was maintained at 370.5°C with a homoeothermic heating device (Harvard Apparatus). Electrocorticogram (ECoG) read- ings, electrocardiographic activity, and respiration rate were monitored constantly to ensure the animals’ wellbeing. The epidural ECoG was recorded above the frontal (somatic sensory-motor) cortex (4.2 mm anterior and 2.0 mm lateral of bregma;Paxinos and Watson, 1986) and was referenced against the ipsilateral cerebellar hemisphere (Mallet et al., 2008). Raw ECoG was band-pass filtered (0.3–1500 Hz,3 dB limits) and amplified (2000; DPA-2FS filter/amplifier; NPI Electronic) before acquisition. Extracellular recordings of the action potentials (“spikes”) of individual neurons (i.e., single-unit activities) in the dorsal striatum were made using glass electrodes (10 –30 Min situ; tip diameter1.2m) containing 0.5MNaCl solution and neurobiotin (NB; 1.5% w/v; Vector Laboratories). Electrodes were lowered into the brain under stereotaxic guidance and using a computer-controlled stepper motor (IVM-1000;

Scientifica) that allowed the electrode depth to be determined with a resolution of 0.1m. Electrode signals were amplified 10 times through the bridge circuitry of an Axoprobe-1A amplifier (Molecular Devices), AC coupled, amplified another 100 times, and band-pass filtered at 300 – 5000 Hz (DPA-2FS filter/amplifier). The ECoG and single-unit activity were each sampled at 16.6 kHz using a Power1401 analog-digital con- verter and a PC running Spike2 acquisition and analysis software (Ver- sion 7.2; Cambridge Electronic Design). After electrophysiological recordings, single neurons were juxtacellularly labeled with NB (Magill et al., 2000). Briefly, positive current pulses (2–10 nA, 200 ms, 50% duty cycle) were applied until the single-unit activity became robustly en- trained by the current injections. After the recording and labeling ses- sions, the animals were given a lethal dose of ketamine (150 mg/kg) and were perfused transcardially with 100 ml of 0.05MPBS, pH 7.4, followed by 300 ml of 0.1% w/v glutaraldehyde and 4% w/v paraformaldehyde in 0.1Mphosphate buffer, pH 7.4 (PB), and then by 200 ml of 4% w/v

paraformaldehyde in PB. Brains were then left in the latter fixative solu- tion at 4°C until sectioning 24 –72 h later.

Electrical stimulation of the cortex and thalamus. Parallel, bipolar stim- ulating electrodes (constructed from nylon-coated stainless steel wires;

California Fine Wire) with tip diameters of100m, a tip separation of

150m, and an impedance of10 kwere implanted into the ipsi- lateral frontal cortex (Magill et al., 2004) and ipsilateral intralaminar thalamus. The coordinates of the cortical stimulation sites (2.5– 4.0 mm anterior and 2.5–3.0 mm lateral of bregma, at depths of 2.0 –2.2 mm below the dura) correspond to layers 5/6 of the primary/secondary motor cortices (Paxinos and Watson, 1986). For thalamic stimulation, we used coordinates (3.7 mm posterior and 1.3 mm lateral of bregma, at depths of 5.4 –5.7 mm below the dura) that consistently led to an electrode track in the parafascicular nucleus (Pfn). Electrical stimuli, which consisted of square-wave current pulses of 0.3 ms duration and variable amplitude (100 –900A), were delivered using a constant-current isolator (A360D;

World Precision Instruments) that was gated by digital outputs from the Power1401 converter. Different stimulation protocols were applied to cortex and then thalamus or vice versa depending on the response of the individual neuron: (1) a single-pulse stimulation delivered to cortex/

thalamus, (2) a paired-pulse stimulus given with a 100 ms interstimulus interval, and (3) a train of 5 stimuli given with a 25 ms interstimulus interval. Regardless of the type of stimulation (single, paired, or 40 Hz train), each was given at 2 s intervals.

Tissue processing for identification of recorded and labeled neurons. Para- sagittal sections (50m) were cut from each brain using a vibrating microtome (VT1000S; Leica Microsystems), collected in series, and washed in PBS. Free-floating sections were then incubated overnight at room temperature in Triton PBS (PBS containing 0.3% v/v Triton X-100; Sigma-Aldrich) containing Cy3-conjugated streptavidin (1:1000 dilution; Life Technologies). Sections containing NB-labeled neuronal somata (those marked with Cy3) were then isolated for molecular char- acterization by indirect immunofluorescence (Sharott et al., 2012). Neu- rons with densely spiny higher-order dendrites were classified as projection neurons (MSNs) and were thus not tested for interneuron markers. Aspiny neurons were tested for expression of immunoreactivity for choline acetyltransferase (ChAT). Briefly, after 1–2 h of incubation in Triton PBS containing 10% v/v normal donkey serum (NDS; Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories), sections containing NB-labeled neuro- nal somata were incubated in Triton PBS containing 2% v/v NDS and a primary antibody against ChAT overnight at room temperature (goat- anti-ChAT, catalog # AB144P, 1:500; Millipore Bioscience Research Re- agents). A fluorescent secondary antibody was then applied (Alexa Fluor 488; donkey-anti-goat; 1:000; Jackson ImmunoResearch) for4 h. Only NB-labeled neurons expressing ChAT were included in this study. To determine the placement of the thalamic stimulating electrode, an anti- body against cerebellin 1 (rabbit anti-CBLN 1, 1:3000; a gift from J.

Morgan, St. Jude Children’s Research Hospital) with heat pretreatment as a means of antigen retrieval (Jiao et al., 1999) was used on sections in which the electrode track was apparent. Cerebellin 1 is a selective marker of the Pfn compared with neighboring structures (Kusnoor et al., 2010);

in some cases, this was combined with Nissl staining to delineate the thalamic nuclei (NeuroTrace, catalog #N21479, 1:100; Invitrogen). Judg- ing from the robust responses of interneurons that we obtained and the anatomical verifications, it is likely that both poles of the stimulating electrodes were located in, or very close to, the Pfn. Despite this, we cannot rule out the possibility that the evoked electric field was not confined to the Pfn, so we conservatively refer to stimulation as being

“thalamic.”

Electron microscopy

Immunohistochemistry. Four recorded and labeled cholinergic interneu- rons from four rats were processed for electron microscopy (EM). The first neuron was a pilot study to establish the immunohistochemical and electron microscopic protocols. The four neurons used for EM analysis were positive for ChAT. Sections containing neuronal processes (lateral and medial to the section containing the soma) were isolated for EM processing. Sections were incubated in cryoprotectant (25% sucrose, 10% glycerol) overnight at 4°C. Sections (one at a time) were then freeze

(4)

thawed using liquid nitrogen. Each section was laid flat in one well of a six-well plate and excess cryoprotectant was removed using filter paper.

The well plate was then lowered over liquid nitrogen until the section froze and went opaque. The section was then rapidly thawed using cryo- protectant that had been kept at room temperature. The sections were then incubated in cryoprotectant for at least 1 h at room temperature or until all sections had sunk to the bottom of the glass vial. The freezing process was then repeated for all of the sections and the sections were thawed with room temperature 0.1MPB. All of the sections were then washed in 0.1MPB twice and three times in PBS and incubated in PBS containing 10% NDS for 1 h at room temperature on a shaker. The sections were then incubated in avidin-biotin-peroxidase complex (ABC, 1:250; Vector Labs) prepared according to the manufacturer’s instructions in a 1% NDS PBS solution.

After incubation in ABC, the sections were washed 3 times in PBS, followed by 3 washes in 0.1MPB, pH 6.0. A peroxidase reaction using tetramethylbenzidine (TMB) as the chromogen was then performed to reveal the NB-filled neuron (Doig et al., 2010). Sections were placed for 20 min in a preincubation solution containing the following: 80 ml of 0.1

MPB, pH 6.0, 4 ml of ammonium paratungstate (1% in deionized H2O), 1 ml of TMB (catalog # T5525, 0.2% dissolved in absolute ethanol;

Sigma-Aldrich), 800 ␮l of ammonium chloride (0.4% in deionized H2O), and 800␮l ofD-glucose (20% in deionized H2O). To perform the reaction, the preincubation solution was removed and replaced with the reaction solution, which consisted of 2 ml of the preincubation solution (as above) plus 2l of glucose oxidase (catalog #G6891; Sigma-Aldrich).

The reaction was initially performed on the section containing the cell body to gauge the optimum processing time; the section was wet mounted and checked under a light microscope (40). The reaction was then allowed to progress for all sections for between 6 and 8 min depend- ing on the neuron. The reaction was stopped with 0.1MPB, pH 6.0.

Sections were then washed 3 times for 5 min with 0.1MPB, pH 6.0. The reaction was then stabilized by incubation in a solution containing the following: 400l of ammonium chloride (0.4% in deionized H2O), 400

l ofD-glucose (20% in deionized H2O), 800l of cobalt (II) chloride (1% in deionized H2O), and 40 mg of DAB dissolved in 40 ml of 0.1MPB, pH 6.0. The stabilization solution was filtered before use. Sections were incubated for 15 min at room temperature. The stabilization solution was then removed and replaced with 3 10 min washes of 0.1MPB, pH 6.0.

During stabilization, the blue-staining color from the reaction step changes to a darker magenta color and background staining is reduced.

Sections were then washed 3 times in 0.1MPB, pH 7.4.

Alternate sections were then processed to reveal either VGluT1 or VGluT2 labeling using the peroxidase-anti-peroxidase (PAP) method (Bolam, 1992). Alternate sections were incubated in a primary antibody against either VGluT1 (rabbit-anti-VGluT1, catalog #VGT3, 1:2000;

MAb Tech) or VGluT2 (rabbit-anti-VGluT2, catalog #, 135403, 1:2000;

Synaptic Systems) overnight at room temperature in PBS. The sections were then incubated in an un-conjugated antibody (donkey-anti-rabbit IgG, catalog #711-035-152, 1:100; Jackson ImmunoResearch) for at least 4 h at room temperature. Next, the sections were incubated in a PAP complex (rabbit PAP, catalog #323-005-024, 1:400; Jackson ImmunoRe- search) for at least 4 h at room temperature. A peroxidase reaction using DAB as the chromogen was used to reveal VGluT1 or VGlut2 labeling.

Sections were incubated in a solution containing DAB (0.05%) dissolved in TRIS buffer for 20 min. Hydrogen peroxidase (H2O2) was then added to the solution to achieve a final concentration of 0.01%. The reaction was monitored using a dissection microscope. All sections were incu- bated for between 3 and 4 min. They were then washed in Tris buffer 3 times, followed by 3 washes in 0.1MPB, pH 7.4.

Sections were then post-fixed in osmium tetroxide (1% w/v in PB;

Oxkem) for 25 min and then washed in 0.1MPB and dehydrated in an ascending series of ethanol dilutions as follows: 15 min in 50% w/v eth- anol, 35 min in 70% ethanol that included 1% w/v uranyl acetate, 15 min in 95% ethanol, and twice for 15 min in absolute ethanol. After absolute ethanol, sections were washed twice in propylene oxide (Sigma-Aldrich) for 15 min and placed into resin (Durcupan ACM; Fluka) and left over- night at room temperature. Sections were then placed on microscope slides, a coverslip was applied, and the resin was cured at 65°C for72 h.

Reconstruction and ultrathin sectioning. The 3D reconstruction of NB- labeled neurons was performed using Neurolucida (MBF) using an oil- immersion objective (60). The tracings within the separate sections were then spliced using Neurolucida and the final version was corrected for shrinkage in all dimensions:x(6.3%),y(6.0%), andz(8%;Sadek et al., 2007). A file with the entire neuron unspliced was saved to be used as a guide for resectioning dendritic fragments. Quantitative data from the reconstructions was then obtained using Neuroexplorer (MBF). For the Sholl analysis, radius segments of 50␮m were used. The data were then exported to Microsoft Excel (2007).

Light microscopic images of the dendrites of each neuron (20) were aligned with the reconstruction to identify specific dendritic fragments in each sagittal section. At least one dendritic fragment was reembedded from each sagittal section from the “top” of the section. Ultrathin sec- tions (50 nm) containing the pieces of dendrite were then resectioned using an ultramicrotome (EM UC6; Leica Microsystems) and collected on pioloform-coated single slot grids. The sections were then contrasted using lead citrate.

Electron microscopy and analysis. The dendritic fragment(s) within each ultrathin section was then imaged using an electron microscope (CM10; Philips) at a high magnification (13,500to 46,000). Each dendritic fragment was analyzed through several grids and in serial sec- tion on each grid, with a minimum of five sections analyzed per grid. The major factor limiting the extent of dendrite that could be analyzed was the penetration of the VGluT antibodies. To control for the possibility of VGluT-false-negative terminals, serial sections on a grid were only exam- ined if there was at least one VGluT1/2-positive profile present in one of the sections on the grid (at a magnification of at least 13,500).

Electron micrographs were then analyzed using ImageJ software (Ver- sion 1.41o). Every synapse formed with the sections of dendrites exam- ined was analyzed and every presynaptic terminal forming a symmetric (type II) or an asymmetric (type I) synapse with the labeled dendrite was recorded. The length of the postsynaptic density for every synapse formed with the cholinergic dendrite was measured. Synaptic density was calculated as the number of synapses divided by the length of dendrite (in micrometers) examined (in thez-axis, from the number of ultrathin sections analyzed) and then expressed as density per 10m.

Estimations of the total number of synapses onto the dendrites of cholinergic interneurons were calculated as follows, based on data ac- quired. To estimate the total number of synapses, the average height examined for each section was calculated based on the number of ultra- thin sections examined and the number of synapses found within that height was obtained from the data. For example, for neuron NJX009, the average height per section examined was 2.12m (out of a potential 45

␮m of section thickness) and a total of 90 synapses (symmetric, asym- metric, and VGluT2 positive) were counted within this thickness. The number of synapses was then multiplied by a correction factor to esti- mate the total number of synapses as if the entire height of the section was analyzed. For NJX009, there were 12 sections, each with a thickness of 45

m (after shrinkage), so the number of synapses (90) was thus multiplied by 20.2 (1818.13). The total number of high endings of dendrite for each neuron was counted from the unspliced Neurolucida files. For NJX009, a possible total of 78 high endings could have been sampled and, out of this, 20 were examined. A correction factor was then used to estimate what the number of synapses would be had every high ending been sampled. In the case of NJX009, this correction factor was 3.9, so the predicted number of synapses for NJX009 is 7090.70 (1818.13*3.9). This method is based on the protocol established by Henny et al. (2012,2013).

We chose to use this method because it is based entirely on the EM ultrastructural data and does not take into account dendritic length, which decreases the error, for the following reasons: (1) the dendritic length would be estimated from the Neurolucida files (and not the ultra- thin sections) and (2) by using this method, the orientation of the den- drite within the section is not incorporated. The method was then repeated to estimate the total number of symmetric, asymmetric, and VGluT1 or VGluT2 synapses for each neuron. Numbers of VGluT1 and VGluT2 synapses were doubled to take into account the fact that the sampling was only performed in one of two series.

(5)

Electrophysiological recordings of TANs in behaving primates Single neurons were recorded from the striatum of four behaving ma- caque monkeys (identified as C, P, G, and B). The monkeys were seated in a restraining box that was described previously (Apicella et al., 1997) and faced a panel placed30 cm in front of them. Each was chronically fitted with a head-restraining device and a recording chamber over a craniot- omy for electrode insertions mainly targeted at the putamen. Surgical and electrophysiological procedures were as described previously (Api- cella et al., 1997). Monkey surgery and behavioral testing protocols were in accordance with guidelines set by the National Institutes of Health and the French government regulations on animal experimentation. Neu- rons were accessed on vertical penetrations with glass-coated tungsten electrodes. The electrodes were advanced with a hydraulic microdrive (MO-95; Narishige) through a stainless steel guide tube that was used to penetrate the dura. Signals from neuronal activity were conventionally amplified, filtered (band pass, 0.3–1.5 kHz), and converted to digital pulses through a window discriminator. Putative cholinergic interneu- rons (TANs) were classified according to their electrophysiological char- acteristics, as described previously (Aosaki et al., 1994), as well as their typical responses to unexpected rewarding stimuli (Apicella et al., 1997).

Our focus on TANs recorded in putamen was not only in keeping with many other past studies of salience-/reward-related TAN activity in be- having primates (Aosaki et al., 1994;Apicella et al., 1997;Morris et al., 2004), but also allowed for the most direct comparison (in terms of broadly equivalent circuits and function) with our sample of rat cholin- ergic interneurons. Although there is some regional bias in the responses of primate striatal neurons to salient/rewarding stimuli, uniformity in TAN responses across the striatal axis has been commonly emphasized (Morris et al., 2004;Adler et al., 2013a;Adler et al., 2013b).

The activity of TANs were studied with respect to two behavioral conditions. In the first condition, a Pavlovian protocol in which the onset of a visual stimulus on the center of the panel at unpredicted times was followed, after a 1 s delay, by the delivery of liquid reward (0.3 ml of apple juice) via a tube positioned directly in front of the monkey’s mouth. All monkeys were highly experienced with the associative relationship be- tween stimulus and reward; that is, the monkey used the stimulus as a predictor of the upcoming reward, as reflected by licking movements starting before reward delivery. We will refer to this situation as the reward-predicting stimulus condition. In the second condition, the same liquid reward was repeatedly delivered at irregular time intervals (5.5–

8.5 s) in the absence of any predictive stimulus. We will refer to this situation as the reward-only condition. The two conditions were pre- sented as separate blocks of 30 – 40 trials each, the order of blocks being chosen randomly. In our previous work, we have demonstrated that the TAN response to a stimulus that predicts reward is paralleled by a lack of responsiveness to the reward itself (after Pavlovian conditioning), whereas most TANs remain responsive to unexpected deliveries of re- ward outside of any task (for review, seeApicella, 2007).

Our database consisted of 79 TANs recorded in the reward-predicting stimulus condition (26 from C, 25 from P, and 28 from G) and 130 TANs recorded in the reward-only condition (23 from C, 10 from G, and 97 from B). Eleven TANs were recorded in both conditions. The behavioral situa- tions, recording methods, and TAN responses properties have been de- scribed previously (Ravel et al., 2003;Apicella et al., 2011;Deffains et al., 2010).

Data analysis

Analysis of short-latency responses of rat cholinergic interneurons to afferent stimulation. Peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) were constructed from 50 –200 consecutive stimulation trials with 2 ms bins and normal- ized to give firing probability (spikes per bin/trials). Cholinergic in- terneurons were considered to respond significantly at “short latency” if 20% of first spikes after the stimulus were fired in a response window of 1.5–20 ms and if, within this window, there was a histogram peak3 SDs of the prestimulus (“baseline”) firing probability (defined from400 to

100 ms before stimulation at 0 ms). Neurons were considered to re- spond significantly at “long latency” if 20% of first spikes after the stim- ulus were fired in a response window of 20 –50 ms and if there was a peak

3 SDs of the baseline in this window. The first spike in each stimulus

trial, rather than the peak of the response in the PSTH, was therefore used to measure the response latency. The mean latencies of the first spikes in a given time window were compared using a Mann–Whitney U test or Wilcoxon signed-rank test as appropriate. The same short-latency anal- ysis windows were also used for both paired-pulse and high-frequency train stimulation. For paired-pulse stimulation, latencies and firing probabilities across the first and second pulse were compared using Wil- coxon signed-rank tests as long as there were5 spikes in this window in response to both pulses. Because our high-frequency stimulation cycle was at 40 Hz, we were able to perform the same analysis as for single stimulation within each 25 ms interval of the high-frequency stimulation pulse. Mean latency and firing probability were therefore calculated for each of the five pulses in the stimulation train. The cumulative sum of the firing probability across the five pulses and its slope was calculated to measure the magnitude of accumulated spiking through the stimulus train.

Characterization of multiphasic responses of rats and primate neurons using PSTHs. We designed a unified framework to characterize the mul- tiphasic responses of both identified rat cholinergic interneurons to cortical/thalamic stimulation in anesthetized rats and of TANs to behav- iorally relevant stimuli (reward-predicting cues or reward delivery with no cue) in monkeys. This involved a two-step approach in which we first calculated the overall temporal profile and established the class of re- sponse (any combination of initial excitation/pause/rebound) of each neuron based on its PSTH and then used this information to calculate parameters of the three phases for each trial.

First, a smoothed PSTH (50 ms bins with 90% overlap) was computed to define the response phases. After these PSTHs were normalized as a percentage of baseline firing, peaks and troughs were detected using a threshold of five contiguous bins over or under the 95% confidence interval of the 400 ms prestimulus baseline firing in the PSTH (from

450 to50 ms for rats, from500 to100 ms for monkeys). In both the rat and monkey datasets, the most elaborate population responses included early increases in firing rate (from here on defined as an “initial excitation”), followed by a decrease in or cessation of firing (“pause”), followed by a renewed increase in firing (“rebound”), as described pre- viously (Schulz and Reynolds, 2013). Significant peaks and troughs in PSTHs were thus assigned to initial excitation, pause, and/or rebound phases based on their latencies compared with the population response and these previous investigations. For single-pulse stimulation in rats, the temporal boundaries of the response phases were as follows: initial excitation: 1–50 ms; pause: 50 – 400 ms; and rebound: 200 –700 ms. For high-frequency train stimulation in rats, the boundaries were as follows:

initial excitation: 1–150 ms (after the first pulse in the train); pause:

50 – 400 ms; and rebound: 200 –700 ms. Therefore, for responses evoked by high-frequency electrical stimulation, the phase of initial excitation encompassed all spiking after the first pulse up until 25 ms after the last pulse. For primate TAN responses to behaviorally relevant stimuli, the boundaries were as follows: initial excitation: 10 –200 ms (after the reward-predicting stimulus or reward delivery); pause: 75–350 ms; and rebound: 200 –700 ms. Note that the windows for the three phases are wide and overlapping to allow for variations in the temporal profile across neurons and jitter in the response latencies. Significant responses were defined as such when they were detected at any time within these boundaries. This is particularly relevant for the primate data in which the latencies for each phase were longer for the reward-only condition. Based on the combination of peaks and troughs displayed, neurons responses were assigned to one of eight categories: Unclassified, Initial excitation only, Pause only, Rebound only, Initial excitation/Pause, Initial excitation/

Pause/Rebound, Initial excitation/Rebound, and Pause/Rebound. The smoothed PSTHs were also used to define the start and end of each response phase. Every PSTH was extensively scrutinized to verify that these boundar- ies and other criteria led to satisfactory response classification.

Trial-by-trial analysis of multiphasic responses of neurons in rats and primates. Having defined the response class and the timing of each PSTH response phase for each neuron in rat and monkey, we used these data to analyze the relationship between the different response phases on a trial- by-trial basis. The PSTH response classification allowed us to establish that the majority of rat and monkey neurons displayed a pause (see Results). Therefore, for each neuron that had a pause in the PSTH, we

(6)

used the pause onset, defined by the first bin in the PSTH that fell below the 95% confidence interval, as a reference point to calculate a value for the number of spikes in the preceding initial excitation, the duration of the pause itself, and the change in firing rate during the subsequent rebound phase for every stimulus trial.

Initial excitation. This initial excitation phase for each trial was defined as the number of spikes fired between the “trigger” (the first electrical stimulation in rat data or the visual cue or reward delivery in primate data) and 50 ms after the start of the pause onset (to allow for single trials in which the initial excitation lasted longer that the PSTH pause onset) as defined by the PSTH. Trials could then be separated based on how many spikes were fired in this initial excitation phase and PSTHs recalculated to include only the trials with a given number of spikes in this window. For group statistics, the number of spikes in each trial was averaged for each individual rat cholinergic interneuron or primate TAN.

Pause. The pause phase for each trial was defined as the interspike interval between the last spike of the initial excitation phase and the next spike. For trials in which no spikes were fired in the initial excitation window (i.e., no activity up to 50 ms after the start of the PSTH pause), the start of the pause for that trial was the start of the PSTH pause (i.e., the first bin in the PSTH to be significantly below the baseline). The individ- ual pause values for each trial were normalized by the mean interspike interval of the baseline period for the entire set of trials (Ding et al., 2010).

For group statistics, the mean pause for each neuron was the mean pause/

trial divided by the mean interspike interval during baseline firing.

Therefore, a value of1 indicates that the pause interval is greater than the mean interspike interval of the baseline.

Rebound. The rebound phase for each trial was defined as the firing rate in the 500 ms after the pause interval for that trial (including the spike that defined the pause offset) normalized by the mean firing rate of the baseline period. Therefore, the “rebound rate” was the percentage change in the firing rate of the 500 ms after the pause compared with the baseline rate.

This trial-by-trial approach had several advantages compared with analyzing responses based on the PSTH. First, the calculation of these values was not directly dependent on the binning of data for the PSTH.

Second, in the cases for which there was no initial excitation or rebound peak in the PSTH, it was still possible to calculate the values for these response phases and to detect single trials that did have increases in spiking during these phases. Third, by defining values for each response phase in each trial, we could isolate and sort trials based on specific characteristics (e.g., one, two, or three spikes in the initial excitation phase/sorting trials by pause length). Finally, using these methods en- abled us to focus our analyses on changes in firing rate within a trial, not consistency in latency with respect to the stimulus across trials. Such changes are less dependent on binning and likely to be more relevant to the online computation performed by the neuron. In summary, our analyses enabled us to investigate, within the same analytical framework, the multiphasic responses of both rat cholinergic interneurons after elec- trical stimulation and monkey TANs after rewarding stimuli.

Results

Cortical and thalamic inputs to individual cholinergic interneurons in the rat

Although cholinergic interneurons only make up a small propor- tion of striatal neurons, it is widely accepted that these interneu- rons play an important role in reward-related behavior through the modulatory actions of the acetylcholine that they release within the striatum. In the first part of this study, cholinergic interneurons were recorded in the striatum of anesthetized rats.

Online identification of putative cholinergic interneurons was facilitated by previous work in urethane-anesthetized rats (Sha- rott et al., 2012) showing that, regardless of brain state, cholin- ergic interneurons fire spontaneously at similar rates (3– 6 Hz;

Fig. 1A). After the recording of spontaneous activity, the same interneurons were then recorded during the delivery (in random order) of a variety of electrical stimuli to the cortex (ipsilateral

motor cortex) and/or thalamus (nominally targeted to ipsilateral parafascicular nucleus). An individual cholinergic interneuron could fire at short latencies (⬍20 ms) in response to single-pulse stimulation of both cortical and thalamic sites (Fig. 1B,C). The interneuron shown inFigure 1responded to cortical stimulation with an average delay to first spike of 9.1 ms (Fig. 1B) and to thalamic stimulation with an average delay of 12.7 ms (Fig. 1C).

These and similar short-latency excitation responses were as- sumed to reflect activation of monosynaptic inputs. Note, how- ever, that the responses of interneurons to single-pulse afferent stimulation were often multiphasic. Therefore, short-latency excita- tions were often followed by (in order) a cessation of firing and a subsequent rebound increase in firing (Fig. 1B,C). After recording and electrical stimulation, interneurons were juxtacellularly labeled with NB (Fig. 1D) and subsequently tested for the expression of ChAT immunoreactivity (Fig. 1E). All rat neurons included in this study were confirmed to express ChAT and were thus unequivocally identified as cholinergic interneurons.

The synaptic innervation of three physiologically and neuro- chemically characterized cholinergic interneurons was examined at the EM level. The somata and dendrites of these cholinergic interneurons were first digitally reconstructed in 3D (Fig. 1F).

Alternate tissue sections were incubated in antibodies against ei- ther vesicular glutamate transporter 1 or 2 (VGluT1 or VGluT2) to quantify inputs from cortex and thalamus, respectively (Doig et al., 2010;Henny et al., 2012;Henny et al., 2013) and then processed for EM. One identified cholinergic interneuron (#AJS044) that ex- hibited robust responses to cortical and thalamic stimulationin vivo (the same neuron as shown throughoutFig. 1) was shown to form synapses with axons originating in both the cortex (VGluT1 positive;

Fig. 1G) and the thalamus (VGluT2 positive;Fig. 1H). This estab- lishes the precedent that an individual cholinergic interneuron can form synapses with axon terminals arising fromboththe cortex and thalamus. Further details of the synaptic innervation of cholinergic interneurons are discussed below.

Synaptic innervation of cholinergic interneurons

A detailed anatomical examination of the synaptic inputs to three identified cholinergic interneurons was performed using EM.

The NB-filled neurons were first reconstructed in 3D before re- embedding and resectioning for EM (Henny et al., 2012;Henny et al., 2013). In agreement with previous studies, cholinergic in- terneurons had distinctly large cell bodies, and between three and six primary dendrites that extend in a radial pattern from the soma for up to⬃700␮m (e.g.,Fig. 1F;Wilson et al., 1990;Inokawa et al., 2010;Sharott et al., 2012). The average total dendritic length for the three neurons reconstructed was 9411␮m (⫾511).

After reconstruction, fragments of dendrites from all parts of the dendritic arbor were sectioned for analysis in the electron microscope. For each neuron, serial sections of 20 –24 dendritic fragments were examined in tissue labeled for either VGluT1 or VGluT2 (Table 1). Dendritic fragments were examined in serial sections in tissue that exhibited staining for VGluT1 or VGluT2.

On average, 42 sections were analyzed for each of the dendritic fragments, giving an overall average of 892 sections per neuron (Table 1). All terminals forming synapses with the dendrites were noted (Fig. 2, Table 1) and categorized as follows: (1) DAB- negative (unlabeled) terminals forming asymmetric (Gray’s type I) synapses (Fig. 2A–C,Table 1); (2) DAB-negative (unlabeled) terminals forming symmetric (Gray’s type II) synapses (Fig. 2D, Table 1); and (3) DAB-positive (i.e., VGluT1-or VGluT2- immunopositive) terminals forming asymmetric synapses (Figs.

1E,F,2B,Table 1).

(7)

Overall, 62% of the terminals in contact with the dendrites of cholinergic interneurons formed symmetric synapses (Fig. 2E) and 38% formed asymmetric synapses (p⫽0.05; Mann–Whit- ney U test, two-tailed, Fig. 2E). Of the terminals that formed asymmetric synapses, 1.7% were positive for VGluT1 and 13%

for VGluT2 (Fig. 2E, inset). The remainder of terminals forming asymmetric synapses were DAB negative in tissue labeled for ei- ther VGluT1 (49.8⫾3.4%) or VGluT2 (35.3⫾2.8%;Fig. 2E, inset). Therefore, the predominant type of synapse formed with the dendrites of cholinergic interneurons was symmetric and formed by unlabeled terminals.

We then examined the distribution of synapses formed with the cholinergic neurons in different parts of the dendritic arbor (Fig. 2F,Table 2). Dendrites were defined as “proximal” to the soma (i.e., within the first 20% of the total length from the soma of the longest dendrite;Henny et al., 2012;Henny et al., 2013),

“distal” (21– 80%); or “most distal” (the farthest 20% of the dis- tance of dendrites from the soma;Table 2). The number and type of terminals forming synapses was examined within each com- partment (Table 2). The number of terminals forming asymmet- ric or symmetric synapses was normalized based on the length of dendrite within each compartment for each dendritic fragment (Fig. 2F). Of the two terminals positive for VGluT1, one was on a proximal dendrite and the other on a distal dendrite (Table 2).

Synapses formed by terminals positive for VGluT2 were found in all dendritic compartments (Table 2).

Overall, the average density of terminals forming asymmetric synapses per 10␮m of dendrite was 9.9⫾1.1 compared with 14.4⫾1.2 symmetric synapses; therefore, over the entire den- dritic tree, the density of symmetric synapses was significantly greater than the density of asymmetric synapses (p⫽0.0038; Mann–

Whitney U test, two-tailed). This ratio of symmetric to asymmetric synapses is in agreement with previous data (Sizemore et al., 2010).

Indeed, in all compartments, there were more terminals forming

Figure 1. Individual identified cholinergic interneurons can respond to cortical and thalamic stimulation and receive synaptic input from the cortex and the thalamus.A, Recording of the spontaneous spike firing of an individual cholinergic interneuron (#AJS044) in an anesthetized rat; vertical scale bars, 1 mV; horizontal scale bar, 1 s.B, Raster plot (top) and PSTH (bottom) showing the response of interneuron inAto single-pulse stimulation of ipsilateral motor cortex.

A single-trial example of an evoked spike waveform after cortical stimulation (arrow) is inset;

horizontal scale bar, 5 ms; vertical scale bar, 1 mV.C, Raster plot (top) and corresponding PSTH (bottom) showing the response of the same interneuron to single-pulse stimulation (arrow) of the ipsilateral thalamus (targeted to the parafascicular nucleus). An example of an evoked spike waveform after thalamic stimulation is inset; horizontal scale bar, 5 ms; vertical scale bar, 1 mV.

4

Note the short-latency excitations (20 ms) and multiphasic responses evoked by cortical and thalamic stimuli.D,E, After recording, the same interneuron was juxtacellularly labeled with NB and tested positive for immunoreactivity against ChAT, thus confirming its cholinergic identity.

Scale bars, 25m.F, Somata and dendrites of the identified cholinergic neuron digitally recon- structed in 3D. Scale bar, 25m.G,H, The same interneuron was then examined using electron microscopy. InG, a dendrite (d) is shown forming an asymmetric synapse (arrowhead) with an axon terminal (white asterisk) that is positive for VGluT1, a marker of cortical terminals. Note the crystalline deposits in the dendrite formed by the TMB. Scale bar, 0.25m. InH, another dendrite (d) is shown forming an asymmetric synapse (arrowhead) with an axon terminal (white asterisk) that is positive for VGluT2, a marker of thalamic terminals.

Table 1. EM analysis of the synaptic innervation of cholinergic interneuron dendrites

Properties examined

Neurons

Average NJX009 AJS044 NJX014 Total number of ultrathin sections examined (50 nm) 849 1039 789 892.33

Total number of dendritic fragments analyzed 20 24 20 21.33

In VGluT1-labeled tissue 11 14 11 12.00

In VGluT2-labeled tissue 9 10 9 9.33

Total number of unlabeled (DAB-negative) asymmetric synapses

23 32 46 33.67

In VGluT1-labeled tissue 11 22 27 20.00

In VGluT2-labeled tissue 12 10 19 13.67

Total number of VGluT1-positive synapses 0 2 0 0.67

Total number of VGluT2-positive synapses 6 5 3 4.67

Total number of (unlabeled) symmetric synapses 61 62 64 62.33 For each neuron examined, several properties were analyzed over a number of sections in either VGluT1- or VGluT2- labeled tissue.

(8)

symmetric than asymmetric synapses; how- ever, this was most prominent proximal to the soma, with asymmetric synapses form- ing 8.6⫾1.1 synapses per 10␮m of dendrite and symmetric synapses forming 17.8⫾2.4 synapses, and indeed this was significant in this proximal compartment (p⫽0.0081, Mann–Whitney U test, two-tailed;Fig. 2F).

In all other compartments, the density of symmetric synapses was not significantly greater than the density of asymmetric syn- apses (p⬎ 0.05, Mann–Whitney U test, two-tailed;Fig. 2F).

Due to the way in which we sampled the synapses, we were able to extrapolate our data and estimate the total numbers of synapses formed with an individual cho- linergic interneuron by the various differ- ent terminals. Our data suggest that an individual cholinergic interneuron has an average of 8450⫾694 afferent synapses in total (Fig. 2G), of which the majority are symmetric (an average of 5166⫾285;Fig.

2G) and the remainder are asymmetric (an average of 2859⫾458;Fig. 2G). We estimate that an individual cholinergic in- terneuron forms an average of 752⫾62 synapses with VGluT2-positive terminals.

The data also indicate that neuron AJS044 would form a total of 294 synapses with VGluT1-positive terminals. It is clear from these data that cholinergic interneurons form more synapses with thalamic than cor- tical terminals, which is in agreement with previous data (Lapper and Bolam, 1992).

Short-latency responses of rat

cholinergic interneurons to cortical and thalamic stimulation

The finding that an individual cholinergic interneuron can receive synaptic input

Figure 2. Terminals forming synapses with the dendrites of cholinergic interneurons.A, Example of a dendrite of a cholinergic interneuron (d) forming an asymmetric synapse (arrowhead) with a terminal negative for VGluT1 (n). Note that there is a terminal positive for VGluT1 (DAB product; white asterisk), forming an asymmetric synapse (arrowhead) with a MSN spine (sp).B, Cholin- ergic interneuron dendrite (d) forms two asymmetric synapses (arrowheads) with a VGluT2 positive terminal (asterisk) and terminal negative for VGluT2 (n). Note that within the same frame there is another positive terminal (asterisk) forming a synapse

4

with an MSN spine (sp).C, A dendrite (d) forms an asymmetric synapse (arrowhead) with a terminal negative for VGluT2 (n).

Note that the same terminal is also forming a synapse with a spine of an MSN (sp) and that within the same frame there is a VGluT2 positive terminal (asterisk). There is also a negative terminal (n) forming a synapse with a spine (sp).D, The den- drite of a cholinergic interneuron (d) forms a symmetric syn- apse (small arrows) with an unlabeled terminal (n), see inset.

Note that within the frame there is a terminal positive for VGluT1. Scale bars, 0.25m.E, Percentages of terminals forming symmetric (blue) or asymmetric (green) synapses with the dendrites examined. Of the terminals that formed asymmetric synapses (green), some were positive for VGluT1 (red) or VGluT2 (violet). The remaining terminals were DAB negative in tissue labeled for either VGluT1 or VGluT2 (inset).

F, Average number of asymmetric (green) and symmetric (blue) synapses normalized for every 10m of dendrite, within each compartment.G, Estimations of the total number of symmetric (blue) and asymmetric (green) synapses formed with the dendrites of cholinergic interneurons based on data collected.

(9)

from both the cortex and the thalamus concurred with the re- sponses of these interneurons to electrical stimuli delivered to these two regionsin vivo. Thirty-two identified cholinergic in- terneurons were included in this study, 18 of which were re- corded with cortical stimulation only; the remaining 14 were recorded during both cortical and thalamic stimulation (deliv- ered independently and in a random order). As a first step, the responses of identified cholinergic interneurons to brief, single- pulse electrical stimulation of the ipsilateral motor cortex (n⫽ 32) and Pfn (n⫽14) were examined (Fig. 3). Studiesin vitrohave reported that excitation of either cortical or thalamic axons with single electrical pulses does not elicit spiking responses from cho- linergic interneurons (Oswald et al., 2009; Ding et al., 2010;

Schulz et al., 2011). In contrast, we found that the firing of many cholinergic interneurons was transiently increased at “short la- tencies” of 1.5–20 ms in response to single-pulse stimulation of the cortex (50% of interneurons tested) and thalamus (64%)in vivo(Fig. 3A–F). Short-latency decreases in interneuron firing were not observed in response to afferent stimulation. Evoked spiking at these short latencies is thought to be indicative of monosynaptic inputs (Mallet et al., 2005;Sharott et al., 2012) and likely requires many corticostriatal or thalamostriatal fibers to be recruited by the stimulation.In vitropreparations, which entail the loss of at least some connections, may thus not allow for the recruitment of enough afferent neurons/axons with a single- pulse stimulus to elicit interneuron spiking.

The mean latencies of first spikes in this 1.5–20 ms window did not significantly differ between responses to cortical (10.2⫾ 0.49 ms) and thalamic (10.7⫾ 0.57 ms) stimulation (Mann–

Whitney,p⬎0.05;Fig. 3G). This was also the case for individual neurons that responded to both cortical and thalamic stimulation (Wilcoxon signed-rank test,p⬎0.05;Fig. 3H). The majority of cholinergic interneurons (10 of 14 tested) also responded to tha- lamic, but not cortical (Fig. 3B,C, inset), stimulation at a longer latency of 20 –50 ms (mean latency: 33.6⫾0.58 ms;Fig. 3E,F, inset). This later component could be due to a recruitment of polysynaptic circuits that ultimately impinge on striatum, such as cortical fibers that might be activated by ITN stimulation (Llinas et al., 2002). In summary, these group data show that cholinergic interneurons can respond with a short-latency increase in firing, indicative of a monosynaptic drive.

In vivo, striatal projection neurons and parvalbumin- expressing (PV⫹) interneurons show distinct and opposite re- sponses to paired-pulse stimulation (100 ms interpulse interval) of the ipsilateral frontal cortex (Mallet et al., 2005). We next examined the short-latency responses of cholinergic interneu- rons to paired-pulse stimulation (100 ms interval) of either cor- tex or thalamus (Fig. 4A,B). For this protocol, we tried to apply a

stimulation current that achieved a spike response to the first pulse around the rheobase (i.e., the current amplitude that evokes a spike after 50% of first pulses). With paired-pulse stimulation of cortex, cholinergic interneurons had a significantly lower probability of short-latency firing to the second pulse (Fig. 4C), as has been described previously for PV⫹interneurons (Mallet et al., 2005). Note that the short-latency response to the second pulse decreased across the whole range of initial firing probabil- ities and was not therefore a phenomenon limited to an initial high firing probability (Fig. 4C). In contrast, there was no change in firing probability across the first and second pulses of thalamic stimulation (Fig. 4D). Although the firing probability of the longer-latency responses was greater after the second pulse for some neurons (Fig. 4B), this was not significant across the pop- ulation. Therefore, in a similar manner to PV⫹interneurons, cholinergic interneurons respond preferentially to the first pulse in a paired-pulse stimulation protocol for cortical stimulation.

This relationship was not observed for thalamic stimulation, sug- gesting that: (1) electrical stimuli delivered to cortical and tha- lamic sites recruited distinct sets of inputs and (2) that there is a fundamental difference in the integration of the two excitatory afferents by cholinergic interneurons.

Previous studies, bothin vitroandin vivo, have suggested that burst-like excitatory inputs to cholinergic interneurons evoke re- sponses that mimic the timing of these neurons to motivationally salient stimuli in behaving animals (Nanda et al., 2009;Oswald et al., 2009; Ding et al., 2010). Therefore, we next examined the responses of cholinergic interneurons to short trains of high- frequency stimuli (5 pulses at 40 Hz) delivered independently to cortex and thalamus. Short-latency interneuron responses could be readily discriminated after one or more pulses of the stimula- tion trains (Fig. 5A,B). Cortical stimulation led to more spikes being fired in response to the first pulse, followed by a gradual decrease in short-latency firing probability throughout the stimulus train (Fig. 5C), which is consistent with interneuron responses to the paired-pulse stimulation at 10 Hz (Fig. 4). Con- versely, the short-latency firing probability increased through the stimulus train in response to thalamic train stimulation, with the maximum response after the third pulse of the stimulus (Fig. 5D).

The difference in firing probability for the first cortical stimula- tion pulse and the mean of the following stimuli (pulses 2–5) narrowly missed significance (p⫽0.05, Wilcoxon signed-rank test;Fig. 5E), but the difference was highly significant when the response was normalized to give the number of spikes as a per- centage of spikes fired across all pulses, thus taking into account baseline firing rate (Fig. 5F). The firing probability for the first thalamic stimulation was significantly lower than the mean of the following stimuli (pulses 2–5) for both raw and normalized mea- Table 2. Distribution of synapses over the dendritic arbor of cholinergic interneurons

Properties examined Proximal (0 –19%) Distal (20 –79%) Most distal (80 –100%)

Number of dendritic fragments analyzed 17 38 9

Average distance from soma (SEM) (m) 59.96 (12.19) 276.53 (34.22) 558.22 (17.00)

Average distance from soma as a percentage of total distance (SEM) 9.31 (1.90) 43.64 (5.40) 85.48 (2.17)

Ultrathin sections analyzed (total) 730 1568 379

Average number of ultrathin sections analyzed in serial section (SEM) 42.94 (7.02) 41.26 (6.26) 42.11 (10.13)

Total number of terminals forming asymmetric synapses 24 58 19

In VGluT1-labeled tissue 14 40 6

In VGlut2-labeled tissue 10 18 13

Total number of VGlut1-positive terminals forming synapses 1 1 0

Total number of VGlut2-positive terminals forming synapses 2 10 2

Total number of terminals forming symmetric synapses 55 103 29

The distribution of terminals forming synapses on dendrites at varying distances from the soma was analyzed for the three neurons examined.

Références

Documents relatifs

XCors that only included cortical burst firing (Fig. 6a) had a larger post-zero peak compared to those of tonic (Supplementary Fig. 6a), indicating enhanced cortical to TC

Therefore, based on the assumption that women newly given birth for the first time (primiparous) would demon- strate a stronger reward-oriented response to the body odor of an

The quire is first converted to a UPIF value (architecture depicted in Figure 10) before the latter is encoded to a posit (Section IV-B).. There are four distinct cases to take

This model shows generally thick, isostati- cally compensated crust (≥37 km) beneath the Hellenide Mountains, except immediately south of the GOC, and a singular region of thin crust

The association duration increases with SO: when nodes are active during a long period, they generate a large number of control messages which overload the network. ATA outperforms

coronal brain section of a VIPcre/tdTomato/GIN mouse shows VIP neurons in red, Martinotti cells in green and calretinin in blue in the primary somatosensory cortex (S1Tr).. (A)

The Bertlmann-Martin inequality [1, 2] yields an upper bound to the ground state mean square radius (ms radius) from the lowest dipole excitation energy.. It concerns a particle

On average, FSI input resistance measured with small current steps in the presence of ethanol was, again, not significantly different from that observed in control solution