• Aucun résultat trouvé

A LITTLE BIT MORE, A LOT BETTER - LANGUAGE EMERGENCE FROM QUANTITATIVE TO QUALITATIVE CHANGE

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "A LITTLE BIT MORE, A LOT BETTER - LANGUAGE EMERGENCE FROM QUANTITATIVE TO QUALITATIVE CHANGE"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

A LITTLE BIT MORE, A LOT BETTER -

LANGUAGE EMERGENCE FROM QUANTITATIVE TO QUALITATIVE CHANGE

JINYUN KE

English Language Institute, University of Michigan, 401 E. Liberty St.

Ann Arbor, MI 48104, USA

CHRISTOPHE COUPÉ

Laboratoire Dynamique du Langage, ISH, 14 Ave Berthelot 69363 Lyon, Cedex 07 France

TAO GONG

Department of Electronic Engineering, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, NT Hong Kong, CHINA

The draft of chimpanzee genome was published recently (Nature September 2005). It has been known that chimpanzees share more than 98% of our DNA and almost all of our genes. In addition to the striking genetic closeness, the studies on chimpanzees in both laboratories and natural habitats have revealed that they share with us many cognitive abilities (Tomasello & Call 1997; Hauser 2005), and exhibit complex social behaviors (de Waal 2005) and rich cultural traditions which are transmitted through social learning (Whiten 2005). In particular, chimpanzees have demonstrated cognitive abilities which are considered crucial for learning and using language, including manipulation of symbols, understanding of abstract concepts, intention reading and attention sharing, the ability of imitation, and so on.

While chimpanzees share a strikingly high degree of similarity with humans, the question about language origin become more intriguing: if chimpanzees are so close to humans in cognitive abilities and social behaviors, why can’t they invent a complex communication system with compositionality, hierarchy, and recursion similar to humans?

Elman (2005) points out that “language sits at the crossroads of a number of small phenotypic changes in our species that interact uniquely to yield language as the outcome” (p114). It is these small phenotypic differences between human and chimpanzees that result in a communication means of a totally different nature. The study of complex nonlinear systems has shown abundant examples of such small quantitative differences leading to phase transitions, i.e.

(2)

qualitative differences, in the system dynamics. One classic example is the bifurcation observed in the logistic map (May 1976), in which the system changes from a stable end state to an oscillation end state, when the parameter changes from 2.999 to 3.001.

We use a computer agent-based model to show how the small changes of a few parameters of cognitive abilities would result in such a phase transition in the outcome of the communication system. The model simulates a group of agents interacting with each other with increasing communication ability. The agents possess a set of pre-linguistic abilities which have been shown to be shared by chimpanzees and humans, i.e. they have simple semantic distinctions between entity and action, and are able to sequence items, learn and use symbols, detect the interlocutor’s intentions, and detect recurrent patterns (Gong et al 2005). The last three abilities are taken as parameters and varied as probabilities in the model.

The simulations show that when these parameters are all of low values, the group of agents can only develop a limited number of holistic signals. However, when these parameters cross some thresholds, a compositional language could emerge with a set of words and a certain dominant word order shared by the agents, which dramatically increases the communication efficacy of the group.

The model thus suggests that even the chimpanzees share a great deal with humans, some small differences could divide the two species apart definitely.

References

de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). A century of getting to know the chimpanzee. Nature, 437/7055: 56-59.

Elman, J.L. (2005). Connectionist models of cognitive development: Where next? Trends in Cognitive Science. 9/3:111-117.

Gong, T., Minett, J. A. Ke, J-Y., Holland, J. H. & Wang, W. S-Y. 2005.

Coevolution of lexicon and syntax from a simulation perspective, Complexity, 10(6):1-13.

Hauser, M. (2005). Our chimpanzee mind. Nature, 437/7055:60-63.

May, R. (1976). Simple Mathematics Models with very complicated dynamics.

Nature, 261(5560):459-467.

Tomasello, M. and J. Call (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

Whiten, A. (2005). The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans.

Nature, 437/7055:52-55.

Références

Documents relatifs

According to the ideas of MDA we assume that automatic transformation steps from the source model (in our case natural language) to the target conceptual model

Our research showed that an estimated quarter of a million people in Glasgow (84 000 homes) and a very conservative estimate of 10 million people nation- wide, shared our

Health workers must recognize that - although conflict is political - they, like other development workers, have a significant social role to play, and that direct

A charity, or charitable organization, in England and Wales is a particular type of voluntary organization which set up for charitable, social, humanitarian or other purposes.. It

We have considered a number and the possible extensions of the error types after Rasmussen’s framework [3] and performed partial in-depth analyses about: level of

Though the complex systems described in this issue succeed in creating more understanding models and touch here and there on the role of affect and the significance of care in

The set 7 consists of the boundary of the unit square and of vertical lines, one for each dyadic rational point on the base interval ofour square.. These

24 Interestingly, this perspective on the social world inspired by field theory has affinities with quantitative methods and in particular with correspondence analysis, since it