• Aucun résultat trouvé

On the role of morphology, syllable structure, frequency and spelling in English vowel reduction

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "On the role of morphology, syllable structure, frequency and spelling in English vowel reduction"

Copied!
2
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

HAL Id: halshs-02143205

https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-02143205

Submitted on 29 May 2019

HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- entific research documents, whether they are pub- lished or not. The documents may come from teaching and research institutions in France or abroad, or from public or private research centers.

L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires publics ou privés.

On the role of morphology, syllable structure, frequency and spelling in English vowel reduction

Quentin Dabouis, Jean-Michel Fournier

To cite this version:

Quentin Dabouis, Jean-Michel Fournier. On the role of morphology, syllable structure, frequency and spelling in English vowel reduction. 27th Manchester Phonology Meeting, May 2019, Manchester, France. �halshs-02143205�

(2)

POTENTIAL FACTORS

Syllable structure: vowels in open syllables are more likely to reduce than vowels in closed syllables (Burzio 1994: 113; Fudge 1984; Halle & Keyser 1971)

Nature of the coda: vowels in syllables closed by obstruents are less likely to reduce than vowels closed by sonorants (Pater 2000) & vowels in syllables closed by non-coronals are less likely to reduce than vowels closed by coronals (Burzio 1994; Dahak 2011; Fudge 1984; Ross 1972).

Prefixation: initial pretonic closed syllables normally do not reduce except if that syllable is a semantically opaque prefix (Chomsky & Halle 1968: 118; Guierre 1979: 253; Libermann & Prince 1977; Pater 2000; Selkirk 1980).

Spelling: vowels spelled with a digraph reduce less than monographs, especially in the initial pretonic position (Dahak 2011; Deschamps et al. 2004: 217).

Word frequency: More frequent words show more reduction than less frequent words (Fidelholtz 1975).

Existence of a base: the existence of a morphological base in which the vowel is stressed can diminish its chances to reduce (Chomsky & Halle 1968: 112, e.g. reláxrèlaxátion, expréssèxpressívity), even more so if that base is more frequent than the derivative (Bermúdez-Otero 2012: 32, after Krazka-Szlenk 2007: §8.1.2).

On the role of morphology, syllable structure, frequency and spelling in English vowel reduction

Q. Dabouis

1

, J.-M. Fournier

2

1

Laboratoire de Recherche sur le Langage (EA 999) – Université Clermont Auvergne, France

2

Laboratoire Ligérien de Linguistique (UMR 7270) – Université de Tours, France

References

Background

Discussion

Bermúdez-Otero, R. “The Architecture of Grammar and the Division of Labour in Exponence.” The Morphology and Phonology of Exponence, edited by Jochen Trommer, Oxford University Press, 2012, pp. 8–83. Burzio, L. Principles of English Stress. Cambridge University Press, 1994. Chomsky, N. & M.

Halle. The Sound Pattern of English. MIT Press, 1968. Cruttenden, A. Gimson’s Pronunciation of English. 8th edition, Routledge, 2014. Dabouis, Q. L’accent Secondaire En Anglais Britannique Contemporain. Ph.D. dissertation. University of Tours, 2016. Dahak, A. Etude Diachronique, Phonologique et Morphologique Des Syllabes Inaccentuées En Anglais Contemporain. Ph.D. dissertation. Université de Paris Diderot, 2011. Deschamps, A, et al. English Phonology and Graphophonemics. Ophrys, 2004. Fudge, E. English Word Stress. G. Allen & Unwin, 1984. Guierre, L. Essai Sur l’accentuation En Anglais Contemporain : Eléments Pour Une Synthèse. Ph.D. dissertation. Université Paris-VII, 1979. Halle, M. & S. Keyser. English Stress: Its Form, Its Growth, and Its Role in Verse. Harper & Row, 1971. Hay, J. & H. Baayen. “Parsing and Productivity.” Yearbook of Morphology 2001, edited by G.E. Booij

& J. van Marle, Kluwer, 2002, pp. 203–35. Jones, D. Cambridge English Pronouncing Dictionary. 17th ed, Cambridge University Press, 2006. Krazka-Szlenk, I. “Analogy: The Relation Between Lexicon and Grammar.” LINCOM Studies in Theoretical Linguistics, no. 38, LINCOM Europa, 2007, p. 154.

Liberman, M. & A. Prince. “On Stress and Linguistic Rhythm.” Linguistic Inquiry, vol. 8, no. 2, 1977, pp. 249–336. Pater, J. “Non-Uniformity in English Secondary Stress: The Role of Ranked and Lexically Specific Constraints.” Phonology, vol. 17, 2000, pp. 237–74. Ross, J. R. “A Reanalysis of English

Word Stress (Part I).” Contributions to Generative Phonology, edited by Michael K. Brame, University of Texas Press, 1972, pp. 229–323. Selkirk, E. O. “The Role of Prosodic Categories in English Word Stress.” Linguistic Inquiry, vol. 11, no. 3, 1980, pp. 563–605. Van Heuven, W. V. J., et al. “Subtlex-UK: A A New and Improved Word Frequency Database for British English.” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, no. 67, 2014, pp. 1176–90. Wells, J. C. Longman Pronunciation Dictionary. 3rd ed, Longman, 2008.

Logistic regression was conducted and three parameters were found to have a significant effect on reduction.

Reduction is more likely if:

➢ The vowel is spelled with a monograph (e.g. acádemy, àvocádo)

➢ The syllable is open (e.g. fiáncee, kàngaróo)

➢ The word has a high frequency.

Global results

Results: Monorphemic words and bound roots + suffix (n = 1120 & 454)

The effect of spelling can be observed in both positions, even though reduction is a lot more common in the intertonic position.

Spelling

Results: Prefixed words and stress-shifted derivatives

In words that are not suffixal derivatives, we distinguished three categories of prefixed words:

➢ Opaque (e.g. accede, betray, collect, promote, receive)

➢ Transparent prefix – non-compositional (e.g. cohabit, deflate, enable, exterior, revive)

➢ Transparent prefix – compositional (e.g. abnormal, co- author, decentralize, reactivate)

Only the main pronunciation is considered here.

Clear difference between compositional and non- compositional structures.

Prefixation (n = 2204) Stress-shifted derivatives (n = 565 & 179)

PROBLEM

No large-scale empirical evaluation of these factors and their interactions has been conducted.

So, what determines vowel reduction in English?

The results confirm the importance of position, syllable structure, spelling, morphology and absolute frequency.

However, there is only weak evidence for segmentability factors and the nature of coda consonants.

Other factors may still need to be considered (“Arab Rule” weight interactions, foreignness, vowel-specific behaviours, etc.) to reach a comprehensive understanding of English vowel reduction.

On monographs followed by <rC>

On semantically opaque prefixes

The results confirm the long-standing assumption that such prefixes have a distinct behaviour and supports analyses that refuse to see words that contain them as morphological simplexes.

On preservation effects

The importance of morphological relationships is found to extend to reduction, although no effects of segmentability (through relative frequency and semantic transparency) can clearly be confirmed.

Laboratoire Ligérien de Linguistique

VOWEL REDUCTION IN ENGLISH

In English, many vowels reduce when unstressed:

atom /ˈætəm/ vs. atomic /əˈtɒmɪk/

fatal /ˈfeɪtəl/ vs. fatality /fəˈtælɪti/

eponymous /ɪˈpɒnɪməs/ (cf. eponym /ˈepənɪm/)

We take reduced vowels (/ə/, /i/, /ɪ/, /ʊ/ or /u/) to be defined as in Cruttenden (2014 : 158): “these are the short vowels with a central or centralised quality (apart from final /i/) and are the least prominent syllables.”

However, reduction is not systematic: some words have full vowels in unstressed positions:

relaxation /ˌriːlækˈseɪʃən/ dynamic /daɪˈnæmɪk/

A number of different factors have been claimed to influence vowel reduction.

AIMS

➢ Conduct a large-scale investigation of these factors using dictionary data, which may then be used as a reference point for later studies using speech data.

➢ Two positions are considered:

▪ Initial pretonic (e.g. arríve)

▪ intertonic (e.g. rèlaxátion)

➢ Draw some of the theoretical consequences of the results.

Methodology

Two datasets have been used:

Initial pretonic position: Data from Jones (2006).

Extraction of all words with no stress on their first syllable.

Once proper names have been excluded, the full dataset contains 9432 words.

Intertonic position: data from Wells (2008), taken from the datasets of Dabouis (2016). Two datasets were used:

monomorphemic words & bound roots + suffix, and derived words with a base stressed on its second syllable.

Our study focuses on monomorphemic words & bound roots + suffix and stress-shifted derivatives for both datasets AND prefixed words for the initial pretonic position.

Only British pronunciations are considered.

Not included in the analyses:

➢ Words with uninterpretable vowels: <i> realized /ɪ/, <u>

realized /ʊ/. /ə(ʊ)/ is treated as Full ~ Reduced.

➢ Intertonic position: words whose second syllable is part of a historical prefix (e.g. recollect, supersede)

➢ Derivational pairs with a different spelling of the vowel (e.g. reveal → revelation)

➢ Neoclassical roots

Vowels followed by <rC> are treated separately because it is not clear whether they should be analysed as representing underlying /Vː/ or /Vr/.

CODING

Based on the proposals found in the literature, the following variables were coded:

SYLLSTRUCTURE: syllables are coded as OPEN, CLOSED or S (when the vowel is followed by /sC/).

CODA-PLACE: Codas were coded as CORONAL or NON-CORONAL.

CODA-MANNER: Codas were coded as OBSTRUENT or SONORANT.

LOGFREQUENCY: token frequency taken from SUBTLEX-UK (Van Heuven et al. 2014), which was log- transformed so as to resemble the way “humans process frequency information” (Hay & Baayen 2002).

SPELLING: vowels were coded as MONOGRAPH or DIGRAPH.

MORPHOLOGY: presence of a semantically opaque prefix (PREFIXED vs. NONPREFIXED)

SEMANTICTRANSPARENCY: Items for which the base appears explicitly in the definition of the derivative in a general dictionary (Dictionary.com) were coded as TRANSPARENT. Others were coded as OPAQUE – Derivatives only.

The dependent variable is VOWELREDUCTION, coded using a four-point scale (Full, Full ~ Reduced, Reduced ~ Full, Reduced).

THE DATA

Syllable structure

Initial pretonic Intertonic

95% C.I.

p-value 95% C.I.

p-value

Lower OR Higher Lower OR Higher

SPELLING-MONO 3.41 4.59 6.19 <2 e-16 1.48 2.00 2.70 9.18 e-06 SYLLSTR-OPEN 4.76 5.74 6.93 < 2 e-16 1.79 2.30 2.94 1.21 e-10 SYLLSTR-SC 1.83 2.59 3.67 1.14 e-08 1.05 1.74 2.87 0.031728 LOGFQ 1.06 1.08 1.11 1.69 e-09 1.02 1.05 1.08 0.000892

7 51 22

43

0 9 8

0 1

3 0 1

1 2 0 0

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

Obstruent Sonorant Obstruent Sonorant

Coronal Non-coronal

The role of the coda

Full Full ~ Red Red ~Full Red

In both positions, there is a clear effect of syllable structure.

Vowels followed by /sC/ (e.g. estáte, bàlustráde) constitute an intermediate class, probably due to the problematic syllabification of /s/.

The effect of the nature of the coda is weakly significant in the initial pretonic position only.

303

45

15 4

134

4

9

1 162

1

21

1 467

4

391

12

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

Monographs Digraphs Monographs Digraphs

Initial pretonic Intertonic

Full Full ~ Red Red ~ Full Red 162

18

123

9 1 5

105

12

17

5

0

4 149

8

5

16

1

4 457

7 3

372 6

13

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

Open sC Closed Open sC Closed

Initial pretonic Intertonic Full Full ~ Red Red ~ Full Red

33 4 42 10 1 47

80

801 58 258 62 8 158 613

25

4

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

Open sC Closed Open sC Closed Open Closed

Opaque prefix Transparent prefix - non- compositional

Transparent prefix - compositional

Full Reduced 2 88

0

23

5

83 4

90 2

0

5

15

3

81

1

2

16

0 21

84 0 0

10 8

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

Pref NonPref Pref NonPref Pref NonPref

Open sC Closed

Full Full ~ Red Red ~ Full Red

9 15 1

7

5 11

5

13 0

2

4

17

16

20

1

2

4

4

372

81 6

1

13

0

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

Non-derived Derived Non-derived Derived Non-derived Derived

Open sC Closed

Intertonic position

Full Full ~ Red Red ~ Full Red

Initial pretonic Intertonic

95% C.I.

p-value 95% C.I.

p-value

Lower OR Higher Lower OR Higher

SPELLING-MONO 1.80 2.70 4.06 2.29 e-06 1.21 2.65 5.77 0.01545 SYLLABLE-OPEN 2.23 2.73 3.33 < 2 e-16 2.75 3.75 5.11 1.8 e-14 SYLLABLE-SC 0.56 0.84 1.25 0.386 0.44 0.80 1.46 0.47611

LOGFQ_DERIVATIVE 1.04 1.08 1.11 < 2 e-16 1.03 1.08 1.14 0.00331 MORPH-PREF 2.58 3.35 4.36 2.89 e-06

162 88 18

23 191 83

105

90

12

0 24 15

149

81

8

2 11 0

457

84 7

0 6 8

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

Non-derived Derived Non-derived Derived Non-derived Derived

Open sC Closed

Initial pretonic position

Full Full ~ Red Red ~ Full Red

Logistic regression revealed significant effects for the following variables:

The difference between derived and non-derived words is clear in all configurations except in closed initial pretonic syllables, where the vast majority of vowels do not reduce.

Interestingly enough, vowels followed by /sC/ behave in a way that is comparable to that of vowels in closed syllables in derivatives, but not in non-derived words.

As could be expected, prefixed derivatives display more reduction (only monographs are shown below):

Reduction in derived and non-derived items (non-prefixed, monographs only) Examples:

Monographs: àborígine, legítimize, umbrélla Digraphs: àmaurósis, authéntic, lìmousíne

Compare:

rémedy → r/ə/médial áspect → /ə ~ æ/spéctual

cóncept → c/ə/ncéptual vítal → v/aɪ/tálity

hóstile → h/ɒ/stílity táctic → t/æ/ctícian

68 55

7 10

7 6

0 2

6 7

4 6

3 4

8 10

0%

20%

40%

60%

80%

100%

Non-derived Derived Non-derived Derived Initial pretonic Intertonic

Full Full ~ Red Red ~Full Red

In non-derived words, the behaviour of vowels followed by <rC>

resembles that of vowels in closed syllables and/or that of digraphs.

In derivatives, there is more reduction in the intertonic than for vowels in closed syllables.

Références

Documents relatifs

The paper mainly aims to reanalyze data with the presently available corpus linguistics tools from a relatively large scale paper-and-pencil based Hungarian verbal

- Pierre Arnoux, Université de la Méditerranée, - Pierre Cartier, École Normale Supérieure de Paris, - Julien Cassaigne, Institut de Mathématiques de Luminy, - Sébastien

First of all, Justin and Pirillo characterized pairs of words directing the same episturmian word in the case of wavy directive words, that is, spinned infinite words

In this paper we consider the characterization of words having a given mapping σ from [1. n ] as suffix permutation according to the lexicographic order. Such characterization,

In this paper, we give a simpler proof of this resuit (Sect, 2) and then, in Sections 3 and 4, we study the occurrences of factors and the return words in episturmian words

If we distinguish the different employments of a word, which are pieces of intentional behaviour in space and time, from its use which is a more comprehensive process and not

Eight years after the war, Beloved appears outside Sethe’s house who then adopts her and starts to believe that the young woman is her dead daughter come back to life..

The pronunciation of prefixed words in English has often been defined in terms of morphology only (see for instance Guierre [1979], Fournier [1996] or Guyot-Talbot