• Aucun résultat trouvé

When children stop trusting what they have perceived

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "When children stop trusting what they have perceived"

Copied!
1
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

It is commonly hypothesized that the knowledge underlying metamemory heuristics develops on the basis of day-to-day experience (Olds & Westerman, 2012). However, to our knowledge, no previous study has examined the processes that explain the development of these metacognitive rules. This study is an attempt to fill this gap.

Recently, several studies have demonstrated that metacognitive heuristics are more malleable and sensitive to empirical training than was previously thought (Unkelbach, 2006). Using the easily learned, easily remembered (ELER) heuristic, children’s ability to learn a new metacognitive rule through an implicit process involving the detection of environmental regularities is investigated.

When children stop trusting what they have perceived

Marie Geurten

1,2

, Sylvie Willems

3

, & Thierry Meulemans

1,3

1Department of Psychology: Cognition and Behavior, University of Liege, Belgium; 2Fund of Scientific Research FNRS 3 Psychological and Speech therapy consultation center (CPLU)

1. to determine whether young children are able to employ the ELER heuristic

2. to examine the impact of implicit training on the application of the ELER heuristic

3. to explore the influence of higher cognitive functions on their use of the ELER heuristic

PARTICIPANTS

60 Belgian French-speaking children (30 female) from 3 age-groups (4-5, 6-7, and 8-9) equal in terms of :

• Intelligence (Matrix Task) • Parental education level

PROCEDURE

Pretest

Trainings

Posttest

INTRODUCTION

AIM OF THE STUDY

METHOD

PRETEST

Predicted recall of the easy pairs was higher than predicted recall of the hard pairs for the whole sample (p < .01, ηp² = .36) and for 4-year-olds (p < .01, ηp² = .41), 6-year-olds (p < .01, ηp² = .36), and for 8-year-olds (p < .01, ηp² = .33)

Fig. 1. Mean of JOLs for the whole sample and for each age group in the pretest and posttest phases as a

function of pairs’ ease of learning. Error bars show 95% confidence intervals.

POSTEST

The comparison of the difference between mean JOLs for easy and hard pairs revealed a decrease in the use of the ELER heuristic in 4-year-olds (p < .01, ηp² = .40) and 6-year-olds (p < .05, ηp² = .20), but not in 8-year-olds (p = .16, ηp² = .10).

EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS

In the 8-year-old group, the children with the best monitoring abilities were also the ones who most resisted the experimental manipulation (β = -.55, p < .05, R² = .23).

None of the executive variables contributed to explaining the JOLs of 4-year-olds and 6-year-4-year-olds at posttest.

RESULTS

• This study has highlighted that the ELER heuristic is learned very early in childhood

• Some evidences are provided that the development of the ELER metacognitive rule occurs through an implicit process involving the detection of environmental regularities

• Results also support Miller and Lloyd’s hypothesis that, once children have reached adequate maturity, high level cognitive functions may regulate the use of the ELER heuristics

• Altogether, these findings seem to shed a consistent light on the experiential explanation of the development of metacognitive heuristics

DISCUSSION

References:

Miller, J. K., & Lloyd, M. E. (2011). The Development of the Fluency Heuristic in Childhood: More Questions than Answers. In P. A. Higham & J. P. Leboe (Eds.), Constructions of Remembering and Metacognition (pp. 28-39). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

Unkelbach, C. (2006). The Learned Interpretation of Cognitive Fluency. Psychological Science, 17, 339-345. Olds, J. M., & Westerman, D. L. (2012). Can fluency be interpreted as novelty? Retraining the interpretation of fluency in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 653-664.

Contact : Marie Geurten / University of Liege – Neuropsychology Unit – Email: mgeurten@ulg.ac.be

Study Encoding

Study Retrieval

JoL Priming Test

Study Encoding

Study Retrieval

JoL Priming Rigged

Lures

Study Encoding

Study Retrieval

Figure

Fig. 1. Mean of JOLs for the whole sample and for each age group in the pretest and posttest phases as a  function of pairs’ ease of learning

Références

Documents relatifs

Il s’agit de la définition du burn-out et de ses symptômes, des facteurs de risque, des facteurs de protection, de l’expérience personnelle comportant les difficultés

RER, % HR max and % _ W W max ) determined with three different data analysis approaches (SIN, MV and P3) and b) gas exchange data, HR and substrate oxidation rates at rest and

INITIAL DESIGN OF THE OCARIOT EXPERIENCE The goal of the gamification strategy is to promote children between 9 and 12 years old to acquire healthy habits in the nutritional

L'étude cristallographique de SnTe a montré qu'à la température ambiante ce composé cristallise dans le système cubique à faces centrées type NaCl (fig.. Dans cette structure,

Motivated by the idea of calculating color for a resource in a determin- istic way by “Color the Linked Data Web” 6 , balloon Synopsis implemented the functionality that

24 World Resources Institute, United Nations Environment Programme, United Nations Development Programme, World Bank, World Resources 1998-1999 A Guide to the Global

Described most often as a set of manipulative and subtle techniques to turn the child against the non-custodial parent, parental alienation syndrome (known as PAS) is considered as

We now report numerical results for initial boundary-value problem for the wave equation (2.1) using piecewise linear finite elements in space) and Newmark scheme with non-uniform