• Aucun résultat trouvé

Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus improves cognitive flexibility but impairs response inhibition in Parkinson disease

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Partager "Deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus improves cognitive flexibility but impairs response inhibition in Parkinson disease"

Copied!
4
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

Deep Brain Stimulation of the Subthalamic Nucleus Improves Cognitive Flexibility but Impairs Response Inhibition in Parkinson Disease

Karsten Witt, MD; Ulrich Pulkowski, MD; Jan Herzog, MD; Delia Lorenz, MD;

Wolfgang Hamel, MD; Gu¨nther Deuschl, MD; Paul Krack, MD

Background:Deep brain stimulation of the subtha- lamic nucleus (STN) improves motor symptoms of Par- kinson disease. Although several studies have assessed cog- nitive functions before surgery and after long-term STN stimulation, only a few have assessed patients while stimu- lation is on and off to more specifically address the short- term cognitive effects of STN deep brain stimulation.

Objective:To examine the short-term effects of STN stimulation on several tests sensitive to executive func- tion and the long-term effects of STN stimulation on a global cognitive scale.

Design:Twenty-three patients with Parkinson disease were tested 6 to 12 months after surgery with STN stimu- lation switched on and off in a random order while tak- ing their regular medication. The Unified Parkinson’s Dis- ease Rating Scale motor score was also rated in the on and off stimulation condition. The neuropsychological battery included digit span, verbal fluency, Stroop color

test, and random number generation in a single- and dual- task condition.

Results:Short-term stimulation improved the results on the Random Number Generation Task, requiring suppres- sion of habitual responses, but induced more errors in the interference task of the Stroop color test. Digit span, ver- bal fluency, and dual-task performance results did not change. There was a significant correlation (r=0.47,P=.02) between improved performance on the Random Number Generation Task and impaired response inhibition in the Stroop interference condition. A preoperative to postop- erative comparison showed no changes in global cogni- tive function with long-term STN deep brain stimulation.

Conclusions:Short-term STN stimulation improves cog- nitive flexibility (giving up habitual responses) but im- pairs response inhibition. Long-term STN stimulation does not change global cognitive function.

Arch Neurol. 2004;61:697-700

L

ONG-TERM DEEP BRAIN STIMU- lation (DBS) of the subtha- lamic nucleus (STN) is an ef- fective treatment of motor symptoms in patients with advanced Parkinson disease (PD). Spe- cific effects on the cognitive domain are still debatable. When comparing cogni- tive function before and after surgery, sev- eral variables can interfere: (1) the surgi- cal procedure, (2) the reduction in dopaminergic medication, (3) DBS itself, and (4) postoperative mood changes. The influence of these factors could be avoided by testing patients with stimulation on and off. Until now, only 2 studies examined PD patients with stimulation on and off. Pil- lon et al1showed that STN DBS improves the speed in parts A and B of the Trail- Making Test, the word and color condi- tion of the Stroop color test, the reaction time performance, and subscores of tests sensitive for spatial working memory. Ja- hanshahi et al2found, in a small series of

patients, that STN DBS improves cogni- tive flexibility, attention, and working memory but worsens conditional associa- tive learning and performance on the in- terference condition of the Stroop color test.

The present study more specifically investigates the effects of short-term STN stimulation on executive function in a larger series of patients and documents the effects of long-term STN stimulation on more global cognitive functioning.

METHODS PATIENTS

Twenty-three PD patients (6 women and 17 men), with a mean ± SD age of 57.4 ± 5.8 years, were examined 6 to 12 months (mean ± SD, 8.8 ± 1.6 months) after bilateral electrode im- plantation in the STN for DBS. All patients had advanced PD, with a mean ± SD disease dura- tion of 15.1 ± 5.5 years.

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION

From the Departments of Neurology (Drs Witt, Pulkowski, Herzog, Lorenz, Deuschl, and Krack) and Neurosurgery (Dr Hamel), Christian-Albrechts-University Kiel, Kiel, Germany.

(REPRINTED) ARCH NEUROL / VOL 61, MAY 2004 WWW.ARCHNEUROL.COM 697

©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/neur/13744/ by a Universite de Geneve User on 04/27/2017

(2)

Dementia (Mattis3Dementia Rating Scale score,⬍130) was an exclusion criterion. Quadripolar electrodes (Medtronic, Min- neapolis, Minn) were implanted using stereotactic magnetic reso- nance imaging and intraoperative electrophysiology, as previ- ously described.4The patients were retested 12 months later using the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale. At testing, the stimu- lation characteristics were as follows: mean ± SD pulse width, 63 ± 13 µs; mean ± SD frequency, 139 ± 15 Hz; and mean ± SD stimulation voltage, 3.2 ± 0.4 V. All electrodes were monopo- lar, using 1 electrode contact. Postoperatively, patients re- ceived a mean levodopa equivalence dosage of 746 mg. Of the tested patients, 19 (82.6%) were taking a dopaminergic ago- nists. Five patients were taking only dopaminergic agonists (3, cabergoline; and 2, pergolide mesylate), and 4 were taking le- vodopa only. Fourteen patients were taking a combination of levodopa and dopamine agonists (9, cabergoline; and 5, pergolide). All patients gave their informed consent. The pro- tocol was approved by the local ethical committee of Christian- Albrechts-University Kiel.

TEST PROCEDURE

Patients were taking their usual antiparkinsonian medication while stimulation was on and off. The conditions of stimula- tion were set 30 minutes before beginning the neurological and neuropsychological examination. The conditions of stimula- tion were counterbalanced in a pseudorandom order. The mo- tor score of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) (part III) was evaluated with stimulation on and off.

Digit span forward and backward was carried out accord- ing to the administration of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale.5 Digit span scores forward and backward were analyzed separately.

Verbal fluency6was tested using the categories female or male, first name, and animals or plants in parallel forms for 1 minute each. The order of categories and consonants was pseu- dorandomized. For literal fluency, words beginning with “K”

and “N” or “L” and “M” were taken. Each fluency task was scored separately. A shortened Stroop color test7contained 4 trials:

(1) reading words (blue, yellow, green, and red) printed in black ink, (2) reading color dots for simple color naming, (3) inter- ference condition reading words (blue, yellow, green, and red) printed in ink of different colors, and (4) interference condi- tion naming the color of the ink of the written words (blue, yellow, green, and red). For each trial, a test containing 36 items was used. The number of errors and the time to finish the test was scored for each trial separately.

For the Random Number Generation Task (RNGT), sub- jects were instructed to generate series of 100 numbers of the digits 1 to 10 in a random fashion paced by a tone (1 Hz). The concept of randomness was explained using standard proce- dures (ie, using instruction based on an analogy of selecting and replacing numbered table tennis balls from a shoe carton).

An example trial was undertaken. If the subject had not un- derstood the instructions, several simple examples of random- ness were given and the examiners (K.W. and U.P.) and the subject executed a test run. The total time taken to generate 100 items was noted. In a dual-task condition, patients gener- ated digits randomly and simultaneously sorted a stack of 100 mixed blue and red cards. The digits were recorded and ana- lyzed by different random measurements; the Evans8Random Number Generation Index is a first-order measure of random- ness. This index varies between 0 and 1; the higher the index, the less random the series. Finally, we calculated the counting scores. These indices measure the tendency to count in ascend- ing or descending series.9Count score 1 (CS1) and count score 2 (CS2) measure the tendency to count in steps of 1 and 2, re- spectively. The CS1 was increased by one2if one counting step (eg, 2-3 or 7-6) was observed. The sequence length is squared

to give higher weights to runs of longer sequences. The total count score is the sum of CS1 and CS2 and reflects all count- ing tendencies in steps of 1 and 2. Subjects with a higher count- ing score are unable to suppress habitual counting tendencies.

STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

A nonparametric analysis (Wilcoxon signed rank test) was per- formed on each of the variables comparing the stimulation on and stimulation off conditions. Furthermore, a correlation within neuropsychological changes (changes in digit span, verbal flu- ency, Stroop color test performance, and different measure- ments of the RNGT) and between neuropsychological and mo- tor changes (UPDRS score) was performed (Spearman rank correlation analysis).

RESULTS

The mean ± SD UPDRS motor score while taking usual medication was 18.8 ± 9.4, and it decreased to 11.5 ± 7.4 after turning on the stimulators (z= 4.20,P= .01).

Table 1displays the results of the neuropsycho- logical testing. There were no significant changes be- tween the on and off stimulation condition for the digit span and verbal fluency scores.

After turning the stimulators on, patients made more errors in the interference condition of the Stroop color test. The speed of this test was not altered significantly in any condition.

In the single-task condition of the RNGT, the CS1 and the total count score decreased significantly after turn- ing on the stimulators. The time to generate 100 items was similar. In the dual-task condition of the RNGT, the CS1 and the CS2 improved with stimulation, but these changes were not significant. To analyze the influence of STN DBS on dual-task performance, we compared the difference of single- and dual-task performance of the CS1 and CS2 with and without DBS. There was no significant influence of the stimulation on dual-task performance.

The correlation analysis (Spearman rank correla- tion) between the positive change on the CS1 and the wors- ening in the Stroop color test (error rate) in the interfer- ence conditions showed a significant positive correlation (r=0.47,P=.02). The correlation analysis (Spearman rank correlation) between the positive change on the total count score and the worsening in the Stroop color test (error rate) in the interference conditions showed a significant posi- tive correlation too (r=0.59,P=.01). The more errors in the interference condition of the Stroop color test were made after turning on the stimulators, the more patients gave up their counting tendencies in the RNGT. There were no further significant correlations between neurological (UPDRS score) and neuropsychological changes.

To assess global changes of cognitive functioning, the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale was repeated 12 months after electrode implantation. The results are shown in Table 2. No significant changes concerning the subitems of the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale were observed.

COMMENT

Subthalamic nucleus stimulation led to mild improve- ment in the UPDRS motor score and to some minor

(REPRINTED) ARCH NEUROL / VOL 61, MAY 2004 WWW.ARCHNEUROL.COM 698

©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/neur/13744/ by a Universite de Geneve User on 04/27/2017

(3)

changes in cognitive abilities while patients took their usual medication. Therefore, DBS was effective in the mo- tor and cognitive domains. This study demonstrates a sig- nificant improvement of counting scores in the RNGT after turning on the stimulation. On the other hand, pa- tients made more errors in the interference condition of the Stroop color test in the stimulation on condition. A further analysis of these changes caused by the STN stimu- lation showed a correlation between improved RNGT per- formance and impaired response inhibition in the Stroop interference condition. Concerning the effects of STN DBS on global cognitive functioning after 12 months, we found no significant changes. These results are in line with those of other studies1,10examining relatively young PD pa- tients after STN stimulation.

In everyday life, we are used to counting in ascend- ing or descending series, mostly in steps of 1 or 2, but we never produce series of digits in a random fashion.

To produce a perfect random series of digits, one has to give up the habit of counting. Higher counting rates were shown by different patient groups with a dysexecutive syndrome (patients with frontal lesions9and patients with PD11). Recently, an imaging study was performed, dem- onstrating the human cortical network subserving ran- dom number generation. Generating numbers ran- domly activates cortical areas involved in executive functioning (the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the an- terior cingulate cortex, and the biparietal and cerebellar cortex).12In summary, (1) theoretical consideration about the task, (2) human lesion studies, and (3) data from func- tional imaging give evidence that executive control is nec- essary for generating digits randomly. Different vari- ables describe the cognitive abilities necessary for successful random generation of numbers. The Evans Ran- dom Number Generation Index reflects monitoring ca- pacities9that did not change significantly under STN Table 1. Results of the Neuropsychological Battery With Stimulation Off and On

Variable

Stimulation Off*

Stimulation On*

z Score

2-Tailed PValue Digit span

Forward 7.69 ± 2.12 7.61 ± 2.50 0.05 .96

Backward 4.91 ± 1.76 5.04 ± 1.77 0.69 .49

Verbal fluency Category

First run 9.43 ± 3.54 9.78 ± 4.13 0.28 .78

Second run 9.13 ± 4.43 8.70 ± 4.24 1.74 .08

Literal

First run 17.17 ± 5.02 15.65 ± 5.22 0.30 .78

Second run 15.74 ± 6.61 16.04 ± 6.71 0.51 .54

Stroop color test

Words printed in black ink

No. of errors 0.04 ± 0.21 0.00 ± 0.00 1.00 .32

Time, s 16.52 ± 2.69 16.57 ± 3.76 0.51 .61

Color dots for color naming

No. of errors 0.04 ± 0.21 0.22 ± 0.42 1.63 .10

Time, s 26.00 ± 6.42 25.07 ± 5.57 0.91 .36

Interference condition Reading words

No. of errors 0.43 ± 0.90 0.47 ± 1.16 0.30 .76

Time, s 24.15 ± 10.36 21.72 ± 9.40 1.90 .06

Naming colors

No. of errors 0.78 ± 1.44 1.74 ± 1.66 2.84 .004

Time, s 55.17 ± 15.60 58.91 ± 15.86 1.72 .08

Random Number Generation Task Single task

Random Number Generation Index 0.37 ± 0.02 0.36 ± 0.01 0.15 .88

Null score 48.04 ± 7.78 48.55 ± 6.10 0.77 .44

Counting score

1 78.04 ± 41.32 50.74 ± 28.23 2.86 .004

2 37.35 ± 44.10 29.30 ± 12.47 0.17 .99

Total 115.39 ± 51.39 80.04 ± 34.65 2.81 .005

Time, s 136.45 ± 33.91 135.90 ± 41.05 0.06 .95

Dual task

Random Number Generation Index 0.41 ± 0.03 0.38 ± 0.02 1.20 .23

Null score 51.14 ± 8.18 49.90 ± 6.63 0.69 .49

Counting score

1 90.83 ± 58.92 78.96 ± 47.68 0.67 .50

2 33.04 ± 21.72 26.09 ± 14.15 1.46 .14

Total 123.87 ± 68.16 105.05 ± 52.03 1.87 .06

Time, s 188.66 ± 58.17 191.84 ± 84.39 1.12 .26

*Data are given as mean ± SD.

(REPRINTED) ARCH NEUROL / VOL 61, MAY 2004 WWW.ARCHNEUROL.COM 699

©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/neur/13744/ by a Universite de Geneve User on 04/27/2017

(4)

stimulation in our study. Digit span forward and re- verse assess working memory capacities too. These data showed no significant change with STN stimulation. The RNGT requires suppression of natural preferences for counting in series. This feature of the RNGT is a classic executive function that can be modified by STN stimu- lation. We confirm the findings of Jahanshahi et al,2who demonstrated decreased (improved) counting tenden- cies after switching on STN stimulators in a group of 7 PD patients. The counting scores are sensitive to the rate of production,9but in our study, the rate of production did not vary between the 2 stimulation conditions. Thus, the changes in the counting scores were not the result of an altered production rate.

With stimulation on, patients made significantly more errors in the interference condition of the Stroop color test, whereas the speed was not altered. This find- ing is in line with that of a previous study2but not with the findings of other studies.1,10

Our patients did not spontaneously complain of symptoms comparable with premature response in ac- tivities of everyday living. These results are in line with those of Saint-Cyr et al.13In their study, a “disinhibi- tion” 6 months after surgery was reported by caregivers, but not by patients, whereas 12 months after surgery, the caregivers noted no more disinhibition but an increase in apathy. This disinhibited behavior may only be a short- lasting effect, analogous to the dyskinesia induced after STN surgery, that also tends to disappear with time af- ter an STN lesion or while undergoing long-term STN stimulation.4This deficit can be demonstrated with sen- sitive instruments like the Stroop color test but gener- ally does not impair the patients in everyday life. There was no significant correlation between motor improve- ment and neuropsychological changes due to STN DBS.

Regarding this lack of correlation between neuropsycho- logical and motor data in our study, one major point that must be considered is the ongoing medication of our pa- tients. Of the tested patients, 82.6% (19/23) took ago- nists, mostly cabergoline, a long-acting agonist with a half- life of 60 hours. A medication withdrawal overnight would not exclude dopaminergic influence, and, therefore, we assessed the patients while they were taking their usual medication. The ongoing medication may mask the full effect of the STN DBS, which might alter the motor rat-

ing and further correlation analysis. Furthermore, the cor- relation analysis showed a significant correlation be- tween a worsening in Stroop color test performance (error rate in the interference condition) and an improvement in counting scores of the RNGT due to STN stimula- tion. These effects may be induced by stimulation of the associative territory of the STN.

In our study, STN DBS did not improve a dual cog- nitive task. Simultaneous cognitive task performance is a dopamine-related function.14Because our patients were undergoing dopaminergic treatment, our results are still preliminary. This issue should be investigated in fur- ther studies using motor and nonmotor tasks simulta- neously with more concurrent attention-demanding tasks.

Accepted for publication December 29, 2003.

Author contributions: Study concept and design(Drs Witt and Deuschl); acquisition of data (Drs Witt, Pulkowski, Herzog, Lorenz, Hamel, and Krack);analy- sis and interpretation of data(Drs Witt and Krack);draft- ing of the manuscript(Drs Witt, Herzog, Lorenz, and Krack);critical revision of the manuscript for important in- tellectual content(Drs Pulkowski, Hamel, and Deuschl);

statistical expertise(Drs Witt and Pulkowski);obtained funding(Drs Herzog, Lorenz, and Krack);administra- tive, technical, and material support(Drs Pulkowski, Her- zog, Lorenz, Hamel, and Krack);study supervision(Drs Deuschl and Krack).

Corresponding author: Gu¨nther Deuschl, MD, Depart- ment of Neurology, Christian-Albrechts-Universita¨t Kiel, Nie- mannsweg 147, D-24105 Kiel, Germany (e-mail:

g.deuschl@neurologie.uni-kiel.de).

REFERENCES

1. Pillon B, Ardouin C, Damier P, et al. Neuropsychological changes between “off” and

“on” STN or GPi stimulation in Parkinson’s disease.Neurology.2000;55:411-418.

2. Jahanshahi M, Ardouin CM, Brown RG, et al. The impact of deep brain stimula- tion on executive function in Parkinson’s disease.Brain.2000;123:1142-1154.

3. Mattis S.Dementia Rating Scale.Odessa, Fla: Psychological Assessment Re- sources Inc; 1988.

4. Wenzelburger R, Koper F, Zhang BR, et al. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation for Parkinson’s disease preferentially improves akinesia of proximal arm move- ments compared to finger movements.Mov Disord.2003;18:1162-1169.

5. Wechsler D.WAIS-R Manual.San Antonio, Tex: Psychological Corp; 1987.

6. Benton AL. Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease.Neuropsycho- logia.1968;6:53-60.

7. Perret E. The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behaviour.Neuropsychologia.1974;12:323-330.

8. Evans F. Monitoring attention deployment by random number generation: an in- dex to measure subjective randomness.Bull Psychonom Soc.1987;12:35-38.

9. Spatt J, Goldenberg G. Components of random generation by normal subjects and patients with dysexecutive syndrome.Brain Cogn.1993;23:231-242.

10. Ardouin C, Pillon B, Peiffer E, et al. Bilateral subthalamic or pallidal stimulation for Parkinson’s disease affects neither memory nor executive functions: a con- secutive series of 62 patients.Ann Neurol.1999;46:217-223.

11. Brown RG, Soliveri P, Jahanshahi M. Executive processes in Parkinson’s dis- ease: random number generation and response suppression.Neuropsycholo- gia.1998;36:1355-1362.

12. Daniels C, Witt K, Wolff S, Jansen O, Deuschl G. Rate dependency of the human cortical network subserving executive functions during generation of random num- ber series: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.Neurosci Lett.2003;

345:25-28.

13. Saint-Cyr JA, Trepanier LL, Kumar R, Lozano AM, Lang AE. Neuropsychological consequences of chronic bilateral stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in Par- kinson’s disease.Brain.2000;123:2091-2108.

14. Malapani C, Pillon B, Dubois B, Agid Y. Impaired simultaneous cognitive task performance in Parkinson’s disease: a dopamine-related dysfunction.Neurol- ogy.1994;44:319-326.

Table 2. Neuropsychological Evaluation Results Before and 12 Months After STN DBS

Mattis Dementia Rating Scale Component

Before STN DBS*

After STN DBS*†

2-Tailed PValue

Total score 136.2 ± 2.9 134.7 ± 5.5 .25

Attention 35.7 ± 0.9 35.0 ± 2.1 .13

Initiation 34.7 ± 2.4 34.0 ± 2.9 .35

Construction 6.0 ± 0.0 5.9 ± 0.2 .32

Conceptualization 36.9 ± 1.6 37.1 ± 1.3 .55

Memory 22.9 ± 1.2 22.7 ± 2.5 .82

Abbreviations: DBS, deep brain stimulation; STN, subthalamic nucleus.

*Data are given as mean ± SD.

†Stimulation on.

(REPRINTED) ARCH NEUROL / VOL 61, MAY 2004 WWW.ARCHNEUROL.COM 700

©2004 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archneur.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/neur/13744/ by a Universite de Geneve User on 04/27/2017

Références

Documents relatifs

STN stimulation can be considered as an alternative to Vim or pallidal surgery, even in patients severely disabled by parkinsonian rest and postural tremor, since

The improvement in the three subtypes of levodopa-induced dyskinesias was significant from the third postoperative month and was mainly due to the decrease in the daily dose of

Three months after the procedures were performed, double-blind, cross- over evaluations demonstrated that stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus was associated with a

Effect of subthalamic nucleus stimulation on levodopa-induced dyskinesia in Parkinson's disease. Effect of subthalamic nucleus stimulation on levodopa-induced dyskinesia in

In the second series of analyses, we investigated whether HC, PD- MO and PD-DBS di ff ered in their experience of the musical GEMS emotions (Tension, Wonder, Joyful

The secondary outcome measures were the subscores on part III (limb tremor, limb rigidity, limb akinesia, speech, postural stability, and gait) and part IV (the dyskinesia items) of

Objective: This study evaluated the cognitive, mood, and behavioural effects of bilateral subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation (STN DBS) in patients with Parkinson’s disease

Using intracranial local field potential (LFP) recordings in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) under- going deep brain stimulation (DBS), we explored the