• Aucun résultat trouvé

Co-Editors / Corédacteurs

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Co-Editors / Corédacteurs"

Copied!
1
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

3

Canadian Historical Association

Co-Editors Corédacteurs

In its most recent online survey, Historica Canada, the organi-zation behind the country’s Heritage Minutes, tested over one thousand respondents on their factual knowledge of Canadian history using true or false questions. The results were not great. According to Historica, two-thirds of those who completed the survey got a failing grade. That is worse than last year when six-ty-two percent of participants failed a similar survey.

But does it really matter that only nineteen percent of this year’s respondents knew that the first recorded instance of dressing in costume on Halloween in North America was documented in Vancouver in 1898? In the age

of Google and Wikipedia, when facts can be checked so easily, does one really need to know when the Indian Act was signed or the name of the first woman to be elected to the Canadian House of Com-mons? (These were not questions, by the way, that were included on Historica’s online test). Isn’t it more important to think well? There will be those who will argue that it is much more import-ant to be able to think creatively than to know a bunch of facts. Albert Einstein perhaps captured

this sentiment best when he stated that: “Imagination is more important than knowledge.” But according to Daniel Willing-ham, a Harvard-educated cognitive scientist who writes books and articles about how to improve one’s learning and teaching abilities, Einstein had it wrong because a solid factual knowl-edge is a prerequisite for thinking outside of the box. “[T]he cognitive processes that are most esteemed—logical thinking, problem solving, and the like—are intertwined with knowl-edge,” Willingham writes. “It is certainly true that facts without skills to use them are of little value. It is equally true that one cannot deploy thinking skills effectively without factual knowl-edge.” Thus, according to Willingham, factual knowledge matters because it determines how fast one can acquire new knowledge on a specific topic. In short, the more one knows, the faster one learns.

This is supported by various studies in experimental psychology. In one such study, which was undertaken by J.P. Van Over-schelde and A.F. Healy, the participants were divided into two

Does it really matter that Canadians lack

a factual knowledge of their collective past?

groups. The first group was given the opportunity to learn more facts on the topic. Then both groups were tested on how fast they learned new facts, that neither group knew, on the same topic. The study demonstrated that the first group absorbed the new facts faster than the second group. Other studies have showed that a prior factual knowledge on any given subject allows one to retain more new information about what has just been read or heard on the subject.

Don’t get me wrong. There is much more to the study of his-tory than knowing the facts. Also, there is much more to being

a good citizen than knowing if the Jolly Jumper was a Cana-dian invention (on the Historica quiz) or when the North-West Rebellion took place (not on the quiz). In fact, in my opinion, the how and the why are more important than the where and the when. That is to say, a factual knowledge might be a prereq-uisite to innovative thinking in many other disciplines, but what is most important in our field is an interest and understanding in the various interpretations of the past. Canadian history is not a collection of “official” facts. Rather it is a collection of competing “truths” and narratives. The special thing about Canada is that – unlike in many nations around the globe where there is a mistaken belief that the past can be synthesized into a single, standardized chronicle – these competing “truths” are allowed to stand side by side.

I understand what Historica is trying to do. Among other things, it is attempting to show that our past is not as dull as dishwater. As Historica’s Chief Executive Officer, Anthony Wilson-Smith, recently stated: “It drives me crazy, this idea that Canadian his-tory is boring. No, it’s not. People just haven’t bothered to look closely enough at the real human stories and quirky things that have happened here.” But rather than test us on our knowledge of quirky things, Canadians might be better served descending into the bog of conflict, and learning the many “histories” that compose our collective experience.

Matt Bellamy,

English Language Secretary

“Imagination is

more important

than knowledge.”

Albert Einstein

Références

Documents relatifs

Visualization of the Terminologies. The representation of terminologies as tree views enables accessibility, comparability, and association between hierarchical struc-

One class of control laws which can handle such constraints explicitly and efficiently and still generate stable motions is Model Predictive Control (MPC) [10], [11], which maintains

Certificat de Santé Communautaire 1983-84. Document de travail destiné aux étudiants. Paris: Faculté de Médecine Xavier-Bichat: 6. La santé communautaire dans le système

Dans les pays anglo-saxons, on trouve le personnage du lapin de Pâques (Easter Bunny) qui cache les œufs en chocolat ou les œufs décorés pour les enfants?. En Grande Bretagne,

In all specimens, the central stitch in the row just medial to the rotator cable provided highest suture retention properties, compared to all other stitches (p \ 0.01)..

For example, whereas for a = 2,25 and the exponential distribution as the distribution of claim amounts the largest claim constitutes only 4,3% of the total loss, in the case of

Infection with the protozoan Toxoplasma gondii is one of the most frequent parasitic infections worldwide. This obligate intracellular parasite was first described

We used the unrelated 1000 Genomes CEU samples as a modern reference population sample and em- ployed two complementary methods: (1) a Bayesian approach (ApproxWF) [29] under