• Aucun résultat trouvé

Semantic and phonological similarities on repeated and non-repeated person naming confusions Manuel Dupont*

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2021

Partager "Semantic and phonological similarities on repeated and non-repeated person naming confusions Manuel Dupont*"

Copied!
1
0
0

Texte intégral

(1)

The effect of semantic and phonological similarities on repeated

and non-repeated person naming confusions

Manuel Dupont*

Personal names are amongst the most difficult words to retrieve in memory even though they are very important in everyday life. Among these retrieval difficulties, we focused on the personal name confusions phenomenon (i.e. Calling someone familiar with the name of another one). The main question here is : which variables influence these naming confusions?

Two variables which are known by previous studies to influence naming confusion were studied here (see Brédart and Dardenne, 20161; Fraas and

al., 20022; Griffin and Wangerman, 20133) i.e. the semantic similarity between the bearers of the names which have been confused, and the

phonological similarity of the confused names.

To evaluate the role of these two variables we have used a face+name association memory task. The second goal of this study was to compare the repeated confusions (i.e. confound the same names more than once) with the non-repeated ones.

BACKGROUND

The phonological similarity between the confused names and the semantic similarity between the bearers of the names impacted the probability of making confusions. When similarities were present, the probability of making a confusion was higher than if there was no similarities between the names.

The studied factors impacted repeated and non-repeated confusions in the same way.

Further research is needed to investigate whether other similarities could also influence the naming confusion phenomenon.

CONCLUSIONS

REFERENCES

(1) Brédart, S., & Dardenne, B. (2016). Similarities between the target and the intruder in naturally occurring repeated person naming errors. Frontiers in psychology, 6.

(2) Fraas, M., Lockwood, J., Neils-Strunjas, J., Shidler, M., Krikorian, R., & Weiler, E. (2002). ‘What's his name?’A comparison of elderly participants' and undergraduate students' misnamings. Archives of gerontology and geriatrics,

34(2), 155-165.

(3) Griffin, Z. M., & Wangerman, T. (2013). Parents accidentally substitute similar sounding sibling names more often than dissimilar names. PloS one, 8(12), e84444.

PARTICIPANTS:

Middle age participant (N=64) Age between 44 and 60 (M= 51.3 years; SD = 4.0, 36♀)

MATERIAL AND PROCEDURE:

Face + name association memory task. Phase 1: Memory phase

Part one : The participants had to memorize the name of 16 faces. These faces were displayed one by one on a computer screen (the 16 face-name couples were presented twice in a different order). Part two : the 16 face were presented without the name, participant had to give the name of each faces. A feedback was given by the experimenter. Participants had to reach the acquisition criterion to finish this part  to give the right first name for each of the 16 faces in a row

Phase 2: Test phase

The 16 faces (without the names) were displayed in the cases of a double entry table (composed of 40 cases) linked with black lines (see figure one). Participants had to give the name of the face and his coordinates in the table (like in battleship game) and pass to the next face following the black line. Five different tables were presented.

Figure 1 : Exemple of a test table.

METHOD

Variables and analysis of the data

* University of Liège, Liège, Belgium

Psychology and Neuroscience of Cognition Research Unit (PsyNCog)

Contact: m.dupont@ulg.ac.be

Repeated Anova with three within-subjects factors (semantic similarity, phonological similarity, type of confusions)

- Main effect of the phonological similarity:

F(1, 63) = 30.25, p < 0.001, ƞp2= 0.32

 more confusions for phonologically related names than for phonologically unrelated

- Main effect of the semantic similarity:

F(1, 63) = 81.46, p <0.001, ƞp2= 0.56

 more confusions for semantically related names than for semantically unrelated

- Interaction between these two factors:

F(1, 63) = 7.55, p = 0.007, ƞp2= 0.11

- Main effect of the type of confusions:

F(1, 63) = 25.59, p<0.001, ƞp2= 0.29

 more non-repeated confusions than non-repeated ones.

Results

Analysis of the confusions

Four types of confusions: 1. “SP” confusions : semantically and phonologically similar confusions 2. “P” confusions : phonologically similar confusions 3. “S” confusions: semantically similar 4. “NonSp” confusions: no similarity in the confusions The dependent measure : The Probability of making confusions for each category

 Number of possible confusions

is different for each category

Semantic factor : the age of the faces (2 modalities)

- Eight young faces - Eight middle age faces

Phonological factor (2 modalities)

- Eight phonologically similar names (manɔ̃, maʁɡo, maʁjɑ̃n, maʁin, maʁsɛl, maʁtɛ̃, matjø, maksɑ̃s)

- Eight phonologically dissimilar names (ɑ̃ʒelik, bʁiʒit, sindi, nataʃa, didje, flɔʁɑ̃, ʒozɛf, ivɔ̃)

Figure

Figure 1 : Exemple of a test table.

Références

Documents relatifs

Proposed solutions either use information retrieval to calculate similarities between models [7]; XML diff algorithms for difference detection within versions of a model [9]; focus

We have selected, for their diversity of experimental conditions, nine studies of limonene oxi- dation at room temperature over long reaction times to be compared to the present

The 31 studies selected for systematic mapping were classi- fied based on the six different topics (i.e., Business Ecosystem, Collaboration Systems, Digital Business Ecosystem,

In sum, the rhyme judgements task from written words and auditory item – picture match- ing task were designed to assess the phonological link between output and input codes.. The

Our results in the ASSIN2 challenge, see Table 3, first place in the Similarity task and second place in Entailment task, show that fine-tuning BERT is at the moment one of the

Then, the empirical results show the following findings: (1) The distribution of similarity is nearly identical regardless of the project; (2) Programmers should avoid giving

3.1 Effect of query profile size on similarity decay First, we wanted to determine how the pattern of similarity decline varies between different query profile sizes.. We

We compared query profiles having variable proportions of matching phenotypes to subject database profiles using both pairwise and groupwise Jaccard (edge-based) and Resnik